1
|
Šlosar L, Pus K, Marusic U. Developmental trajectories of motor imagery in relation to physical fitness in children aged 7 to 14 years: A 1-year follow-up study. J Exp Child Psychol 2025; 249:106115. [PMID: 39461324 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2024] [Revised: 08/05/2024] [Accepted: 09/30/2024] [Indexed: 10/29/2024]
Abstract
Motor imagery (MI) is fundamentally linked to the motor system. It improves motor learning and optimizes motor actions without physical execution, highlighting its unique role in rehabilitation programs and motor performance. Understanding the developmental trajectories of MI and the factors influencing its variability across ages could enable more effective, age-specific strategies for pediatric rehabilitation. This study assessed 65 children aged 7 to 14 years at two time points 1 year apart. MI ability was assessed using the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children, and physical fitness was evaluated using the SLOfit testing battery. Among the three perspectives assessed; internal visual imagery (IVI), external visual imagery (EVI), and kinesthetic imagery (KI), KI was unique in not correlating with age at both time points. The development of MI perspectives varied between athletes and non-athletes, with non-athletes showing a decline in IVI compared with athletes. This differential was further evidenced by significant differences in KI between the groups at the second assessment, with a similar trend observed at the first assessment. Of the physical fitness tests, only the 600-m run correlated consistently with KI at both assessments. Our findings suggest that regular participation in sports significantly affects KI performance, highlighting the importance of sports participation for the development of MI abilities in children. Future research should examine additional assessment points in different age groups and sport experience to better understand the development of MI and its potential implications for pediatric rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luka Šlosar
- Science and Research Centre Koper, Institute for Kinesiology Research, 6000 Koper, Slovenia; Department of Health Sciences, Alma Mater Europaea University, 2000 Maribor, Slovenia
| | - Katarina Pus
- Science and Research Centre Koper, Institute for Kinesiology Research, 6000 Koper, Slovenia; Department of Health Sciences, Alma Mater Europaea University, 2000 Maribor, Slovenia; Faculty of Sport, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Uros Marusic
- Science and Research Centre Koper, Institute for Kinesiology Research, 6000 Koper, Slovenia; Department of Health Sciences, Alma Mater Europaea University, 2000 Maribor, Slovenia.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Šlosar L, Puš K, Marušič U. Validation of the Slovenian Version of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children (MIQ-C): A Measurement Tool to Assess the Imagery Ability of Motor Tasks in Children. Zdr Varst 2023; 62:113-120. [PMID: 37327132 PMCID: PMC10263371 DOI: 10.2478/sjph-2023-0016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2022] [Accepted: 03/17/2023] [Indexed: 06/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Purpose The ability to perform motor imagery has been shown to influence individual athletic performance and rehabilitation. Recent evidence supports its potential as a training tool to improve motor skills in children. Although there is a standardized assessment of the imagery abilities in Slovenian-speaking adults, there is currently no validated instrument for use with Slovenian children. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to conduct a linguistic validation study of the movement imagery questionnaire for children (MIQ-C). Methods A total of 100 healthy children (mean age 10.3±1.3 years; 50 female) were assessed with a Slovenian version of the MIQ-C at Day 1 and Day 8. Inter-day agreement was examined using intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC). Construct validity and internal consistency were assessed using a Cronbach's alpha coefficient and exploratory - confirmatory factor analysis, respectively. Results The test-retest ICC were very high for all three scales examined (ICCKI=0.90; ICCIVI=0.92; ICCEVI=0.90). Excellent internal consistency (up to 0.90) was found for kinaesthetic and both visual imageries. Confirmatory analysis confirmed a three-factorial structure of the MIQ-C. Conclusions The Slovenian version of the MIQ-C proved to be highly reliable and valid in assessing children's motor imagery abilities, and as such for use with Slovene-speaking children. Moreover, this standardized instrument can be a helpful tool in training and rehabilitation practice with children aged 7-12 years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luka Šlosar
- Science and Research Centre Koper, Institute for Kinesiology Research, Kozlovičeva 23, 6000Koper, Slovenia
- Alma Mater Europaea – ECM, Department of Health Sciences, Slovenska 17, 2000Maribor, Slovenia
| | - Katarina Puš
- Science and Research Centre Koper, Institute for Kinesiology Research, Kozlovičeva 23, 6000Koper, Slovenia
- Alma Mater Europaea – ECM, Department of Health Sciences, Slovenska 17, 2000Maribor, Slovenia
- Faculty of Sport, University of Ljubljana, Gortanova 22, 1000Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Uroš Marušič
- Science and Research Centre Koper, Institute for Kinesiology Research, Kozlovičeva 23, 6000Koper, Slovenia
- Alma Mater Europaea – ECM, Department of Health Sciences, Slovenska 17, 2000Maribor, Slovenia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Saleem GT. Defining and measuring motor imagery in children: mini review. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1227215. [PMID: 37655192 PMCID: PMC10466893 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1227215] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2023] [Accepted: 08/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Motor imagery (MI) is the ability to engage in the mental representation of a task consciously or automatically without generating a voluntary movement. While the construct of MI and its various dimensions have been comprehensively studied in adults, research remains limited in children. Children as young as 5 years old can engage in MI, and this engagement is crucial to their motor development and skill acquisition. Further, the degree of skill achievement is directly linked to MI responsiveness. Clinicians and researchers often measure MI responsiveness in children to facilitate skill development and retention. However, few measures exist that can appropriately assess MI responsiveness in children. To date, a focused review examining the MI dimensions in children as well as comparing the characteristics of MI measures in children is lacking, and thus a research gap exists. This paper examines past and current research describing MI ability in children from the theoretical, developmental, and neurological lens and systematically analyzes the properties of three widely used operations - the movement imagery questionnaire in children (MIQ-C), the Florida praxis imaginary questionnaire (FPIQ-C), and the mental chronometry paradigm (MCP) - to measure MI and its dimensions in children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ghazala T. Saleem
- Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, School of Public Health and Health Professions, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
EbrahimiSani S, Sohrabi M, Taheri H, Agdasi MT, Amiri S. Effects of virtual reality training intervention on predictive motor control of children with DCD - A randomized controlled trial. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2020; 107:103768. [PMID: 33161293 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2020] [Revised: 08/20/2020] [Accepted: 08/20/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
It has been hypothesised that deficits in the functions of predictive motor control and internal modeling may contribute to motor control issues of children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). Virtual reality (VR) technologies have great potential to provide opportunity for Motor observation and motor imagery (MI) which could enhance learning and development of motor skills in children with DCD. Thus, the present study aimed to investigate the benefits of a VR training intervention to improve predictive motor control functions of children with DCD. Forty female children with DCD (aged 7-10) were randomly assigned to VR and control groups. In this study, an experimental pre-post and follow-up design was used, and Predictive motor control functions were measured before and after the VR intervention and two-months later. Predictive motor control was evaluated using MI (by hand rotation task), action planning (by sword placement task), and rapid and online control (by rotational tracking task) tests. VR intervention consisted of a selection of Xbox 360 Kinect games that were performed for sixteen 30-min sessions over 8 weeks. Compared to the control group, the VR group improved significantly on measures of MI, motor planning, and rapid and online control scores from pre- to post-test and retained their performance to follow-up. Overall, it seems that virtual reality training program may be used as an appropriate intervention approach for developing the ability of MI and predictive motor control functions in DCD children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Soghra EbrahimiSani
- Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran.
| | - Mehdi Sohrabi
- Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran.
| | - Hamidreza Taheri
- Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran.
| | - Mohammad Tagi Agdasi
- Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Sport Sciences, University of Tabriz, Tabriz, Iran.
| | - Shahrokh Amiri
- Research Center of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Krajenbrink H, Lust J, Wilson P, Steenbergen B. Development of motor planning in children: Disentangling elements of the planning process. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 199:104945. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104945] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2020] [Revised: 07/03/2020] [Accepted: 07/03/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
6
|
van de Walle de Ghelcke A, Skoura X, Edwards MG, Quercia P, Papaxanthis C. Action representation deficits in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. J Neuropsychol 2020; 15:215-234. [PMID: 32816402 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2019] [Revised: 07/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Developmental dyslexia (DD), a severe and frequent disorder of reading acquisition, is characterized by a diversity of cognitive and motor deficits whose interactions still remain under debate. Although deficits in the automatization of sensorimotor control have been highlighted, internal action representation allowing prediction has never before been investigated. In this study, we considered action representation of 18 adolescents with pure DD and 18 age-matched typical readers. Participants actually and mentally performed a visually guided pointing task involving strong spatiotemporal constraints (speed/accuracy trade-off paradigm). While actual and mental movement times of typical readers were isochronous and both conformed to Fitts' law, the movement times of dyslexics differed between conditions, and only the actual movement times conformed to Fitts' law. Furthermore, the quality of motor imagery correlated with word reading abilities. This suggests that the process of action representation is impaired in pure DD and supports the sensorimotor perspective of DD. Theoretical implications are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice van de Walle de Ghelcke
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute and Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Xanthi Skoura
- INSERM UMR1093-CAPS, UFR des Sciences du Sport, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
| | - Martin Gareth Edwards
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute and Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Patrick Quercia
- INSERM UMR1093-CAPS, UFR des Sciences du Sport, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
| | - Charalambos Papaxanthis
- INSERM UMR1093-CAPS, UFR des Sciences du Sport, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Ianì F, Limata T, Bucciarelli M, Mazzoni G. Children's kinematic false memories. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2020.1796686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Ianì
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Torino, Turin, Italy
| | - Teresa Limata
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Torino, Turin, Italy
| | - Monica Bucciarelli
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Torino, Turin, Italy
- Centro di Logica, Linguaggio, e Cognizione, Università di Torino, Turin, Italy
| | - Giuliana Mazzoni
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Sapienza Università di Roma, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Deconinck FJ, D’Hondt E, Caeyenberghs K, Lenoir M, Augustijn MJ. The association between mental rotation capacity and motor impairment in children with obesity-an exploratory study. PeerJ 2019; 7:e8150. [PMID: 31844572 PMCID: PMC6911341 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.8150] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2019] [Accepted: 11/04/2019] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Motor impairments are relatively common in children with obesity and evidence suggests that these difficulties go beyond those expected based on the extra weight. This study aimed to investigate the mental rotation capacity in children with obesity, i.e., the ability to mentally view and rotate spatial information, which is a function of both visual-spatial and action representation processes. In particular, we examined whether children with obesity solved mental hand rotation tasks using an egocentric perspective (i.e., motor imagery) and if performance was related to their motor competence. METHODS Fifty children (age range: 7-11 y) of which 19 with obesity and motor impairments (OB-) as assessed by the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (2nd version), 13 with obesity without motor impairments (OB+) and 18 control children with a healthy weight (HW) and normal motor competence, were submitted to a classic hand rotation task. Sitting at a desk the children were instructed to indicate the laterality of a picture of a hand displayed on a monitor as quickly and as accurately as possible. RESULTS The results indicate no differences in response time between groups (2,648 ms, 2,558 ms, 2,664 ms for OB-, OB+ and HW respectively). The OB- group, however, had significantly lower accuracy rates and inverse efficiency scores than the HW group (Accuracy: 72% vs. 89%; Inverse efficiency: 4,428 vs. 3,238). No difference was observed in accuracy and inverse efficiency between the OB+ and HW group (Accuracy: 86%; Inverse efficiency: 3,432). In all groups, slower and more error-prone responses were observed when the angle of rotation was larger and when the hand on display was incongruent with the posture of the participants, which indicates that judgments were made from an egocentric perspective and involved motor imagery. CONCLUSION All children, including those with obesity, appear to engage in motor imagery. This notion needs to be investigated further in children with obesity and motor impairments, given their generally lower accuracy and decreased efficiency, which may indicate a reduced mental rotation capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Eva D’Hondt
- Faculty of Physical Education and Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Karen Caeyenberghs
- Cognitive Neurosciences Unit, Deakin University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Matthieu Lenoir
- Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
England A, Brusseau T, Burns R, Koester D, Newton M, Thiese M, Chase B. The Cognitive Structure of the Basketball Free Throw in Adolescent Physical Education Students. Motor Control 2019; 23:472-484. [PMID: 30971162 DOI: 10.1123/mc.2018-0035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2018] [Revised: 11/16/2018] [Accepted: 11/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
In adult performers, research suggests that mental representations (MRs) mediate performance of skilled movement. During adolescence, cortical brain areas responsible for generating MRs develop rapidly along with limb size, which, together, may affect movement and movement representations. The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between adolescent MRs and free-throw shooting expertise. Using structural dimensional analysis of MRs, skilled (n = 11) and less skilled (n = 11) participants sorted free-throw submovements according to their relatedness in movement execution. Data were analyzed using a hierarchical cluster analysis, factor analysis, and invariance test to examine between-group cluster comparisons. Cluster solutions for the skilled and less skilled participants were significantly variant (λ = 0.56). This method of measuring MRs distinguished expertise-related differences in MRs in an adolescent population. Findings may influence methods in which practitioners detect motor-planning faults, track development, and provide feedback to trainees.
Collapse
|
10
|
Errante A, Bozzetti F, Sghedoni S, Bressi B, Costi S, Crisi G, Ferrari A, Fogassi L. Explicit Motor Imagery for Grasping Actions in Children With Spastic Unilateral Cerebral Palsy. Front Neurol 2019; 10:837. [PMID: 31447762 PMCID: PMC6692593 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00837] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2019] [Accepted: 07/19/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Motor Imagery (MI) refers to mental simulation of a motor action without producing any overt movement. Previous studies showed that children with Unilateral Cerebral Palsy (UCP) are impaired in implicit MI, as demonstrated by the performance of Hand Laterality Judgment tasks. The aim of this study was to examine the specificity of explicit MI deficits in UCP children. Methods: A group of UCP children (n = 10; aged 9-14) performed a mental chronometry task consisting in grasping an object and placing it into a container, or in imagining to perform the same action. As control, a group of typically developing (TD) children, matched by age, performed the same task. Movement durations for executed and imagined trials were recorded. A subgroup of 7 UCP children and 10 TD children also underwent a session of functional MRI to examine the activation of parieto-frontal areas typically associated to MI processes, during the imagination of reaching-grasping actions performed with the paretic hand. Results: Behavioral results revealed the existence of a correlation between executed and imagined movement durations both in TD and UCP groups. Moreover, the regression analysis in TD children showed that higher scores in mental chronometry tasks were positively correlated to increased bilateral activation of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), superior parietal lobule (SPL), and dorsal premotor (PMd) cortex. A similar analysis revealed in the UCP group a positive correlation between a higher score in the mental chronometry task and bilateral activations of IPS, and to activation of contralesional, right PMd, and putamen during imagination of grasping movements. Conclusions: These results provide new insights on the relationship between MI capacity and motor deficits in UCP children, suggesting the possibility of the use of explicit MI training to improve patient's upper limb motor functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antonino Errante
- Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Francesca Bozzetti
- Neuroradiology Unit, Department of Diagnostic, University Hospital of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Silvia Sghedoni
- Azienda Unità Sanitaria Locale - IRCCS of Reggio Emilia, Reggio Emilia, Italy
| | - Barbara Bressi
- Azienda Unità Sanitaria Locale - IRCCS of Reggio Emilia, Reggio Emilia, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
| | - Stefania Costi
- Azienda Unità Sanitaria Locale - IRCCS of Reggio Emilia, Reggio Emilia, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
| | - Girolamo Crisi
- Neuroradiology Unit, Department of Diagnostic, University Hospital of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Adriano Ferrari
- Azienda Unità Sanitaria Locale - IRCCS of Reggio Emilia, Reggio Emilia, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
| | - Leonardo Fogassi
- Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Fusco A, Iasevoli L, Iosa M, Gallotta MC, Padua L, Tucci L, Antonucci G, Baldari C, Guidetti L. Dynamic motor imagery mentally simulates uncommon real locomotion better than static motor imagery both in young adults and elderly. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0218378. [PMID: 31242209 PMCID: PMC6594612 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218378] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2018] [Accepted: 05/31/2019] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
A new form of Motor Imagery (MI), called dynamic Motor Imagery (dMI) has recently been proposed. The dMI adds to conventional static Motor Imagery (sMI) the presence of simultaneous actual movements partially replicating those mentally represented. In a previous research conducted on young participants, dMI showed to be temporally closer than sMI in replicating the real performance for some specific locomotor conditions. In this study, we evaluated if there is any influence of the ageing on dMI. Thirty healthy participants were enrolled: 15 young adults (27.1±3.8 y.o.) and 15 older adults (65.9±9.6y.o.). The performance time and the number of steps needed to either walk to a target (placed at 10m from participants) or to imagine walking to it, were assessed. Parameters were measured for sMI, dMI and real locomotion (RL) in three different locomotor conditions: forward walking (FW), backward walking (BW), and lateral walking (LW). Temporal performances of sMI and dMI did not differ between RL in the FW, even if significantly different to each other (p = 0.0002). No significant differences were found for dMI with respect to RL for LW (p = 0.140) and BW (p = 0.438), while sMI was significantly lower than RL in LW (p<0.001). The p-value of main effect of age on participants’ temporal performances was p = 0.055. The interaction between age and other factors such as the type of locomotion (p = 0.358) or the motor condition (p = 0.614) or third level interaction (p = 0.349) were not statistically significant. Despite a slight slowdown in the performance of elderly compared to young participants, the temporal and spatial accuracy was better in dMI than sMI in both groups. Motor imagery processes may be strengthened by the feedback generated through dMI, and this effect appears to be unaffected by age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Augusto Fusco
- IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Milan, Italy
- * E-mail:
| | - Luigi Iasevoli
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Section of Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico", Rome, Italy
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
| | - Marco Iosa
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
| | - Maria Chiara Gallotta
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Section of Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico", Rome, Italy
| | - Luca Padua
- Department of Geriatrics, Neurosciences and Orthopaedics, Sacred Heart Catholic University, Rome, Italy
- Fondazione Policlinico Universitario A. Gemelli IRCCS, Rome, Italy
| | - Livia Tucci
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Section of Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico", Rome, Italy
| | - Gabriella Antonucci
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
- Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
| | | | - Laura Guidetti
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, Section of Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico", Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
When do children acquire the ability to understand recursion-that is, repeated loops of actions, as in cookery recipes or computer programs? Hitherto, studies have focused either on unconscious recursions in language and vision or on the difficulty of conscious recursions-even for adults-when learning to program. In contrast, we examined 10- to 11-year-old fifth-graders' ability to deduce the consequences of loops of actions in informal algorithms and to create such algorithms for themselves. In our experiments, the children tackled problems requiring the rearrangement of cars on a toy railway with a single track and a siding-an environment that in principle allows for the execution of any algorithm-that is, it has the power of a universal Turing machine. The children were not allowed to move the cars, so each problem's solution called for them to envision the movements of cars on the track. We describe a theory of recursive thinking, which is based on kinematic simulations and which we have implemented in a computer program embodying mental models of the cars and track. Experiment 1 tested children's ability to deduce rearrangements of the cars in a train from descriptions of algorithms containing a single loop of actions. Experiment 2 assessed children's spontaneous creation of similar sorts of algorithms. The results showed that fifth-grade children with no training in computer programming have systematic abilities to deduce from and to create informal recursive algorithms.
Collapse
|
13
|
Sooley M, Cressman EK, Martini R. Movement imagery as a predictor of online control in typically developing children. Dev Neuropsychol 2018; 43:508-523. [PMID: 29847159 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2018.1479754] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
The ability to mentally represent actions is suggested to play a role in the online control of movement in healthy adults. Children's movement imagery ability and online control have been shown to develop at similar nonlinear rates. The current study investigated the relationship between movement imagery and online control in children by comparing implicit and explicit movement imagery measures with the ability to make online trajectory corrections. Imagery ability was a significant predictor of children's online control of movement once general reaching efficiency was controlled for. These findings extend the proposed relationship between movement imagery and online control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcus Sooley
- a School of Human Kinetics, Faculty of Health Sciences , University of Ottawa (MSc student) , CBI Health Centre , Ottawa , Ontario, Canada
| | - Erin K Cressman
- b School of Human Kinetics, Faculty of Health Sciences , University of Ottawa , Ottawa , ON
| | - Rose Martini
- c Occupational Therapy Program, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences , University of Ottawa , Ottawa , Ontario , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Guilbert J, Jouen F, Molina M. Motor imagery development and proprioceptive integration: Which sensory reweighting during childhood? J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 166:621-634. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2017] [Revised: 09/27/2017] [Accepted: 09/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
15
|
Krüger M, Ebersbach M. Mental rotation and the human body: Children's inflexible use of embodiment mirrors that of adults. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017; 36:418-437. [PMID: 29277908 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12228] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2017] [Revised: 11/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Adults' mental rotation performance with body-like stimuli is enhanced if these stimuli are anatomically compatible with a human body, but decreased by anatomically incompatible stimuli. In this study, we investigated these effects for kindergartners and first-graders: When asked to mentally rotate cube configurations attached with human body parts in an anatomically compatible way, allowing for the projection of a human body, children performed better than with pure cube combinations. By contrast, when body parts were attached in an anatomically incompatible way, disallowing the projection of a human body, children performed worse than with pure combinations. This experiment is of specific interest against the background of two different theoretical approaches concerning imagery and the motor system in development: One approach assumes an increasing integration of motor processes and imagery over time that enables older children and adults to requisition motor resources for imagery processes, while the other postulates that imagery stems from early sensorimotor processes in the first place, and is disentangled from it over time. The finding that children of the two age groups tested show exactly the same effects as adults when mentally rotating anatomically compatible and incompatible stimuli is interpreted in favour of the latter approach. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? In mental rotation, adults perform better when rotating anatomically possible stimuli as compared to rotating standard cube combinations. Performance is worse when rotating anatomically impossible stimuli. What does this study add? The present study shows that children's mental transformations mirror those of adults in these respects. In case of the anatomically impossible stimuli, this highlights an inflexible use of embodiment in both age groups. This is in line with the Piagetian assumption of imagery being based on sensorimotor processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Markus Krüger
- Institute of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Adams ILJ, Lust JM, Steenbergen B. Development of motor imagery ability in children with developmental coordination disorder - A goal-directed pointing task. Br J Psychol 2017; 109:187-203. [DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2017] [Revised: 09/19/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Imke L. J. Adams
- Behavioural Science Institute; Radboud University; Nijmegen The Netherlands
| | - Jessica M. Lust
- Behavioural Science Institute; Radboud University; Nijmegen The Netherlands
| | - Bert Steenbergen
- Behavioural Science Institute; Radboud University; Nijmegen The Netherlands
- School of Psychology; Australian Catholic University; Sydney New South Wales Australia
- Centre for Disability and Development Research; Australian Catholic University; Sydney New South Wales Australia
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Spruijt S, Jongsma MLA, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B. Examining Developmental Changes in Children's Motor Imagery: A Longitudinal Study. Adv Cogn Psychol 2017; 13:257-266. [PMID: 29062438 PMCID: PMC5645803 DOI: 10.5709/acp-0226-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2016] [Accepted: 08/22/2017] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Using a longitudinal design, the present study examined developmental changes in
the employment of (motor) imagery strategies on the hand laterality judgment
(HLJ) task in children. All children (N = 23) participated
three times, at ages of 5, 6, and 7 years. Error percentages and response
durations were compared to a priori defined sinusoid models, representing
different strategies to judge hand laterality. Response durations of correct and
incorrect trials were included. Observed data showed that task performance was
affected by motor constraints, both in children who performed accurately at 5
years of age and in the children who did not. This is the first study to show
that 5-year-olds—even when not successful at the task—employ motor imagery when
engaged in this task. Importantly, although the children became faster and more
accurate with age, no developmental changes in the employed motor imagery
strategy were observed at ages of 5, 6, and 7 years. We found that 5-year-old
children are able to use a motor imagery strategy to perform the HL J task.
Although performance on this task improved with age, our analyses showed that
the employed strategy to solve this task remained invariant across age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steffie Spruijt
- Radboud University, Behavioural Science Institute, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | | | - John van der Kamp
- University of Hong Kong, Institute of Human Performance, Hong Kong, China
| | - Bert Steenbergen
- Australian Catholic University, School of Psychology, Melbourne, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Yoxon E, Welsh TN. Independent Development of Imagination and Perception of Fitts' Law in Late Childhood and Adolescence. J Mot Behav 2017. [PMID: 28644766 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2017.1327408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Recent neurophysiological and behavioral research suggests perception-action systems are tightly coupled. Accordingly, Fitts' law has been observed when individuals execute, perceive, and imagine actions. Developmental research has found that (a) children demonstrate Fitts' law in imagined actions and (b) imagined movement time (MT) becomes closer to actual MT as age increases. However, action execution, imagination, and perception have yet to be assessed together in children. The authors investigated how imagined and perceived MTs related to actual MTs in children and adolescents. It was found that imagined MTs were longer than execution MTs were. Perception MTs were lower than execution MTs for children and more consistent with execution MTs for adolescents. These results suggest potential mechanistic differences in action imagination and perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Yoxon
- a Center for Motor Control, Faculty of Kinesiology & Physical Education , University of Toronto , Canada
| | - Timothy N Welsh
- a Center for Motor Control, Faculty of Kinesiology & Physical Education , University of Toronto , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
Developmental data suggested that mental simulation skills become progressively dissociated from overt motor activity across development. Thus, efficient simulation is rather independent from current sensorimotor information. Here, we tested the impact of bodily (sensorimotor) information on simulation skills of adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Typically-developing (TD) and ASD participants judged laterality of hand images while keeping one arm flexed on chest or while holding both arms extended. Both groups were able to mentally simulate actions, but this ability was constrained by body posture more in ASD than in TD adolescents. The strong impact of actual body information on motor simulation implies that simulative skills are not fully effective in ASD individuals.
Collapse
|
20
|
Is there symmetry in motor imagery? Exploring different versions of the mental chronometry paradigm. Atten Percept Psychophys 2016; 78:1794-805. [PMID: 27173486 PMCID: PMC4972863 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1112-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Motor imagery and motor execution share similar processes. However, only some factors that affect motor execution affect motor imagery in the same way. We investigated whether bimanual coordination constraints (parallel movements are performed slower than symmetric movements) are observed in motor imagery and whether the way of implementing the mental chronometry paradigm, which is used to investigate motor imagery, influences the results. Participants imagined and executed repetitive symmetric and parallel bimanual movements in three different tasks. Participants performed a certain number of movement repetitions (number task), repeated movements for a fixed duration (duration task), and performed movements in synchrony with pacing sounds (synchronization task). In both, imagination and execution, inter-response intervals were longer with parallel movements than with symmetric movements (number task and duration task), and the percentage of correct movements was lower with parallel than with symmetric movements (synchronization task). Performance of imagined and executed movements was correlated in all tasks. However, imagination took longer or was rated as less accurate than execution, and in the synchronization task the coordination constraint affected accuracy more in execution than in imagination. Thus, motor imagery and overt execution involve shared and unique processes. The synchronization task offers a promising alternative to investigate motor imagery, because the speed-accuracy trade-off is taken into account, different tempi can be used, and psychometric functions can be calculated.
Collapse
|
21
|
Adams ILJ, Lust JM, Wilson PH, Steenbergen B. Testing predictive control of movement in children with developmental coordination disorder using converging operations. Br J Psychol 2016; 108:73-90. [DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12183] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2015] [Revised: 10/28/2015] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Imke L. J. Adams
- Behavioural Science Institute; Radboud University Nijmegen; The Netherlands
| | - Jessica M. Lust
- Behavioural Science Institute; Radboud University Nijmegen; The Netherlands
| | - Peter H. Wilson
- School of Psychology; Australian Catholic University; Melbourne Victoria Australia
| | - Bert Steenbergen
- Behavioural Science Institute; Radboud University Nijmegen; The Netherlands
- School of Psychology; Australian Catholic University; Melbourne Victoria Australia
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Sachet AB, Frey SH, Jacobs S, Taylor M. Development of the Correspondence Between Real and Imagined Fine and Gross Motor Actions. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2014.963585] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
23
|
Ferguson G, Wilson P, Smits-Engelsman B. The influence of task paradigm on motor imagery ability in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Hum Mov Sci 2015; 44:81-90. [DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2015] [Revised: 06/30/2015] [Accepted: 08/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
24
|
Corporaal SHA, Swinnen SP, Duysens J, Bruijn SM. Slow maturation of planning in obstacle avoidance in humans. J Neurophysiol 2015; 115:404-12. [PMID: 26561604 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00701.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Accepted: 11/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Complex gait (e.g., obstacle avoidance) requires a higher cognitive load than simple steady-state gait, which is a more automated movement. The higher levels of the central nervous system, responsible for adjusting motor plans to complex gait, develop throughout childhood into adulthood. Therefore, we hypothesize that gait strategies in complex gait are likely to mature until adulthood as well. However, little is known about the maturation of complex gait from childhood into adolescence and adulthood. To address this issue, we investigated obstacle avoidance in forty-four 8- to 18-yr-old participants who walked at preferred speed along a 6-m walkway on which a planar obstacle (150% of step length, 1 m wide) was projected. Participants avoided the obstacle by stepping over this projection, while lower body kinematics were recorded. Results showed that step length and speed adjustments during successful obstacle avoidance were similar across all ages, even though younger children modified step width to a greater extent. Additionally, the younger children used larger maximal toe elevations and take-off distances than older children. Moreover, during unsuccessful trials, younger children deployed exaggerated take-off distances, which resulted in obstacle contact upon the consecutive heel strike. These results indicate that obstacle avoidance is not fully matured in younger children, and that the inability to plan precise foot placements is an important factor contributing to failures in obstacle avoidance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sharissa H A Corporaal
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Stephan P Swinnen
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium;
| | - Jacques Duysens
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; Biomechatronics Laboratory, Mechatronics Department, Escola Politécnica, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Sjoerd M Bruijn
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; MOVE Research Institute Amsterdam, Department of Human Movement Sciences, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands; and Department of Orthopedic Surgery, First Affiliated Hospital, Fujian Medical University, Fujian, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
The ability of 6- to 8-year-old children to use motor imagery in a goal-directed pointing task. J Exp Child Psychol 2015; 139:221-33. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2014] [Revised: 06/15/2015] [Accepted: 06/15/2015] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
26
|
Spruijt S, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B. Current insights in the development of children's motor imagery ability. Front Psychol 2015; 6:787. [PMID: 26113832 PMCID: PMC4461854 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00787] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2015] [Accepted: 05/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Over the last two decades, the number of studies on motor imagery in children has witnessed a large expansion. Most studies used the hand laterality judgment paradigm or the mental chronometry paradigm to examine motor imagery ability. The main objective of the current review is to collate these studies to provide a more comprehensive insight in children’s motor imagery development and its age of onset. Motor imagery is a form of motor cognition and aligns with forward (or predictive) models of motor control. Studying age-related differences in motor imagery ability in children therefore provides insight in underlying processes of motor development during childhood. Another motivation for studying age-related differences in motor imagery is that in order to effectively apply motor imagery training in children (with motor impairments), it is pertinent to first establish the age at which children are actually able to perform motor imagery. Overall, performance in the imagery tasks develops between 5 and 12 years of age. The age of motor imagery onset, however, remains equivocal, as some studies indicate that children of 5 to 7 years old can already enlist motor imagery in an implicit motor imagery task, whereas other studies using explicit instructions revealed that children do not use motor imagery before the age of 10. From the findings of the current study, we can conclude that motor imagery training is potentially a feasible method for pediatric rehabilitation in children from 5 years on. We suggest that younger children are most likely to benefit from motor imagery training that is presented in an implicit way. Action observation training might be a beneficial adjunct to implicit motor imagery training. From 10 years of age, more explicit forms of motor imagery training can be effectively used.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steffie Spruijt
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - John van der Kamp
- Research Institute Move, VU University Amsterdam Amsterdam, Netherlands ; Institute of Human Performance, University of Hong Kong Hong Kong, China
| | - Bert Steenbergen
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen Nijmegen, Netherlands ; School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University Melbourne, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Molina M, Kudlinski C, Guilbert J, Spruijt S, Steenbergen B, Jouen F. Motor imagery for walking: a comparison between cerebral palsy adolescents with hemiplegia and diplegia. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2015; 37:95-101. [PMID: 25460223 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.10.053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2014] [Accepted: 10/29/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
The goal of the study was to investigate whether motor imagery (MI) could be observed in cerebral palsy (CP) participants presenting a bilateral affected body side (diplegia) as it has been previously revealed in participants presenting a unilateral body affected sided (hemiplegia). MI capacity for walking was investigated in CP adolescents diagnosed with hemiplegia (n=10) or diplegia (n=10) and in adolescents with typical motor development (n=10). Participants were explicitly asked to imagine walking before and after actually walking toward a target located at 4 m and 8 m. Movement durations for executed and imagined trials were recorded. ANOVA and Pearson's correlation analyses revealed the existence of time invariance between executed and imagined movement durations for the control group and both groups of CP participants. However, results revealed that MI capacity in CP participants was observed for the short distance (4 m) but not for the long distance (8 m). Moreover, even for short distance, CP participants performed worse than typical adolescents. These results are discussed inline of recent researches suggesting that MI in CP participants may not depend on the side of the lesion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michèle Molina
- Laboratoire Psychologie des Actions Langagières et Motrices, Université de Caen, France.
| | - Cyril Kudlinski
- Laboratoire Psychologie des Actions Langagières et Motrices, Université de Caen, France
| | - Jessica Guilbert
- Laboratoire Psychologie des Actions Langagières et Motrices, Université de Caen, France
| | - Steffie Spruijt
- Radboud University Nijmegen, Behavioural Science Institute, The Netherlands
| | - Bert Steenbergen
- Radboud University Nijmegen, Behavioural Science Institute, The Netherlands; Australian Catholic University, School of Psychology, Melbourne, Australia
| | - François Jouen
- Laboratoire Cognitions Humaine et Artificielle, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, France
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Ferguson G, Duysens J, Smits-Engelsman B. Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder are deficient in a visuo-manual tracking task requiring predictive control. Neuroscience 2015; 286:13-26. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.11.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2014] [Revised: 11/11/2014] [Accepted: 11/13/2014] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
29
|
Glazebrook CM, Kiernan D, Welsh TN, Tremblay L. How one breaks Fitts's Law and gets away with it: Moving further and faster involves more efficient online control. Hum Mov Sci 2014; 39:163-76. [PMID: 25485765 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2014] [Revised: 11/07/2014] [Accepted: 11/13/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Adam, Mol, Pratt, and Fischer (2006) reported what they termed "a violation of Fitts's Law" - when participants aimed to targets in an array, movement times (MTs) to the last target location (highest index of difficulty (ID)) were shorter than predicted by Fitts's Law. Based on the results of subsequent studies in which placeholders were present either during planning and/or execution stages of the movements, it was suggested that the violation may emerge because of context-dependent changes in planning processes. The present study examined this planning explanation by conducting detailed kinematic analyses of movements. Participants performed aiming movements to sets of 3 targets in different placeholder arrays with different movement amplitudes. Consistent with previous Fitts's Law violation findings, MTs were not significantly longer for movements to the last versus middle target location. Interestingly, the pattern of peak limb velocities (typically associated with planning processes) did not mirror the changes in MTs. On the other hand, analyses of the effector's spatial variability during the movement suggested greater involvement of online control processes when the target was in the last position. Based on these results, we suggest that the Fitts' Law violation observed here occurred because of more efficient online control processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cheryl M Glazebrook
- Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 2W6, Canada; Faculty of Kinesiology and Recreation Management, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada; Health, Leisure, and Human Performance Research Institute, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada.
| | - Dovin Kiernan
- Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 2W6, Canada; Department of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, College Park, SPH Building, College Park, MD 20742, United States
| | - Timothy N Welsh
- Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 2W6, Canada
| | - Luc Tremblay
- Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 2W6, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Sekiyama K, Kinoshita T, Soshi T. Strong biomechanical constraints on young children's mental imagery of hands. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2014; 1:140118. [PMID: 26064568 PMCID: PMC4448770 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.140118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2014] [Accepted: 11/10/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Mental rotation (MR) of body parts is a useful paradigm to investigate how people manipulate mental imagery related to body schema. It has been documented that adult participants use 'motor imagery' for MR of hands: a behavioural indication is a biomechanical effect, that is, hand pictures in orientations to which imitative hand movement would be biomechanically difficult require longer response times to be visually identified as the left or right hand. However, little is known about the typical developmental trajectory of the biomechanical effect, which could offer clues to understanding how children acquire the ability to manipulate body schema. This study investigated developmental changes in the biomechanical effect in schoolchildren. Eighty-four children (from 6 to 11 years old, grouped into 1st, 2nd, 3rd, 4th and 5th graders) and fifteen adults made hand laterality judgements in an MR paradigm. The results indicated that the biomechanical effect is stronger for younger children, and that there is a transitional period (around 7-8 years) during which children shift from action execution to imagery in manipulating body schema. The results suggest that mental imagery of hands has a stronger motor aspect in the transitional period than later in childhood and adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaoru Sekiyama
- Division of Cognitive Psychology, Kumamoto University, Kumamoto, Japan
- School of Systems Information Science, Future University, Hakodate, Japan
| | - Toshiro Kinoshita
- School of Systems Information Science, Future University, Hakodate, Japan
| | - Takahiro Soshi
- Division of Cognitive Psychology, Kumamoto University, Kumamoto, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
McCormick SA, Causer J, Holmes PS. The influence of early aging on eye movements during motor simulation. AGE (DORDRECHT, NETHERLANDS) 2014; 36:9671. [PMID: 25005270 PMCID: PMC4150898 DOI: 10.1007/s11357-014-9671-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2013] [Accepted: 06/18/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Movement based interventions such as imagery and action observation are used increasingly to support physical rehabilitation of adults during early aging. The efficacy of these more covert approaches is based on an intuitively appealing assumption that movement execution, imagery and observation share neural substrate; alteration of one influences directly the function of the other two. Using eye movement metrics this paper reports findings that question the congruency of the three conditions. The data reveal that simulating movement through imagery and action observation may offer older adults movement practice conditions that are not constrained by the age-related decline observed in physical conditions. In addition, the findings provide support for action observation as a more effective technique for movement reproduction in comparison to imagery. This concern for imagery was also seen in the less congruent temporal relationship in movement time between imagery and movement execution suggesting imagery inaccuracy in early aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sheree A. McCormick
- Cognitive Motor Function Group, Institute for Performance Research, Manchester Metropolitan University Cheshire Faculty, Crewe Green Road, Crewe Cheshire, CW1 5DU UK
| | - Joe Causer
- Brain and Behaviour Laboratory, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| | - Paul S. Holmes
- Cognitive Motor Function Group, Institute for Performance Research, Manchester Metropolitan University Cheshire Faculty, Crewe Green Road, Crewe Cheshire, CW1 5DU UK
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Assessing motor imagery using the hand rotation task: Does performance change across childhood? Hum Mov Sci 2014; 35:50-65. [DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2013] [Revised: 01/21/2014] [Accepted: 03/31/2014] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
33
|
Hyde C, Wilmut K, Fuelscher I, Williams J. Does implicit motor imagery ability predict reaching correction efficiency? A test of recent models of human motor control. J Mot Behav 2013; 45:259-69. [PMID: 23663190 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2013.785927] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Neurocomputational models of reaching indicate that efficient purposive correction of movement midflight (e.g., online control) depends on one's ability to generate and monitor an accurate internal (neural) movement representation. In the first study to test this empirically, the authors investigated the relationship between healthy young adults' implicit motor imagery performance and their capacity to correct their reaching trajectory. As expected, after controlling for general reaching speed, hierarchical regression demonstrated that imagery ability was a significant predictor of hand correction speed; that is, faster and more accurate imagery performance associated with faster corrections to reaching following target displacement at movement onset. They argue that these findings provide preliminary support for the view that a link exists between an individual's ability to represent movement mentally and correct movement online efficiently.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Hyde
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Health, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Spruijt S, Jouen F, Molina M, Kudlinski C, Guilbert J, Steenbergen B. Assessment of motor imagery in cerebral palsy via mental chronometry: the case of walking. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2013; 34:4154-4160. [PMID: 24076980 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2013] [Revised: 08/29/2013] [Accepted: 08/30/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Recent studies show varying results on whether motor imagery capacity is compromised in individuals with cerebral palsy (CP). Motor imagery studies in children predominantly used the implicit hand laterality task. In this task participants judge the laterality of displayed hand stimuli. A more explicit way of studying motor imagery is mental chronometry. This paradigm is based on the comparison between the movement durations of actually performing a task and imagining the same task. The current study explored motor imagery capacity in CP by means of mental chronometry of a whole body task. Movement durations of 20 individuals with CP (mean age=13 years, SD=3.6) were recorded in two conditions: actual walking and imagined walking. Six unique trajectories were used that varied in difficulty via manipulation of walking distance and path width. We found no main effect of condition (actual walking versus imagining) on movement durations. Difficulty of the walking trajectory did affect movement durations. In general, this was expressed by an increase in movement durations with increasing difficulty of the task. No interaction between task difficulty and movement condition was found. Our results show that task difficulty has similar effects on movement durations for both actual walking and imagined walking. These results exemplify that the tested individuals were able to use motor imagery in an explicit task involving walking. Previous studies using the implicit hand laterality task showed varying results on motor imagery capacity in CP. We therefore conclude that motor imagery capacity is task dependent and that an explicit paradigm as the one used in this study may reveal the true motor imagery capacity. The implications of these findings for the use of motor imagery training are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steffie Spruijt
- Radboud University Nijmegen, Behavioural Science Institute, PO Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Steenbergen B, Jongbloed-Pereboom M, Spruijt S, Gordon AM. Impaired motor planning and motor imagery in children with unilateral spastic cerebral palsy: challenges for the future of pediatric rehabilitation. Dev Med Child Neurol 2013; 55 Suppl 4:43-6. [PMID: 24237279 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/04/2013] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Compromised action performance is one of the most characteristic features of children with unilateral spastic cerebral palsy (USCP). Current rehabilitation efforts predominantly aim to improve the capacity and performance of the affected arm. Recent evidence, however, suggests that compromised motor planning may also negatively affect performance of activities of daily living. In this paper we will first discuss the recent evidence for this motor planning deficit, followed by studies on motor imagery in this population. Motor imagery is an experimental approach in which the contents of the motor plan become overt. Converging evidence indicates a compromised motor imagery ability in USCP. As the neural structures of both motor planning and motor imagery overlap, rehabilitation by motor imagery training may alleviate motor problems in USCP. Increasing evidence for this approach exists in older adults with stroke. We conclude this review with recommendations on such a training approach for children with USCP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bert Steenbergen
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Age-related changes in motor imagery from early childhood to adulthood: Probing the internal representation of speed-accuracy trade-offs. Hum Mov Sci 2013; 32:1151-62. [DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2012.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2012] [Revised: 06/27/2012] [Accepted: 06/30/2012] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
37
|
|
38
|
Crognier L, Skoura X, Vinter A, Papaxanthis C. Mental representation of arm motion dynamics in children and adolescents. PLoS One 2013; 8:e73042. [PMID: 24009727 PMCID: PMC3757012 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2013] [Accepted: 07/16/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor imagery, i.e., a mental state during which an individual internally represents an action without any overt motor output, is a potential tool to investigate action representation during development. Here, we took advantage of the inertial anisotropy phenomenon to investigate whether children can generate accurate motor predictions for movements with varying dynamics. Children (9 and 11 years), adolescents (14 years) and young adults (21 years) carried-out actual and mental arm movements in two different directions in the horizontal plane: rightwards (low inertia) and leftwards (high inertia). We recorded and compared actual and mental movement times. We found that actual movement times were greater for leftward than rightward arm movements in all groups. For mental movements, differences between leftward versus rightward movements were observed in the adults and adolescents, but not among the children. Furthermore, significant differences between actual and mental times were found at 9 and 11 years of age in the leftward direction. The ratio R/L (rightward direction/leftward direction), which indicates temporal differences between low inertia and high inertia movements, was inferior to 1 at all ages, except for the mental movements at 9 years of age, indicating than actual and mental movements were shorter for the rightward than leftward direction. Interestingly, while the ratio R/L of actual movements was constant across ages, it gradually decreased with age for mental movements. The ratio A/M (actual movement/mental movement), which indicates temporal differences between actual and mental movements, was near to 1 in the adults' groups, denoting accurate mental timing. In children and adolescents, an underestimation of mental movement times appeared for the leftward movements only. However, this overestimation gradually decreased with age. Our results showed a refinement in the motor imagery ability during development. Action representation reached maturation at adolescence, during which mental actions were tightly related to their actual production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lionel Crognier
- Université de Bourgogne, Unité de Formation et de Recherche en Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives, Dijon, France
- Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Unité 1093, Cognition, Action, et Plasticité Sensorimotrice, Dijon, France
| | - Xanthi Skoura
- Université de Bourgogne, Laboratoire d'Etude de l'Apprentissage et du Développement (LEAD), CNRS, UMR 5022, Dijon, France
| | - Annie Vinter
- Université de Bourgogne, Laboratoire d'Etude de l'Apprentissage et du Développement (LEAD), CNRS, UMR 5022, Dijon, France
| | - Charalambos Papaxanthis
- Université de Bourgogne, Unité de Formation et de Recherche en Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives, Dijon, France
- Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Unité 1093, Cognition, Action, et Plasticité Sensorimotrice, Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
McCormick SA, Causer J, Holmes PS. Active vision during action execution, observation and imagery: evidence for shared motor representations. PLoS One 2013; 8:e67761. [PMID: 23825683 PMCID: PMC3692467 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067761] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2013] [Accepted: 05/21/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The concept of shared motor representations between action execution and various covert conditions has been demonstrated through a number of psychophysiological modalities over the past two decades. Rarely, however, have researchers considered the congruence of physical, imaginary and observed movement markers in a single paradigm and never in a design where eye movement metrics are the markers. In this study, participants were required to perform a forward reach and point Fitts' Task on a digitizing tablet whilst wearing an eye movement system. Gaze metrics were used to compare behaviour congruence between action execution, action observation, and guided and unguided movement imagery conditions. The data showed that participants attended the same task-related visual cues between conditions but the strategy was different. Specifically, the number of fixations was significantly different between action execution and all covert conditions. In addition, fixation duration was congruent between action execution and action observation only, and both conditions displayed an indirect Fitts' Law effect. We therefore extend the understanding of the common motor representation by demonstrating, for the first time, common spatial eye movement metrics across simulation conditions and some specific temporal congruence for action execution and action observation. Our findings suggest that action observation may be an effective technique in supporting motor processes. The use of video as an adjunct to physical techniques may be beneficial in supporting motor planning in both performance and clinical rehabilitation environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sheree A. McCormick
- Institute for Performance Research, Manchester Metropolitan University, Crewe, United Kingdom
| | - Joe Causer
- Brain and Behaviour Laboratory, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom
| | - Paul S. Holmes
- Institute for Performance Research, Manchester Metropolitan University, Crewe, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Conson M, Mazzarella E, Trojano L. Developmental changes of the biomechanical effect in motor imagery. Exp Brain Res 2013; 226:441-9. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3456-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2012] [Accepted: 02/13/2013] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
41
|
Di Rienzo F, Collet C, Hoyek N, Guillot A. Selective effect of physical fatigue on motor imagery accuracy. PLoS One 2012; 7:e47207. [PMID: 23082148 PMCID: PMC3474822 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2012] [Accepted: 09/12/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
While the use of motor imagery (the mental representation of an action without overt execution) during actual training sessions is usually recommended, experimental studies examining the effect of physical fatigue on subsequent motor imagery performance are sparse and yielded divergent findings. Here, we investigated whether physical fatigue occurring during an intense sport training session affected motor imagery ability. Twelve swimmers (nine males, mean age 15.5 years) conducted a 45 min physically-fatiguing protocol where they swam from 70% to 100% of their maximal aerobic speed. We tested motor imagery ability immediately before and after fatigue state. Participants randomly imagined performing a swim turn using internal and external visual imagery. Self-reports ratings, imagery times and electrodermal responses, an index of alertness from the autonomic nervous system, were the dependent variables. Self-reports ratings indicated that participants did not encounter difficulty when performing motor imagery after fatigue. However, motor imagery times were significantly shortened during posttest compared to both pretest and actual turn times, thus indicating reduced timing accuracy. Looking at the selective effect of physical fatigue on external visual imagery did not reveal any difference before and after fatigue, whereas significantly shorter imagined times and electrodermal responses (respectively 15% and 48% decrease, p<0.001) were observed during the posttest for internal visual imagery. A significant correlation (r = 0.64; p<0.05) was observed between motor imagery vividness (estimated through imagery questionnaire) and autonomic responses during motor imagery after fatigue. These data support that unlike local muscle fatigue, physical fatigue occurring during intense sport training sessions is likely to affect motor imagery accuracy. These results might be explained by the updating of the internal representation of the motor sequence, due to temporary feedback originating from actual motor practice under fatigue. These findings provide insights to the co-dependent relationship between mental and motor processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Franck Di Rienzo
- CRIS EA 647, Performance Mentale, Motrice et du Matériel (P3M), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, F-69000 Villeurbanne Cedex, France
| | - Christian Collet
- CRIS EA 647, Performance Mentale, Motrice et du Matériel (P3M), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, F-69000 Villeurbanne Cedex, France
| | - Nady Hoyek
- CRIS EA 647, Performance Mentale, Motrice et du Matériel (P3M), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, F-69000 Villeurbanne Cedex, France
| | - Aymeric Guillot
- CRIS EA 647, Performance Mentale, Motrice et du Matériel (P3M), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, F-69000 Villeurbanne Cedex, France
- Institut Universitaire de France, F-75000 Paris, France
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Heremans E, Smits-Engelsman B, Caeyenberghs K, Vercruysse S, Nieuwboer A, Feys P, Helsen W. Keeping an eye on imagery: the role of eye movements during motor imagery training. Neuroscience 2011; 195:37-44. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.07.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2011] [Revised: 07/08/2011] [Accepted: 07/12/2011] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
43
|
Moran A, Guillot A, Macintyre T, Collet C. Re-imagining motor imagery: building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. Br J Psychol 2011; 103:224-47. [PMID: 22506748 DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02068.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 94] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
One of the most remarkable capacities of the mind is its ability to simulate sensations, actions, and other types of experience. A mental simulation process that has attracted recent attention from cognitive neuroscientists and sport psychologists is motor imagery or the mental rehearsal of actions without engaging in the actual physical movements involved. Research on motor imagery is important in psychology because it provides an empirical window on consciousness and movement planning, rectifies a relative neglect of non-visual types of mental imagery, and has practical implications for skill learning and skilled performance in special populations (e.g., athletes, surgeons). Unfortunately, contemporary research on motor imagery is hampered by a variety of semantic, conceptual, and methodological issues that prevent cross-fertilization of ideas between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. In this paper, we review these issues, suggest how they can be resolved, and sketch some potentially fruitful new directions for inter-disciplinary research in motor imagery.
Collapse
|
44
|
Hoyek N, Champely S, Collet C, Fargier P, Guillot A. Age and gender-related differences in the temporal congruence development between motor imagery and motor performance. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2009. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
45
|
Static and Dynamic Visuomotor Task Performance in Children With Acquired Brain Injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2009; 24:363-73. [DOI: 10.1097/htr.0b013e3181af0810] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
46
|
Caeyenberghs K, van Roon D, Swinnen S, Smits-Engelsman B. Deficits in executed and imagined aiming performance in brain-injured children. Brain Cogn 2009; 69:154-61. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.07.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2008] [Revised: 07/01/2008] [Accepted: 07/01/2008] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|