1
|
EMERY KARAJ, ISHERWOOD ZOEYJ, WEBSTER MICHAELA. Gaining the system: limits to compensating color deficiencies through post-receptoral gain changes. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2023; 40:A16-A25. [PMID: 37132998 PMCID: PMC10157001 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.480035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2022] [Accepted: 12/14/2022] [Indexed: 05/04/2023]
Abstract
Color percepts of anomalous trichromats are often more similar to normal trichromats than predicted from their receptor spectral sensitivities, suggesting that post-receptoral mechanisms can compensate for chromatic losses. The basis for these adjustments and the extent to which they could discount the deficiency are poorly understood. We modeled the patterns of compensation that might result from increasing the gains in post-receptoral neurons to offset their weakened inputs. Individual neurons and the population responses jointly encode luminance and chromatic signals. As a result, they cannot independently adjust for a change in the chromatic inputs, predicting only partial recovery of the chromatic responses and increased responses to achromatic contrast. These analyses constrain the potential sites and mechanisms of compensation for a color loss and characterize the utility and limits of neural gain changes for calibrating color vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- KARA J. EMERY
- Department of Psychology and Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno NV 89557
- Center for Data Science, New York University, New York NY 10011
| | - ZOEY J. ISHERWOOD
- Department of Psychology and Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno NV 89557
| | - MICHAEL A. WEBSTER
- Department of Psychology and Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno NV 89557
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Robinson AE, Bosten JM, MacLeod DIA. Nonlinear cortical encoding of color predicts enhanced McCollough effects in anomalous trichromats. Vision Res 2023; 203:108153. [PMID: 36470153 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108153] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2022] [Revised: 10/24/2022] [Accepted: 11/03/2022] [Indexed: 12/05/2022]
Abstract
Nonlinear encoding of chromatic contrast by the early visual cortex predicts that anomalous trichromats will show a larger McCollough effect than normal trichromats. In Experiment 1 we employed the McCollough effect to probe the cortical representation of saturation in normal trichromats, and used the results to predict enhanced McCollough effects for anomalous trichromats, which we measured in Experiment 2. In Experiment 1 three participants adapted to red and green orthogonal gratings of four different saturations. Using nulling to measure aftereffect strength, we found that halving the saturation of the inducing gratings decreased aftereffect strength only slightly, consistent with a compressive coding of saturation in early visual cortex. In anomalous trichromats, cone contrasts between red and green are greatly decreased from those of normal trichromats, but induced aftereffects are only slightly decreased, because of the non-linearity in the cortical encoding of saturation. To null the aftereffect, however, the retinal color deficiency must be overcome by adding more color to the null than required by normal trichromats. We confirmed this prediction in Experiment 2 where four anomalous trichromats required nulling stimuli approximately four times more saturated than did normal trichromats. We consider two competing models to explain our results: in a 'pigment swap' model anomalous trichromats have an altered photopigment but process color postreceptorally in the same way as normal trichromats; in a 'postreceptoral compensation' model the cortical representation of red-green contrasts is amplified to compensate for reduced cone contrasts. The latter provided a better fit to our data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alan E Robinson
- UC San Diego Department of Psychology; UC San Diego Department of Cognitive Science
| | - Jenny M Bosten
- UC San Diego Department of Psychology; School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Abstract
In our tendency to discuss the objective properties of the external world, we may fail to notice that our subjective perceptions of those properties differ between individuals. Variability at all levels of the color vision system creates diversity in color perception, from discrimination to color matching, appearance, and subjective experience, such that each of us lives in a unique perceptual world. In this review, I discuss what is known about individual differences in color perception and its determinants, particularly considering genetically mediated variability in cone photopigments and the paradoxical effects of visual environments in both contributing to and counteracting individual differences. I make the case that, as well as being of interest in their own right and crucial for a complete account of color vision, individual differences can be used as a methodological tool in color science for the insights that they offer about the underlying mechanisms of perception. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Vision Science, Volume 8 is September 2022. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenny M Bosten
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom;
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Rezeanu D, Barborek R, Neitz M, Neitz J. Potential value of color vision aids for varying degrees of color vision deficiency. OPTICS EXPRESS 2022; 30:8857-8875. [PMID: 35299329 PMCID: PMC8970702 DOI: 10.1364/oe.451331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
Red-green color vision deficiency (CVD) is the most common single locus genetic disorder in humans, affecting approximately 8% of males and 0.4% of females [G. H. M. Waaler, Acta Ophthalmol.5, 309 (2009)10.1111/j.1755-3768.1927.tb01016.x]; however, only about 1/4 of CVD individuals are dichromats who rely on only two cone types for color vision. The remaining 3/4 are anomalous trichromats whose CVD is milder, being based on three cone types, and who still perform remarkably well on many color-based tasks. To illustrate this, we have developed an algorithm that computes the relative loss of color discrimination in red-green CVD individuals with varying degrees of deficiency and accurately simulates their color experience for color normal observers. The resulting simulation illustrates the large gap in color discrimination between dichromats and even the most severe anomalous trichromats, showing that, relative to dichromats, the majority of anomalous trichromats can function without aids for color vision deficiency.
Collapse
|
5
|
Smet KA, Webster MA, Whitehead LA. Color appearance model incorporating contrast adaptation - implications for individual differences in color vision. COLOR RESEARCH AND APPLICATION 2021; 46:759-773. [PMID: 34334884 PMCID: PMC8320589 DOI: 10.1002/col.22620] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2020] [Accepted: 01/14/2021] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
Color appearance models use standard color matching functions to derive colorimetric information from spectral radiometric measurements of a visual environment, and they process that information to predict color perceptual attributes such as hue, chroma and lightness. That processing is usually done by equations with fixed numerical coefficients that were predetermined to yield optimal agreement for a given standard observer. Here we address the well-known fact that, among color-normal observers, there are significant differences of color matching functions. These cause disagreements between individuals as to whether certain colors match, an important effect that is often called observer metamerism. Yet how these individual sensitivity differences translate into differences in perceptual metrics is not fully addressed by many appearance models. It might seem that appearance could be predicted by substituting an individual's color matching functions into an otherwise-unchanged color appearance model, but this is problematic because the model's coefficients were not optimized for the new observer. Here we explore a solution guided by the idea that processes of adaptation in the visual system tend to compensate color perception for differences in cone responses and consequent color matching functions. For this purpose, we developed a simple color appearance model that uses only a few numerical coefficients, yet accurately predicts the perceptual attributes of Munsell samples under a selected standard lighting condition. We then added a feedback loop to automatically adjust the model coefficients, in response to switching between cone fundamentals simulating different observers and color matching functions. This adjustment is intended to model long term contrast adaptation in the vision system by maintaining average overall color contrast levels. Incorporating this adaptation principle into color appearance models could allow better assessments of displays and illumination systems, to help improve color appearances for most observers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Lorne A. Whitehead
- Department of Physics and Astronomy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Boehm AE, Bosten J, MacLeod DIA. Color discrimination in anomalous trichromacy: Experiment and theory. Vision Res 2021; 188:85-95. [PMID: 34293614 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.05.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2020] [Revised: 05/28/2021] [Accepted: 05/29/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
In anomalous trichromacy, the color signals available from comparing the activities of the two classes of cone sensitive in the medium and long wavelength parts of the spectrum are much reduced from those available in normal trichromacy, and color discrimination thresholds along the red-green axis are correspondingly elevated. Yet there is evidence that suprathreshold color perception is relatively preserved; this has led to the suggestion that anomalous trichromats post-receptorally amplify their impoverished red-green signals. To test this idea, we measured chromatic discrimination from white and from saturated red and green pedestals. If there is no post-receptoral compensation, the anomalous trichromat's loss of chromatic contrast will apply equally to the pedestal and to the test color. Coupled with a compressively nonlinear neural representation of saturation, this means that a given pedestal contrast will cause a smaller than normal modulation of discrimination sensitivity. We examined cases where chromatic pedestals impair the color discrimination of normal trichromatic observers. As predicted, anomalous observers experienced less impairment than normal trichromats, though they remained less sensitive than normal trichromats. Although the effectiveness of chromatic pedestals in impairing color discrimination was less for anomalous than for normal trichromats, the chromatic pedestals were more effective for anomalous observers than would be expected if the anomalous post-receptoral visual system were the same as in normal trichromacy; the hypothesis of zero compensation can be rejected. This might suggest that the effective contrast of the pedestal is post-receptorally amplified. But on closer analysis, the results do not support candidate simple models involving post-receptoral compensation either.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra E Boehm
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA; Vision Science Graduate Group, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA; School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA
| | - Jenny Bosten
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA; School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Lindsey DT, Brown AM, Hutchinson LN. Appearance of special colors in deuteranomalous trichromacy. Vision Res 2021; 185:77-87. [PMID: 33962212 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2020] [Revised: 03/04/2021] [Accepted: 04/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Deuteranomalous color matching behavior is different from normal because the middle-wavelength sensitive cones contain an abnormal L' pigment instead of the M pigment of the normal observer. However, there is growing evidence that deuteranomalous color experience is not very different from that of normal trichromats. Here, normal and deuteranomalous observers chose monochromatic unique yellow lights. They also chose broadband lights, displayed on a computer monitor, that corresponded to eight special colors: the Hering unique hues (red, yellow, green, blue), and binary colors perceptually midway between them (orange, lime, cyan, purple). Deuteranomalous monochromatic unique yellow was shifted towards red, but all the broadband special color selections were physically similar for normal and deuteranomalous observers. Deuteranomalous special colors, including monochromatic unique yellow, were similar to those of normal observers when expressed in a color-opponent chromaticity diagram based on their own visual pigments, but only if (1) color-opponent responses were normalized to white, and (2) the deuteranomalous diagram was expanded along the r - g dimension to compensate for the reduced difference between deuteranomalous L- and L'-cone photopigments. Particularly, deuteranomalous observers did not choose binary colors with extra r - g impact to overcome their insensitivity along the r - g dimension. This result can only be compatible with the known abnormality of the deuteranomalous L' photopigment if deuteranomalous observers adjust their perceptual representation of colors to compensate for their color vision deficiency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Delwin T Lindsey
- Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, USA; College of Optometry, Ohio State University, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Vanston JE, Tregillus KEM, Webster MA, Crognale MA. Task-dependent contrast gain in anomalous trichromats. Vision Res 2021; 184:14-22. [PMID: 33773293 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2020] [Revised: 01/25/2021] [Accepted: 02/04/2021] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Anomalous trichromacy is a form of color vision deficiency characterized by the presence of three cone types, but with shifted spectral sensitivities for L or M cones, causing a red-green color deficiency. However, long-term adaptation to this impoverished opponent input may allow for a more normal color experience at the suprathreshold level ("compensation"). Recent experimental evidence points to the presence of compensation in some tasks. The current study used threshold detection, suprathreshold contrast matching, and a reaction-time task to compare contrast coding in normal and anomalous observers along the cardinal cone-opponent axes. Compared to color normals, anomals required more L-M contrast, but not S contrast, to detect stimuli and to match an achromatic reference stimulus. Reaction times were measured for several contrast levels along the two cone-opponent axes. Anomals had higher overall reaction times, but their reaction-time versus contrast functions could be matched to those of controls simply by scaling contrast by the detection thresholds. Anomalous participants were impaired relative to controls for L-M stimuli in all three tasks. However, the contrast losses were three times greater for thresholds and reaction times than for suprathreshold matches. These data provide evidence for compensation in anomalous trichromats, but highlight the role that the experimental task plays in revealing it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John E Vanston
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno 1664 N Virginia St., Reno, NV 89557, USA; School of Optometry and Vision Science Graduate Group, University of California, Minor Hall, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA.
| | - Katherine E M Tregillus
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno 1664 N Virginia St., Reno, NV 89557, USA
| | - Michael A Webster
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno 1664 N Virginia St., Reno, NV 89557, USA
| | - Michael A Crognale
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno 1664 N Virginia St., Reno, NV 89557, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Emery KJ, Kuppuswamy Parthasarathy M, Joyce DS, Webster MA. Color perception and compensation in color deficiencies assessed with hue scaling. Vision Res 2021; 183:1-15. [PMID: 33636681 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2020] [Revised: 12/07/2020] [Accepted: 01/14/2021] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Anomalous trichromats have three classes of cone receptors but with smaller separation in the spectral sensitivities of their longer-wave (L or M) cones compared to normal trichromats. As a result, the differences in the responses of the longer-wave cones are smaller, resulting in a weaker input to opponent mechanisms that compare the LvsM responses. Despite this, previous studies have found that their color percepts are more similar to normal trichromats than the smaller LvsM differences predict, suggesting that post-receptoral processes might amplify their responses to compensate for the weaker opponent inputs. We evaluated the degree and form of compensation using a hue-scaling task, in which the appearance of different hues is described by the perceived proportions of red-green or blue-yellow primary colors. The scaling functions were modeled to estimate the relative salience of the red-green to blue-yellow components. The red-green amplitudes of the 10 anomalous observers were 1.5 times weaker than for a group of 26 normal controls. However, their relative sensitivity at threshold for detecting LvsM chromatic contrast was on average 6 times higher, consistent with a 4-fold gain in the suprathreshold hue-scaling responses. Within-observer variability in the settings was similar for the two groups, suggesting that the suprathreshold gain did not similarly amplify the noise, at least for the dimension of hue. While the compensation was pronounced it was nevertheless partial, and anomalous observers differed systematically from the controls in the shapes of the hue-scaling functions and the corresponding loci of their color categories. Factor analyses further revealed different patterns of individual differences between the groups. We discuss the implications of these results for understanding both the processes of compensation for a color deficiency and the limits of these processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kara J Emery
- Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV 89557, United States
| | - Mohana Kuppuswamy Parthasarathy
- Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV 89557, United States
| | - Daniel S Joyce
- Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV 89557, United States
| | - Michael A Webster
- Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV 89557, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Tregillus KEM, Isherwood ZJ, Vanston JE, Engel SA, MacLeod DIA, Kuriki I, Webster MA. Color Compensation in Anomalous Trichromats Assessed with fMRI. Curr Biol 2020; 31:936-942.e4. [PMID: 33326771 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.11.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2020] [Revised: 10/14/2020] [Accepted: 11/16/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Anomalous trichromacy is a common form of congenital color deficiency resulting from a genetic alteration in the photopigments of the eye's light receptors. The changes reduce sensitivity to reddish and greenish hues, yet previous work suggests that these observers may experience the world to be more colorful than their altered receptor sensitivities would predict, potentially indicating an amplification of post-receptoral signals. However, past evidence suggesting such a gain adjustment rests on subjective measures of color appearance or salience. We directly tested for neural amplification by using fMRI to measure cortical responses in color-anomalous and normal control observers. Color contrast response functions were measured in two experiments with different tasks to control for attentional factors. Both experiments showed a predictable reduction in chromatic responses for anomalous trichromats in primary visual cortex. However, in later areas V2v and V3v, chromatic responses in the two groups were indistinguishable. Our results provide direct evidence for neural plasticity that compensates for the deficiency in the initial receptor color signals and suggest that the site of this compensation is in early visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine E M Tregillus
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, 1664 N. Virginia Street, Reno, NV 89557, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, 75 E River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.
| | - Zoey J Isherwood
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, 1664 N. Virginia Street, Reno, NV 89557, USA
| | - John E Vanston
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, 1664 N. Virginia Street, Reno, NV 89557, USA
| | - Stephen A Engel
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, 75 E River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA
| | - Donald I A MacLeod
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, Muir Lane, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | - Ichiro Kuriki
- Research Institute for Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai 2 Chome-1-1 Katahira, Aoba Ward, Sendai Miyagi 980-8577, Japan
| | - Michael A Webster
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, 1664 N. Virginia Street, Reno, NV 89557, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Isherwood ZJ, Joyce DS, Parthasarathy MK, Webster MA. Plasticity in perception: insights from color vision deficiencies. Fac Rev 2020; 9:8. [PMID: 33659940 PMCID: PMC7886061 DOI: 10.12703/b/9-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Inherited color vision deficiencies typically result from a loss or alteration of the visual photopigments absorbing light and thus impact the very first step of seeing. There is growing interest in how subsequent steps in the visual pathway might be calibrated to compensate for the altered receptor signals, with the possibility that color coding and color percepts might be less severely impacted than the receptor differences predict. These compensatory adjustments provide important insights into general questions about sensory plasticity and the sensory and cognitive processes underlying how we experience color.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daniel S Joyce
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, NV, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
A new study finds that individuals with color deficiencies report long-term changes in their color vision after only a few days of wearing glasses that boost color contrasts, potentially because they learn to see or interpret color in new ways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Webster
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, 1664 N. Virginia Street, Reno, NV 89557, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Cohen MA, Botch TL, Robertson CE. The limits of color awareness during active, real-world vision. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020; 117:13821-13827. [PMID: 32513698 PMCID: PMC7306755 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1922294117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Color ignites visual experience, imbuing the world with meaning, emotion, and richness. As soon as an observer opens their eyes, they have the immediate impression of a rich, colorful experience that encompasses their entire visual world. Here, we show that this impression is surprisingly inaccurate. We used head-mounted virtual reality (VR) to place observers in immersive, dynamic real-world environments, which they naturally explored via saccades and head turns. Meanwhile, we monitored their gaze with in-headset eye tracking and then systematically altered the visual environments such that only the parts of the scene they were looking at were presented in color and the rest of the scene (i.e., the visual periphery) was entirely desaturated. We found that observers were often completely unaware of these drastic alterations to their visual world. In the most extreme case, almost a third of observers failed to notice when less than 5% of the visual display was presented in color. This limitation on perceptual awareness could not be explained by retinal neuroanatomy or previous studies of peripheral visual processing using more traditional psychophysical approaches. In a second study, we measured color detection thresholds using a staircase procedure while a set of observers intentionally attended to the periphery. Still, we found that observers were unaware when a large portion of their field of view was desaturated. Together, these results show that during active, naturalistic viewing conditions, our intuitive sense of a rich, colorful visual world is largely incorrect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Cohen
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Amherst College, Amherst, MA 01002;
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, 02139
| | - Thomas L Botch
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH 03755
| | - Caroline E Robertson
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH 03755
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Webster MA. The Verriest Lecture: Adventures in blue and yellow. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2020; 37:V1-V14. [PMID: 32400510 PMCID: PMC7233477 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.383625] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2019] [Accepted: 12/20/2019] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Conventional models of color vision assume that blue and yellow (along with red and green) are the fundamental building blocks of color appearance, yet how these hues are represented in the brain and whether and why they might be special are questions that remain shrouded in mystery. Many studies have explored the visual encoding of color categories, from the statistics of the environment to neural processing to perceptual experience. Blue and yellow are tied to salient features of the natural color world, and these features have likely shaped several important aspects of color vision. However, it remains less certain that these dimensions are encoded as primary or "unique" in the visual representation of color. There are also striking differences between blue and yellow percepts that may reflect high-level inferences about the world, specifically about the colors of light and surfaces. Moreover, while the stimuli labeled as blue or yellow or other basic categories show a remarkable degree of constancy within the observer, they all vary independently of one another across observers. This pattern of variation again suggests that blue and yellow and red and green are not a primary or unitary dimension of color appearance, and instead suggests a representation in which different hues reflect qualitatively different categories rather than quantitative differences within an underlying low-dimensional "color space."
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
Individual differences are a conspicuous feature of color vision and arise from many sources, in both the observer and the world. These differences have important practical implications for comparing and correcting perception and performance, and important theoretical implications for understanding the design principles underlying color coding. Color percepts within and between individuals often vary less than the variations in spectral sensitivity might predict. This stability is achieved by a variety of processes that compensate perception for the sensitivity limits of the eye and brain. Yet judgments of color between individuals can also vary widely, and in ways that are not readily explained by differences in sensitivity or the environment. These differences are uncorrelated across different color categories, and could reflect how these categories are learned or represented.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kara J Emery
- Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno
| | - Michael A Webster
- Graduate Program in Integrative Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Ao M, Li X, Qiu W, Hou Z, Su J, Wang W. The impact of age-related cataracts on colour perception, postoperative recovery and related spectra derived from test of hue perception. BMC Ophthalmol 2019; 19:56. [PMID: 30786855 PMCID: PMC6383292 DOI: 10.1186/s12886-019-1057-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2018] [Accepted: 01/29/2019] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Cataract patients were always excluded from studies on ageing of colour vision; thus, effect of age-related cataracts on deterioration of colour perception has not been analysed. In present study, impacts of age-related cataracts on colour discrimination, postoperative recovery and related spectra were investigated. Methods In this cohort study, thirty age-related cataract patients scheduled for binocular surgery and 30 elderly volunteers were enrolled. Colour discrimination under photopic (1000 lx) and mesopic (40 lx) conditions was evaluated with Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue test. The total error score (TES) and partial error score (PES) were calculated. Results Preoperatively, the TES in the patient group was 129.7 ± 59.5 at 1000 lx and 194.6 ± 74.5 at 40 lx, exhibiting worse discrimination than the volunteer group (TES1000lux = 71.5 ± 37.5 and TES40lux = 113.1 ± 38.8, p ≤ 0.001). Inferior perception were detected in the yellow to green-yellow (Y-GY), green-yellow to green (GY-G), green to blue-green (G-BG) and blue-green to blue (BG-B) colour bands (p ≤ 0.003), corresponding to the 470 nm–580 nm range of the visible light spectrum. Under mesopic conditions, the impact expanded to all colour bands except for yellow-red to yellow (YR-Y). Postoperatively, the TES in the patient group were 80.4 ± 62.4 at 1000 lx and 112.0 ± 85.2 at 40 lx, which were lower than those of the preoperative phase (p ≤ 0.001) but similar to those of the volunteer group (p ≥ 0.505). Postoperative improvement occurred in the Y-GY, GY-G and G-BG colour bands (490 nm to 580 nm) at 1000 lx (p ≤ 0.001) and shifted to the Y-GY, GY-G, G-BG and BG-B colour bands (470 nm to 580 nm) at 40 lx (p ≤ 0.001). Deterioration of hue perception for decrement of illumination was detected in the red to yellow-red (R-YR), Y-GY, G-BG, BG-B, blue to purple-blue (B-PB) and red-purple to red (RP-R) colour bands (450 nm to 500 nm) in the volunteer group (p ≤ 0.002) and the R-YR, G-BG, BG-B, B-PB, PB-P and red-purple to red (RP-R) colour bands (from the short-wavelength end to 500 nm) in the patient group preoperatively (p ≤ 0.001). Conclusions Phacoemulsification could effectively rebuild colour perception in patients with age-related cataract. The postoperative benefits were most significant in colour bands corresponding with spectrum from 470 nm to 580 nm. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1186/s12886-019-1057-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mingxin Ao
- Department of Ophthalmology, Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China
| | - Xuemin Li
- Department of Ophthalmology, Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China
| | - Weiqiang Qiu
- Department of Ophthalmology, Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China
| | - Zhiqiang Hou
- Department of Ophthalmology, Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China
| | - Jie Su
- Department of Ophthalmology, Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China
| | - Wei Wang
- Department of Ophthalmology, Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China. .,Beijing Key Laboratory of Restoration of Damaged Ocular Nerve, Number Forty-Nine North Garden Road Haidian District, Beijing, 86100191, China.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Vanston JE, Crognale MA. Effects of eccentricity on color contrast. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2018; 35:B122-B129. [PMID: 29603965 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.35.00b122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2017] [Accepted: 01/24/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Using near-threshold stimuli, human color sensitivity has been shown to decrease across the visual field, likely due in part to physiological differences between the fovea and periphery. It remains unclear to what extent this holds true for suprathreshold stimuli. The current study used suprathreshold contrast matching to examine how perceived contrast varies with eccentricity along the cardinal axes in a cone-opponent space. Our data show that, despite increasing stimulus size in the periphery, the LM axis stimuli were still perceived as reduced in contrast, whereas the S axis perceived contrast was observed to increase with eccentricity.
Collapse
|
18
|
Rabin J, Kryder A, Lam D. Binocular facilitation of cone-specific visual evoked potentials in colour deficiency. Clin Exp Optom 2017. [PMID: 28636141 DOI: 10.1111/cxo.12567] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Neural compensatory mechanisms have been proposed, which preserve the binocular visual field in glaucoma, as well as cognition in Alzheimer's disease and motor function in Parkinson's disease. It is conceivable that comparable mechanisms operate to preserve function in congenital and/or dystrophic disease. In hereditary colour vision deficiency (CVD), we observed significant facilitation in the amplitude of the binocular cone-specific visual evoked potential (VEP) compared to the monocular amplitude for the cone type corresponding to the CVD. We propose that this finding may reflect preservation of function in hereditary colour vision deficiency. METHODS Binocular and monocular L, M and S cone-specific VEPs were recorded from 12 colour vision deficient subjects and 17 with normal colour vision, confirmed to be CVD or normal on a battery of colour vision tests. Binocular VEP amplitudes were compared to monocular amplitudes within subjects and between subject groups. RESULTS Subjects with CVDs showed binocular facilitation of VEP amplitude (enhancement more than 2.0 times; mean: 2.8 times, p = 0.0003) for the cone type corresponding to their CVD. Mean facilitation of 2.8 times exceeded binocular enhancement for other cone types within CVDs (2.8 times versus 1.2 times) and compared to colour vision normals (2.8 times versus 1.2 times). CONCLUSIONS Hereditary CVDs show binocular facilitation of cone VEP signals for the cone type corresponding to their CVD. As CVD is typically assessed with foveal stimuli, our findings using wider-field binocular stimulation suggest that enhanced colour perception may occur in CVD across a more extensive area of visual field. These results may relate to binocular visual field enhancement in glaucoma and improved colour vision in CVD at supra-threshold levels of stimulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeff Rabin
- The Rosenberg School of Optometry, University of the Incarnate Word, San Antonio, Texas, USA
| | - Andrew Kryder
- The Rosenberg School of Optometry, University of the Incarnate Word, San Antonio, Texas, USA
| | - Dan Lam
- The Rosenberg School of Optometry, University of the Incarnate Word, San Antonio, Texas, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
Many techniques have been developed to visualize how an image would appear to an individual with a different visual sensitivity: e.g., because of optical or age differences, or a color deficiency or disease. This protocol describes a technique for incorporating sensory adaptation into the simulations. The protocol is illustrated with the example of color vision, but is generally applicable to any form of visual adaptation. The protocol uses a simple model of human color vision based on standard and plausible assumptions about the retinal and cortical mechanisms encoding color and how these adjust their sensitivity to both the average color and range of color in the prevailing stimulus. The gains of the mechanisms are adapted so that their mean response under one context is equated for a different context. The simulations help reveal the theoretical limits of adaptation and generate "adapted images" that are optimally matched to a specific environment or observer. They also provide a common metric for exploring the effects of adaptation within different observers or different environments. Characterizing visual perception and performance with these images provides a novel tool for studying the functions and consequences of long-term adaptation in vision or other sensory systems.
Collapse
|
20
|
Sato K, Inoue T. Perception of color emotions for single colors in red-green defective observers. PeerJ 2016; 4:e2751. [PMID: 27957394 PMCID: PMC5149061 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.2751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2016] [Accepted: 11/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
It is estimated that inherited red-green color deficiency, which involves both the protan and deutan deficiency types, is common in men. For red-green defective observers, some reddish colors appear desaturated and brownish, unlike those seen by normal observers. Despite its prevalence, few studies have investigated the effects that red-green color deficiency has on the psychological properties of colors (color emotions). The current study investigated the influence of red-green color deficiency on the following six color emotions: cleanliness, freshness, hardness, preference, warmth, and weight. Specifically, this study aimed to: (1) reveal differences between normal and red-green defective observers in rating patterns of six color emotions; (2) examine differences in color emotions related to the three cardinal channels in human color vision; and (3) explore relationships between color emotions and color naming behavior. Thirteen men and 10 women with normal vision and 13 men who were red-green defective performed both a color naming task and an emotion rating task with 32 colors from the Berkeley Color Project (BCP). Results revealed noticeable differences in the cleanliness and hardness ratings between the normal vision observers, particularly in women, and red-green defective observers, which appeared mainly for colors in the orange to cyan range, and in the preference and warmth ratings for colors with cyan and purple hues. Similarly, naming errors also mainly occurred in the cyan colors. A regression analysis that included the three cone-contrasts (i.e., red-green, blue-yellow, and luminance) as predictors significantly accounted for variability in color emotion ratings for the red-green defective observers as much as the normal individuals. Expressly, for warmth ratings, the weight of the red-green opponent channel was significantly lower in color defective observers than in normal participants. In addition, the analyses for individual warmth ratings in the red-green defective group revealed that luminance cone-contrast was a significant predictor in most red-green-defective individuals. Together, these results suggest that red-green defective observers tend to rely on the blue-yellow channel and luminance to compensate for the weak sensitivity of long- and medium-wavelength (L-M) cone-contrasts, when rating color warmth.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Keiko Sato
- Faculty of Engineering, Kagawa University , Takamatsu , Kagawa , Japan
| | - Takaaki Inoue
- Graduate School of Engineering, Kagawa University , Takamatsu , Kagawa , Japan
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
|
22
|
Boehm AE, MacLeod DIA, Bosten JM. Compensation for red-green contrast loss in anomalous trichromats. J Vis 2014; 14:19. [PMID: 25413625 DOI: 10.1167/14.13.19] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
For anomalous trichromats, threshold contrasts for color differences captured by the L and M cones and their anomalous analogs are much higher than for normal trichromats. The greater spectral overlap of the cone sensitivities reduces chromatic contrast both at and above threshold. But above threshold, adaptively nonlinear processing might compensate for the chromatically impoverished photoreceptor inputs. Ratios of sensitivity for threshold variations and for color appearance along the two cardinal axes of MacLeod-Boynton chromaticity space were calculated for three groups: normals (N = 15), deuteranomals (N = 9), and protanomals (N = 5). Using a four-alternative forced choice (4AFC) task, threshold sensitivity was measured in four color-directions along the two cardinal axes. For the same participants, we reconstructed perceptual color spaces for the positions of 25 hues using multidimensional scaling (MDS). From the reconstructed color spaces we extracted "color difference ratios," defined as ratios for the size of perceived color differences along the L/(L + M) axis relative to those along the S/(L + M) axis, analogous to "sensitivity ratios" extracted from the 4AFC task. In the 4AFC task, sensitivity ratios were 38% of normal for deuteranomals and 19% of normal for protanomals. Yet, in the MDS results, color difference ratios were 86% of normal for deuteranomals and 67% of normal for protanomals. Thus, the contraction along the L/(L + M) axis shown in the perceptual color spaces of anomalous trichromats is far smaller than predicted by their reduced sensitivity, suggesting that an adaptive adjustment of postreceptoral gain may magnify the cone signals of anomalous trichromats to exploit the range of available postreceptoral neural signals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A E Boehm
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA Vision Science Graduate Group, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA
| | - D I A MacLeod
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - J M Bosten
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Webster MA. Probing the functions of contextual modulation by adapting images rather than observers. Vision Res 2014; 104:68-79. [PMID: 25281412 PMCID: PMC4253075 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2014] [Revised: 08/21/2014] [Accepted: 09/01/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Countless visual aftereffects have illustrated how visual sensitivity and perception can be biased by adaptation to the recent temporal context. This contextual modulation has been proposed to serve a variety of functions, but the actual benefits of adaptation remain uncertain. We describe an approach we have recently developed for exploring these benefits by adapting images instead of observers, to simulate how images should appear under theoretically optimal states of adaptation. This allows the long-term consequences of adaptation to be evaluated in ways that are difficult to probe by adapting observers, and provides a common framework for understanding how visual coding changes when the environment or the observer changes, or for evaluating how the effects of temporal context depend on different models of visual coding or the adaptation processes. The approach is illustrated for the specific case of adaptation to color, for which the initial neural coding and adaptation processes are relatively well understood, but can in principle be applied to examine the consequences of adaptation for any stimulus dimension. A simple calibration that adjusts each neuron's sensitivity according to the stimulus level it is exposed to is sufficient to normalize visual coding and generate a host of benefits, from increased efficiency to perceptual constancy to enhanced discrimination. This temporal normalization may also provide an important precursor for the effective operation of contextual mechanisms operating across space or feature dimensions. To the extent that the effects of adaptation can be predicted, images from new environments could be "pre-adapted" to match them to the observer, eliminating the need for observers to adapt.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Webster
- Department of Psychology/296, University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV 89557, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Tregillus K, Webster MA. Dynamics of color contrast adaptation. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2014; 31:A314-A321. [PMID: 24695188 PMCID: PMC3979535 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.31.00a314] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Many forms of color adaptation have been found to reflect both short- and long-term adjustments. We explored the buildup and decay of adaptation to chromatic contrast (temporal modulations of color) for which the dynamics are unknown. A matching task was used to track the perceived contrast of chromatic pulses of varying physical contrast during and after adapting for 1 h to a high contrast modulation repeated over five successive days. The adaptation was characterized by rapid response changes that remained stable in both time course and form across sessions. There was no consistent evidence for long-term plasticity over the time scales we tested.
Collapse
|
25
|
Melin AD, Kline DW, Hickey CM, Fedigan LM. Food search through the eyes of a monkey: a functional substitution approach for assessing the ecology of primate color vision. Vision Res 2013; 86:87-96. [PMID: 23643907 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.04.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2012] [Revised: 04/17/2013] [Accepted: 04/24/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Efficient detection and selection of reddish fruits against green foliage has long been thought to be a major selective pressure favoring the evolution of primate trichromatic color vision. This has recently been questioned by studies of free-ranging primates that fail to show predicted differences in foraging efficiency between dichromats and trichromats. In the present study, we use a unique approach to evaluate the adaptive significance of trichromacy for fruit detection by undertaking a functional substitution model. The color vision phenotypes of neotropical monkeys are simulated for human observers, who use a touch-sensitive computer interface to search for monkey food items in digital images taken under natural conditions. We find an advantage to trichromatic phenotypes - especially the variant with the most spectrally separated visual pigments - for red, yellow and greenish fruits, but not for dark (purple or black) fruits. These results indicate that trichromat advantage is task-specific, and that shape, size and achromatic contrast variation between ripe and unripe fruits cannot completely mitigate the advantage of color vision. Similarities in fruit foraging performance between primates with different phenotypes in the wild likely reflect the behavioral flexibility of dichromats in overcoming a chromatic disadvantage.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A D Melin
- Department of Anthropology, University of Calgary, Canada.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
Visual coding is a highly dynamic process and continuously adapting to the current viewing context. The perceptual changes that result from adaptation to recently viewed stimuli remain a powerful and popular tool for analyzing sensory mechanisms and plasticity. Over the last decade, the footprints of this adaptation have been tracked to both higher and lower levels of the visual pathway and over a wider range of timescales, revealing that visual processing is much more adaptable than previously thought. This work has also revealed that the pattern of aftereffects is similar across many stimulus dimensions, pointing to common coding principles in which adaptation plays a central role. However, why visual coding adapts has yet to be fully answered.
Collapse
|