1
|
Ji H, Chen Z, Qiao Y, Yan J, Chen G, Luo Q, Cui L, Zong Y, Xie Q, Niu CM. Hemodynamic activity is not parsimoniously tuned to index-of-difficulty in movement with dual requirements on speed-accuracy. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 18:1398601. [PMID: 39045507 PMCID: PMC11263286 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1398601] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2024] [Accepted: 06/24/2024] [Indexed: 07/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Background Reaching movements are crucial for daily living and rehabilitation, for which Fitts' Law describes a speed-accuracy trade-off that movement time increases with task difficulty. This study aims to investigate whether cortical activation in motor-related areas is directly linked to task difficulty as defined by Fitts' Law. Understanding this relationship provides a physiological basis for parameter selection in therapeutic exercises. Methods Sixteen healthy subjects performed 2D reaching movements using a rehabilitation robot, with their cortical responses detected using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Task difficulty was manipulated by varying target size and distance, resulting in 3 levels of index-of-difficulty (ID). Kinematic signals were recorded alongside cortical activity to assess the relationship among movement time, task difficulty, and cortical activation. Results Our results showed that movement time increased with ID by 0.2974s/bit across all subjects (conditional r2 = 0.6434, p < 0.0001), and all subjects showed individual trends conforming Fitts' Law (all p < 0.001). Neither activation in BA4 nor in BA6 showed a significant correlation with ID (p > 0.05), while both the target size and distance, as well as the interaction between them, showed a significant relationship with BA4 or BA6 activation (all p < 0.05). Conclusion This study found that although kinematic measures supported Fitts' Law, cortical activity in motor-related areas during reaching movements did not correlate directly with task difficulty as defined by Fitts' Law. Additional factors such as muscle activation may call for different cortical control even when difficulty was identical.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haibiao Ji
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
- School of Medicine, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
| | - Zhi Chen
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
- School of Medicine, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
| | - Yongjun Qiao
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
| | - Jin Yan
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
- School of Medicine, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
| | - Gaoxiang Chen
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
| | - Qi Luo
- School of Automotive and Mechanical Engineering, Changsha University of Science and Technology, Changsha, China
| | - Lijun Cui
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
| | - Ya Zong
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
| | - Qing Xie
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
- School of Medicine, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
| | - Chuanxin M. Niu
- Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Ruijin Hospital, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
- School of Medicine, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Phillips JG, Van Gemmert AWA, Hughes B. Incompatibility Influences Cursor Placement When Pointing to Images of Cups. HUMAN FACTORS 2024; 66:1004-1016. [PMID: 35971754 DOI: 10.1177/00187208221121203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE As images are used within graphical user interfaces to signify menu selection, it is important to understand how image properties can influence cursor placement online. BACKGROUND Objects have multiple dimensions that create potential ambiguity and Stroop-like confusions for the operator if a previously habitual response conflicts with the required response. METHODS To examine the impact of compatibility and other contextual factors, 41 participants used a computer Mouse or touch screen to place the cursor upon images of full or empty cups that varied in size, and direction of handle. RESULTS Cursor placements took longer using the Mouse than touch screen. Although participants were placing the cursor on images, the size of the cup, whether it was empty or full, and the handle orientation interacted in their effects upon response duration and cursor placement. The effects of cup size reversed for empty cups or those with incompatible handles. CONCLUSION Context can influence cursor placement. Perceived spillability influenced precision requirements, and the cup handle can serve as target or a flanking distractor. APPLICATION Image content can influence screen hotspots. As performance can change with cup spillability, this bodes well for attempts to detect intent from cursor trajectories.
Collapse
|
3
|
Phillips JG, Pringle LRD, Hughes B, Van Gemmert A. Cursor movements to targets labelled "stop": a kinematic analysis. ERGONOMICS 2023; 66:1369-1381. [PMID: 36368901 DOI: 10.1080/00140139.2022.2147219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2022] [Accepted: 10/31/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
The label 'Stop' potentially generates conflict-signifying important corrective action, or a warning not to touch. To examine potential conflict between an incongruent label (i.e. STOP) and an imperative command (i.e. MOVE!), 18 participants used a computer mouse to move a crosshair cursor to targets with superimposed labels. Trials systematically varied Imperative (blank or MOVE!), Label (+GO + or STOP) and movement Distance. Kinematic analyses examined response latency, movement duration and accuracy. Incongruent labels had little impact upon response latencies, but they affected cursor deceleration and the variability of cursor placement. Although reading is assumed to be obligatory, the impact of written labels is not immediate, instead affecting cursor deceleration. Indeed, responses to controls labelled STOP were less accurate than those labelled + GO+. As labelled interfaces can create error versus command confusions, enhancing the discriminability of controls to afford more obvious visible cues as to method of use is recommended. Practitioner summary: Emergency stop and shutdown controls can cause response conflict as their labels signify both urgent corrective actions and 'don't touch'. Response conflict caused by confusing superimposed labels is resolved as cursors near the target control and may result in reduced movement accuracy. Prior warnings may influence resolution of response conflict. Abbreviations: Hz: Hertz; M: Mean; ms: millisecond; mm: millimetre; S: second; SD: Standard Deviation; SE: Standard Error; USB: Universal Serial Bus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J G Phillips
- Psychology Department, Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - L R D Pringle
- Psychology Department, Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - B Hughes
- Psychology Department, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - A Van Gemmert
- School of Kinesiology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Rentsch S, Rand MK. Eye-hand coordination during visuomotor adaptation with different rotation angles. PLoS One 2014; 9:e109819. [PMID: 25333942 PMCID: PMC4198129 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109819] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2014] [Accepted: 08/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
This study examined adaptive changes of eye-hand coordination during a visuomotor rotation task. Young adults made aiming movements to targets on a horizontal plane, while looking at the rotated feedback (cursor) of hand movements on a monitor. To vary the task difficulty, three rotation angles (30°, 75°, and 150°) were tested in three groups. All groups shortened hand movement time and trajectory length with practice. However, control strategies used were different among groups. The 30° group used proportionately more implicit adjustments of hand movements than other groups. The 75° group used more on-line feedback control, whereas the 150° group used explicit strategic adjustments. Regarding eye-hand coordination, timing of gaze shift to the target was gradually changed with practice from the late to early phase of hand movements in all groups, indicating an emerging gaze-anchoring behavior. Gaze locations prior to the gaze anchoring were also modified with practice from the cursor vicinity to an area between the starting position and the target. Reflecting various task difficulties, these changes occurred fastest in the 30° group, followed by the 75° group. The 150° group persisted in gazing at the cursor vicinity. These results suggest that the function of gaze control during visuomotor adaptation changes from a reactive control for exploring the relation between cursor and hand movements to a predictive control for guiding the hand to the task goal. That gaze-anchoring behavior emerged in all groups despite various control strategies indicates a generality of this adaptive pattern for eye-hand coordination in goal-directed actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Rentsch
- Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors (IfADo), Dortmund, Germany
| | - Miya K Rand
- Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors (IfADo), Dortmund, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
O'Reilly C, Plamondon R, Lebrun LH. Linking brain stroke risk factors to human movement features for the development of preventive tools. Front Aging Neurosci 2014; 6:150. [PMID: 25071559 PMCID: PMC4085479 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00150] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2013] [Accepted: 06/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper uses human movement analyses to assess the susceptibility of brain stroke, one of the most important causes of disability in elders. To that end, a computerized battery of nine neuromuscular tests has been designed and evaluated with a sample of 120 subjects with or without stoke risk factors. The kinematics of the movements produced was analyzed using a computational neuromuscular model and predictive characteristics were extracted. Logistic regression and linear discriminant analysis with leave-one-out cross-validation was used to infer the probability of presence of brain stroke risk factors. The clinical potential value of movement information for stroke prevention was assessed by computing area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (AUC) for the diagnostic of risk factors based on motion analysis. AUC mostly varying between 0.6 and 0.9 were obtained, depending on the neuromuscular test and the risk factor investigated (obesity, diabetes, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, cigarette smoking, and cardiac disease). Our results support the feasibility of the proposed methodology and its potential application for the development of brain stroke prevention tools. Although further research is needed to improve this methodology and its outcome, results are promising and the proposed approach should be of great interest for many experimenters open to novel approaches in preventive medicine and in gerontology. It should also be valuable for engineers, psychologists, and researchers using human movements for the development of diagnostic and neuromuscular assessment tools.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian O'Reilly
- Laboratoire Scribens, Département de Génie Électrique, École Polytechnique de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada ; Dream and Nightmare Laboratory, Center for Advanced Research in Sleep Medicine, Hôpital du Sacré-Coeur de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada ; Department of Psychiatry, University of Montreal Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Réjean Plamondon
- Laboratoire Scribens, Département de Génie Électrique, École Polytechnique de Montréal Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Louise-Hélène Lebrun
- Département de Neurologie Vasculaire, Centre Hospitalier de l'Université de Montréal, Hôpital Notre-Dame Montréal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
This paper reports the results of a model-based analysis of movements gathered in a 4×4 experimental design of speed/accuracy tradeoffs with variable target distances and width. Our study was performed on a large (120 participants) and varied sample (both genders, wide age range, various health conditions). The delta-lognormal equation was used for data modeling to investigate the interaction between the output of the agonist and the antagonist neuromuscular systems. Empirical observations show that the subjects must correlate more tightly the impulse commands sent to both neuromuscular systems in order to achieve good performances as the difficulty of the task increases whereas the correlation in the timing of the neuromuscular action co-varies with the size of the geometrical properties of the task. These new phenomena are discussed under the paradigm provided by the Kinematic Theory and new research hypotheses are proposed for further investigation of the speed/accuracy tradeoffs.
Collapse
|
7
|
Ifft PJ, Lebedev MA, Nicolelis MAL. Cortical correlates of fitts' law. Front Integr Neurosci 2011; 5:85. [PMID: 22275888 PMCID: PMC3250970 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2011] [Accepted: 12/02/2011] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Fitts’ law describes the fundamental trade-off between movement accuracy and speed: it states that the duration of reaching movements is a function of target size (TS) and distance. While Fitts’ law has been extensively studied in ergonomics and has guided the design of human–computer interfaces, there have been few studies on its neuronal correlates. To elucidate sensorimotor cortical activity underlying Fitts’ law, we implanted two monkeys with multielectrode arrays in the primary motor (M1) and primary somatosensory (S1) cortices. The monkeys performed reaches with a joystick-controlled cursor toward targets of different size. The reaction time (RT), movement time, and movement velocity changed with TS, and M1 and S1 activity reflected these changes. Moreover, modifications of cortical activity could not be explained by changes of movement parameters alone, but required TS as an additional parameter. Neuronal representation of TS was especially prominent during the early RT period where it influenced the slope of the firing rate rise preceding movement initiation. During the movement period, cortical activity was correlated with movement velocity. Neural decoders were applied to simultaneously decode TS and motor parameters from cortical modulations. We suggest that sensorimotor cortex activity reflects the characteristics of both the movement and the target. Classifiers that extract these parameters from cortical ensembles could improve neuroprosthetic control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter J Ifft
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Duke University Durham, NC, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
STEVENSON ROGER, PHILLIPS JAMESG, TRIGGS TOMJ. MOUSE AND DISPLAY TABLETS AS CURSOR CONTROL DEVICES. INT J PATTERN RECOGN 2011. [DOI: 10.1142/s0218001404003733] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Given the proliferation of touch-sensitive screen technologies, the factors contributing to efficiency of cursor control device were examined to better inform choice and design of Graphical User Interfaces. Fitts' law can be used to describe the relative efficiency of these cursor control devices. Experiment One required 18 participants to move a cursor between targets depicted upon a WACOM PL400 display graphics tablet, over various amplitudes, using a computer mouse or stylus, examining the speed and accuracy of cursor placement. The touch sensitive screen allowed faster acquisition of targets than the mouse. Since the mouse has a higher control/display ratio (gain) than the touch sensitive screen, Experiment Two addressed manipulation of the gain settings on computer mouse performance. Low gain offered some accuracy benefits. Possible posture problems associated with the touch sensitive screen were explored in Experiment Three. Screen orientation had little effect upon cursor positioning performance, suggesting the benefits of the touch sensitive screen can be transferred to user-friendly postures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- ROGER STEVENSON
- Psychology Department, Monash University, VIC 3800, Australia
| | | | - TOM J. TRIGGS
- Psychology Department, Monash University, VIC 3800, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
|
10
|
Ketcham CJ, Rodriguez TM, Zihlman KA. Targeted Aiming Movements Are Compromised in Nonaffected Limb of Persons With Stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2007; 21:388-97. [PMID: 17369510 DOI: 10.1177/1545968306297872] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Background. Research has shown that movement impairments following stroke are typically associated with the limb contralateral to the side of the stroke. Prior studies identified ipsilateral motor declines across a variety of tasks. Objective. Two experiments were conducted to better understand the ipsilateral contributions to organization and execution of proximal upper extremity multisegment aiming movements in persons with right-hemispheric stroke. Methods. Participants performed reciprocal aiming (Experiment 1) and 2-segment aiming movements (Experiment 2) on a digitizing tablet. In both experiments, target size and/or target orientation were manipulated to examine the influence of accuracy constraints on the planning and organization of movements. Results. Kinematic measures, submovement analysis, and harmonicity measures were included in this study. Declines in organization and execution of multisegment movements were found to contribute to performance decrements and slowing in stroke patients. Furthermore, stroke patients were unable to efficiently plan multisegment movements as one functional unit, resulting in discrete movements. Conclusions . Results suggest the importance of considering ipsilateral contributions to the control and organization of targeted aiming movements as well as implications for rehabilitation and recovery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline J Ketcham
- Motor Control Laboratory, Department of Health and Kinesiology, Texas A&M University, College Station, 77843-4243, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Brindle TJ, Nitz AJ, Uhl TL, Kifer E, Shapiro R. Kinematic and EMG characteristics of simple shoulder movements with proprioception and visual feedback. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 2006; 16:236-49. [PMID: 16111896 DOI: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2005.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2005] [Revised: 06/23/2005] [Accepted: 06/27/2005] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
The objective of this study was to determine if simple, shoulder movements use the dual control hypothesis strategy, previously demonstrated with elbow movements, and to see if this strategy also applies in the absence of visual feedback. Twenty subjects were seated with their right arm abducted to 90 degrees and externally rotated in the scapular plane. Subjects internally rotated to a target position using a custom shoulder wheel at three different speeds with and without visual feedback. Kinematics were collected with a motion analysis system and electromyographic (EMG) recordings of the pectoralis major (PECT), infraspinatus (INFRA), anterior and posterior (ADELT, PDELT) deltoid muscles were used to evaluate muscle activity patterns during movements. Kinematics changed as movement speed increased with less accuracy (p<0.01). Greater EMG activity was observed in the PECT, PDELT, and INFRA with shorter durations for the ADELT, PDELT and INFRA. Movements with only kinesthetic feedback were less accurate (p<0.01) and performed faster (p<0.01) than movements with visual feedback. EMG activity suggests no major difference in CNS control strategies in movements with and without visual feedback. Greater resolution with visual feedback enables the implementation of a dual control strategy, allowing greater movement velocity while maintaining accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Timothy J Brindle
- Physical Disabilities Branch, National Institutes of Health, Building 10-CRC, Room 1-1425 MCS 1604, Bethesda, MD 20892-1604, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Fimbel EJ, Lemay M, Arguin M. Speed-accuracy trade-offs in myocontrol. Hum Mov Sci 2006; 25:165-80. [PMID: 16460824 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.12.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2005] [Revised: 09/23/2005] [Accepted: 12/03/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Myoelectric (EMG) signals are used in assistive technology for prostheses, computer and domestic control. However, little is known about the capacity of controlling these signals. Specifically, it is unclear whether myocontrol, i.e., the control of myoelectric signals, obeys the same laws as motor control. Neurologically intact adult participants performed pointing tasks with EMG signals captured from the forehead or the hand in two modalities (sustained: stabilize the signal amplitude in the target; impulsion: produce an impulse and return to resting level). In the sustained modality, the time to reach the target (reach time) increased logarithmically with target amplitude, which is compatible with the predictions of Fitts' law. The rate of failure was not significantly affected by target amplitude. In the impulsion modality, the reach time and the rate of failure followed a bow-shaped pattern as a function of target amplitude. Stabilization time in the sustained modality followed a convex (bow-shaped) pattern for the forehead and a concave pattern for the hand. This was the only significant effect of electrode placement in this study. These findings suggest that myocontrol obeys laws that are distinct from those determining motor control, and that the muscular and intra-muscular synergies that produce EMG signals are specific of each pointing modality and target amplitude.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eric J Fimbel
- Electrical Engineering Department, Ecole de technologie supérieure, 1100 Notre Dame West, Montréal, Qué., Canada H3C 1K3.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Trommershäuser J, Gepshtein S, Maloney LT, Landy MS, Banks MS. Optimal compensation for changes in task-relevant movement variability. J Neurosci 2006; 25:7169-78. [PMID: 16079399 PMCID: PMC6725228 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1906-05.2005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 119] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Effective movement planning should take into account the consequences of possible errors in executing a planned movement. These errors can result from either sensory uncertainty or variability in movement planning and production. We examined the ability of humans to compensate for variability in sensory estimation and movement production under conditions in which variability is increased artificially by the experimenter. Subjects rapidly pointed at a target region that had an adjacent penalty region. Target and penalty hits yielded monetary rewards and losses. We manipulated the task-relevant variability by perturbing visual feedback of finger position during the movement. The feedback was shifted in a random direction with a random amplitude in each trial, causing an increase in the task-relevant variability. Subjects were unable to counteract this form of perturbation. Rewards and penalties were based on the perturbed, visually specified finger position. Subjects rapidly acquired an estimate of their new variability in <120 trials and adjusted their aim points accordingly. We compared subjects' performance to the performance of an optimal movement planner maximizing expected gain. Their performance was consistent with that expected from an optimal movement planner that perfectly compensated for externally imposed changes in task-relevant variability. When exposed to novel stimulus configurations, aim points shifted in the first trial without showing any detectable trend across trials. These results indicate that subjects are capable of changing their pointing strategy in the presence of externally imposed noise. Furthermore, they manage to update their estimate of task-relevant variability and to transfer this estimate to novel stimulus configurations.
Collapse
|