1
|
Hessels RS, Nuthmann A, Nyström M, Andersson R, Niehorster DC, Hooge ITC. The fundamentals of eye tracking part 1: The link between theory and research question. Behav Res Methods 2024; 57:16. [PMID: 39668288 PMCID: PMC11638287 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02544-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/13/2024] [Indexed: 12/14/2024]
Abstract
Eye tracking technology has become increasingly prevalent in scientific research, offering unique insights into oculomotor and cognitive processes. The present article explores the relationship between scientific theory, the research question, and the use of eye-tracking technology. It aims to guide readers in determining if eye tracking is suitable for their studies and how to formulate relevant research questions. Examples from research on oculomotor control, reading, scene perception, task execution, visual expertise, and instructional design are used to illustrate the connection between theory and eye-tracking data. These examples may serve as inspiration to researchers new to eye tracking. In summarizing the examples, three important considerations emerge: (1) whether the study focuses on describing eye movements or uses them as a proxy for e.g., perceptual, or cognitive processes, (2) the logical chain from theory to predictions, and (3) whether the study is of an observational or idea-testing nature. We provide a generic scheme and a set of specific questions that may help researchers formulate and explicate their research question using eye tracking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roy S Hessels
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | | - Marcus Nyström
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | | | - Diederick C Niehorster
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
- Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Ignace T C Hooge
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Hooge ITC, Niehorster DC, Nyström M, Hessels RS. Large eye-head gaze shifts measured with a wearable eye tracker and an industrial camera. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:5820-5833. [PMID: 38200239 PMCID: PMC11335818 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02316-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/04/2023] [Indexed: 01/12/2024]
Abstract
We built a novel setup to record large gaze shifts (up to 140∘ ). The setup consists of a wearable eye tracker and a high-speed camera with fiducial marker technology to track the head. We tested our setup by replicating findings from the classic eye-head gaze shift literature. We conclude that our new inexpensive setup is good enough to investigate the dynamics of large eye-head gaze shifts. This novel setup could be used for future research on large eye-head gaze shifts, but also for research on gaze during e.g., human interaction. We further discuss reference frames and terminology in head-free eye tracking. Despite a transition from head-fixed eye tracking to head-free gaze tracking, researchers still use head-fixed eye movement terminology when discussing world-fixed gaze phenomena. We propose to use more specific terminology for world-fixed phenomena, including gaze fixation, gaze pursuit, and gaze saccade.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ignace T C Hooge
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Diederick C Niehorster
- Lund University Humanities Lab and Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Marcus Nyström
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Roy S Hessels
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Dunn MJ, Alexander RG, Amiebenomo OM, Arblaster G, Atan D, Erichsen JT, Ettinger U, Giardini ME, Gilchrist ID, Hamilton R, Hessels RS, Hodgins S, Hooge ITC, Jackson BS, Lee H, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S, Mcilreavy L, Muratori LM, Niehorster DC, Nyström M, Otero-Millan J, Schlüssel MM, Self JE, Singh T, Smyrnis N, Sprenger A. Minimal reporting guideline for research involving eye tracking (2023 edition). Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:4351-4357. [PMID: 37507649 PMCID: PMC11225961 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02187-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/28/2023] [Indexed: 07/30/2023]
Abstract
A guideline is proposed that comprises the minimum items to be reported in research studies involving an eye tracker and human or non-human primate participant(s). This guideline was developed over a 3-year period using a consensus-based process via an open invitation to the international eye tracking community. This guideline will be reviewed at maximum intervals of 4 years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matt J Dunn
- School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK.
| | - Robert G Alexander
- Departments of Ophthalmology, Neurology, and Physiology/Pharmacology, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
| | | | - Gemma Arblaster
- Health Sciences School, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
- Orthoptic Department, Sheffield Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Sheffield, UK
| | - Denize Atan
- Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | | | | | - Mario E Giardini
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK
| | - Iain D Gilchrist
- School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | - Ruth Hamilton
- Department of Clinical Physics & Bioengineering, Royal Hospital for Children, NHS Greater Glasgow & Clyde, Glasgow, UK
- College of Medical, Veterinary & Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
| | - Roy S Hessels
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | | | - Ignace T C Hooge
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Brooke S Jackson
- Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
| | - Helena Lee
- Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
| | - Stephen L Macknik
- Departments of Ophthalmology, Neurology, and Physiology/Pharmacology, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
| | - Susana Martinez-Conde
- Departments of Ophthalmology, Neurology, and Physiology/Pharmacology, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
| | - Lee Mcilreavy
- School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK
| | - Lisa M Muratori
- Department of Physical Therapy, School of Health Professions, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Diederick C Niehorster
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
- Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Marcus Nyström
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Jorge Otero-Millan
- Herbert Wertheim School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Michael M Schlüssel
- UK EQUATOR Centre, Centre for Statistics in Medicine (CSM), Nuffield Department of Orthopaedics, Rheumatology and Musculoskeletal Sciences (NDORMS), University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Jay E Self
- Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
| | - Tarkeshwar Singh
- Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| | - Nikolaos Smyrnis
- 2nd Department of Psychiatry, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Medical School, General University Hospital Attikon, Athens, Greece
| | - Andreas Sprenger
- Department of Neurology and Institute of Psychology II, Center of Brain, Behavior and Metabolism (CBBM), University of Luebeck, Luebeck, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ghiani A, Amelink D, Brenner E, Hooge ITC, Hessels RS. When knowing the activity is not enough to predict gaze. J Vis 2024; 24:6. [PMID: 38984899 PMCID: PMC11238878 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2023] [Accepted: 05/31/2024] [Indexed: 07/11/2024] Open
Abstract
It is reasonable to assume that where people look in the world is largely determined by what they are doing. The reasoning is that the activity determines where it is useful to look at each moment in time. Assuming that it is vital to accurately judge the positions of the steps when navigating a staircase, it is surprising that people differ a lot in the extent to which they look at the steps. Apparently, some people consider the accuracy of peripheral vision, predictability of the step size, and feeling the edges of the steps with their feet to be good enough. If so, occluding part of the view of the staircase and making it more important to place one's feet gently might make it more beneficial to look directly at the steps before stepping onto them, so that people will more consistently look at many steps. We tested this idea by asking people to walk on staircases, either with or without a tray with two cups of water on it. When carrying the tray, people walked more slowly, but they shifted their gaze across steps in much the same way as they did when walking without the tray. They did not look at more steps. There was a clear positive correlation between the fraction of steps that people looked at when walking with and without the tray. Thus, the variability in the extent to which people look at the steps persists when one makes walking on the staircase more challenging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Ghiani
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Daan Amelink
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Eli Brenner
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Ignace T C Hooge
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Roy S Hessels
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Li HH, Sprague TC, Yoo AH, Ma WJ, Curtis CE. Neural mechanisms of resource allocation in working memory. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.05.11.593695. [PMID: 38766258 PMCID: PMC11100829 DOI: 10.1101/2024.05.11.593695] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2024]
Abstract
To mitigate capacity limits of working memory, people allocate resources according to an item's relevance. However, the neural mechanisms supporting such a critical operation remain unknown. Here, we developed computational neuroimaging methods to decode and demix neural responses associated with multiple items in working memory with different priorities. In striate and extrastriate cortex, the gain of neural responses tracked the priority of memoranda. Higher-priority memoranda were decoded with smaller error and lower uncertainty. Moreover, these neural differences predicted behavioral differences in memory prioritization. Remarkably, trialwise variability in the magnitude of delay activity in frontal cortex predicted differences in decoded precision between low and high-priority items in visual cortex. These results suggest a model in which feedback signals broadcast from frontal cortex sculpt the gain of memory representations in visual cortex according to behavioral relevance, thus, identifying a neural mechanism for resource allocation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hsin-Hung Li
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43201, USA
- These authors contributed equally
| | - Thomas C Sprague
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA
- These authors contributed equally
| | - Aspen H Yoo
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Wei Ji Ma
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Clayton E Curtis
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Aizenman AM, Gegenfurtner KR, Goettker A. Oculomotor routines for perceptual judgments. J Vis 2024; 24:3. [PMID: 38709511 PMCID: PMC11078167 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.5.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2023] [Accepted: 03/09/2024] [Indexed: 05/07/2024] Open
Abstract
In everyday life we frequently make simple visual judgments about object properties, for example, how big or wide is a certain object? Our goal is to test whether there are also task-specific oculomotor routines that support perceptual judgments, similar to the well-established exploratory routines for haptic perception. In a first study, observers saw different scenes with two objects presented in a photorealistic virtual reality environment. Observers were asked to judge which of two objects was taller or wider while gaze was tracked. All tasks were performed with the same set of virtual objects in the same scenes, so that we can compare spatial characteristics of exploratory gaze behavior to quantify oculomotor routines for each task. Width judgments showed fixations around the center of the objects with larger horizontal spread. In contrast, for height judgments, gaze was shifted toward the top of the objects with larger vertical spread. These results suggest specific strategies in gaze behavior that presumably are used for perceptual judgments. To test the causal link between oculomotor behavior and perception, in a second study, observers could freely gaze at the object or we introduced a gaze-contingent setup forcing observers to fixate specific positions on the object. Discrimination performance was similar between free-gaze and the gaze-contingent conditions for width and height judgments. These results suggest that although gaze is adapted for different tasks, performance seems to be based on a perceptual strategy, independent of potential cues that can be provided by the oculomotor system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Avi M Aizenman
- Psychology Department Giessen University, Giessen, Germany
- http://aviaizenman.com/
| | - Karl R Gegenfurtner
- Psychology Department Giessen University, Giessen, Germany
- https://www.allpsych.uni-giessen.de/karl/
| | - Alexander Goettker
- Psychology Department Giessen University, Giessen, Germany
- https://alexgoettker.com/
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Ghiani A, Mann D, Brenner E. Methods matter: Exploring how expectations influence common actions. iScience 2024; 27:109076. [PMID: 38361615 PMCID: PMC10867666 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.109076] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2023] [Revised: 12/21/2023] [Accepted: 01/26/2024] [Indexed: 02/17/2024] Open
Abstract
Behavior in controlled laboratory studies is not always representative of what people do in daily life. This has prompted a recent shift toward conducting studies in natural settings. We wondered whether expectations raised by how the task is presented should also be considered. To find out, we studied gaze when walking down and up a staircase. Gaze was often directed at steps before stepping on them, but most participants did not look at every step. Importantly, participants fixated more steps and looked around less when asked to navigate the staircase than when navigating the same staircase but asked to walk outside. Presumably, expecting the staircase to be important made participants direct their gaze at more steps, despite the identical requirements when on the staircase. This illustrates that behavior can be influenced by expectations, such as expectations resulting from task instructions, even when studies are conducted in natural settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Ghiani
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Sciences and Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - David Mann
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Sciences and Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Eli Brenner
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Amsterdam Movement Sciences and Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Hooge ITC, Niehorster DC, Hessels RS, Benjamins JS, Nyström M. How robust are wearable eye trackers to slow and fast head and body movements? Behav Res Methods 2023; 55:4128-4142. [PMID: 36326998 PMCID: PMC10700439 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-022-02010-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/11/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
How well can modern wearable eye trackers cope with head and body movement? To investigate this question, we asked four participants to stand still, walk, skip, and jump while fixating a static physical target in space. We did this for six different eye trackers. All the eye trackers were capable of recording gaze during the most dynamic episodes (skipping and jumping). The accuracy became worse as movement got wilder. During skipping and jumping, the biggest error was 5.8∘. However, most errors were smaller than 3∘. We discuss the implications of decreased accuracy in the context of different research scenarios.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ignace T C Hooge
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Diederick C Niehorster
- Lund University Humanities Lab and Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Roy S Hessels
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Jeroen S Benjamins
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, and Social, Health and Organisational Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Marcus Nyström
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Pedziwiatr MA, Heer S, Coutrot A, Bex PJ, Mareschal I. Influence of prior knowledge on eye movements to scenes as revealed by hidden Markov models. J Vis 2023; 23:10. [PMID: 37721772 PMCID: PMC10511023 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.10.10] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2023] [Accepted: 08/14/2023] [Indexed: 09/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Human visual experience usually provides ample opportunity to accumulate knowledge about events unfolding in the environment. In typical scene perception experiments, however, participants view images that are unrelated to each other and, therefore, they cannot accumulate knowledge relevant to the upcoming visual input. Consequently, the influence of such knowledge on how this input is processed remains underexplored. Here, we investigated this influence in the context of gaze control. We used sequences of static film frames arranged in a way that allowed us to compare eye movements to identical frames between two groups: a group that accumulated prior knowledge relevant to the situations depicted in these frames and a group that did not. We used a machine learning approach based on hidden Markov models fitted to individual scanpaths to demonstrate that the gaze patterns from the two groups differed systematically and, thereby, showed that recently accumulated prior knowledge contributes to gaze control. Next, we leveraged the interpretability of hidden Markov models to characterize these differences. Additionally, we report two unexpected and interesting caveats of our approach. Overall, our results highlight the importance of recently acquired prior knowledge for oculomotor control and the potential of hidden Markov models as a tool for investigating it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marek A Pedziwiatr
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
| | - Sophie Heer
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
| | - Antoine Coutrot
- Univ Lyon, CNRS, INSA Lyon, UCBL, LIRIS, UMR5205, F-69621 Lyon, France
| | - Peter J Bex
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Isabelle Mareschal
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Goettker A, Borgerding N, Leeske L, Gegenfurtner KR. Cues for predictive eye movements in naturalistic scenes. J Vis 2023; 23:12. [PMID: 37728915 PMCID: PMC10516764 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.10.12] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2023] [Accepted: 08/23/2023] [Indexed: 09/22/2023] Open
Abstract
We previously compared following of the same trajectories with eye movements, but either as an isolated targets or embedded in a naturalistic scene-in this case, the movement of a puck in an ice hockey game. We observed that the oculomotor system was able to leverage the contextual cues available in the naturalistic scene to produce predictive eye movements. In this study, we wanted to assess which factors are critical for achieving this predictive advantage by manipulating four factors: the expertise of the viewers, the amount of available peripheral information, and positional and kinematic cues. The more peripheral information became available (by manipulating the area of the video that was visible), the better the predictions of all observers. However, expert ice hockey fans were consistently better at predicting than novices and used peripheral information more effectively for predictive saccades. Artificial cues about player positions did not lead to a predictive advantage, whereas impairing the causal structure of kinematic cues by playing the video in reverse led to a severe impairment. When videos were flipped vertically to introduce more difficult kinematic cues, predictive behavior was comparable to watching the original videos. Together, these results demonstrate that, when contextual information is available in naturalistic scenes, the oculomotor system is successfully integrating them and is not relying only on low-level information about the target trajectory. Critical factors for successful prediction seem to be the amount of available information, experience with the stimuli, and the availability of intact kinematic cues for player movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Goettker
- Justus Liebig Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University, Giessen, Germany
| | | | - Linus Leeske
- Justus Liebig Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
| | - Karl R Gegenfurtner
- Justus Liebig Universität Giessen, Giessen, Germany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg and Justus Liebig University, Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Hoogerbrugge AJ, Strauch C, Nijboer TCW, Van der Stigchel S. Don't hide the instruction manual: A dynamic trade-off between using internal and external templates during visual search. J Vis 2023; 23:14. [PMID: 37486300 PMCID: PMC10382786 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.7.14] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/25/2023] Open
Abstract
Visual search is typically studied by requiring participants to memorize a template initially, for which they subsequently search in a crowded display. Search in daily life, however, often involves templates that remain accessible externally, and may therefore be (re)attended for just-in-time encoding or to refresh internal template representations. Here, we show that participants indeed use external templates during search when given the chance. This behavior was observed during both simple and complex search, scaled with task difficulty, and was associated with improved performance. Furthermore, we show that participants used external sampling not only to offload memory, but also as a means of verifying whether the template was remembered correctly at the end of trials. We conclude that the external world may not only provide the challenge (e.g., distractors), but may dynamically ease search. These results argue for extensions of state-of-the-art models of search, because external sampling seems to be used frequently, in at least two ways and is actually beneficial for task performance. Our findings support a model of visual working memory that emphasizes a resource-efficient trade-off between storing and (re)attending external information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex J Hoogerbrugge
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
| | - Christoph Strauch
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
| | - Tanja C W Nijboer
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
- Center of Excellence for Rehabilitation Medicine, UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, The Netherlands
- De Hoogstraat Rehabilitation, Department of Rehabilitation, Physical Therapy Science & Sports, UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Ghiani A, Van Hout LR, Driessen JG, Brenner E. Where do people look when walking up and down familiar staircases? J Vis 2023; 23:7. [PMID: 36633872 PMCID: PMC9840440 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.1.7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Many activities in daily life do not impose strict requirements on gaze. We investigated gaze when walking up and down staircases within one's own house. We anticipated that using a variety of staircases in different environments and not informing participants that stair climbing was the focus of investigation might provide a description of gaze behavior that is closer to that used in our daily life than doing so under circumstances in which the focus is explicitly and exclusively directed at the stairs. We analyzed several measures, including the order in which participants fixated the steps. We confirmed that people often look at the steps sequentially, but found that they often made fixations back to steps they had already fixated. They also regularly skipped looking at several steps to fixate further ahead. On average, they directed their gaze at about half the steps. They looked further ahead when ascending than when descending staircases. Overall, the results are similar to those found under highly constrained laboratory conditions, although we do report some differences. One such difference is a tendency to fixate fewer steps. Another is that participants fixated steps that were less far ahead when descending staircases. We also introduced some new analyses that may help understand gaze behavior during stair climbing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Ghiani
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, The Netherlands.,
| | - Liz R. Van Hout
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, The Netherlands,
| | - Joost G. Driessen
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, The Netherlands,
| | - Eli Brenner
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, The Netherlands.,
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Rehrig G, Barker M, Peacock CE, Hayes TR, Henderson JM, Ferreira F. Look at what I can do: Object affordances guide visual attention while speakers describe potential actions. Atten Percept Psychophys 2022; 84:1583-1610. [PMID: 35484443 PMCID: PMC9246959 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-022-02467-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
As we act on the world around us, our eyes seek out objects we plan to interact with. A growing body of evidence suggests that overt visual attention selects objects in the environment that could be interacted with, even when the task precludes physical interaction. In previous work, objects that afford grasping interactions influenced attention when static scenes depicted reachable spaces, and attention was otherwise better explained by general informativeness. Because grasping is but one of many object interactions, previous work may have downplayed the influence of object affordances on attention. The current study investigated the relationship between overt visual attention and object affordances versus broadly construed semantic information in scenes as speakers describe or memorize scenes. In addition to meaning and grasp maps-which capture informativeness and grasping object affordances in scenes, respectively-we introduce interact maps, which capture affordances more broadly. In a mixed-effects analysis of 5 eyetracking experiments, we found that meaning predicted fixated locations in a general description task and during scene memorization. Grasp maps marginally predicted fixated locations during action description for scenes that depicted reachable spaces only. Interact maps predicted fixated regions in description experiments alone. Our findings suggest observers allocate attention to scene regions that could be readily interacted with when talking about the scene, while general informativeness preferentially guides attention when the task does not encourage careful consideration of objects in the scene. The current study suggests that the influence of object affordances on visual attention in scenes is mediated by task demands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gwendolyn Rehrig
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, 95616, USA.
| | - Madison Barker
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, 95616, USA
| | - Candace E Peacock
- Department of Psychology and Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Taylor R Hayes
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - John M Henderson
- Department of Psychology and Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Fernanda Ferreira
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, 95616, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Lappi O. Gaze Strategies in Driving-An Ecological Approach. Front Psychol 2022; 13:821440. [PMID: 35360580 PMCID: PMC8964278 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.821440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2021] [Accepted: 02/07/2022] [Indexed: 01/16/2023] Open
Abstract
Human performance in natural environments is deeply impressive, and still much beyond current AI. Experimental techniques, such as eye tracking, may be useful to understand the cognitive basis of this performance, and "the human advantage." Driving is domain where these techniques may deployed, in tasks ranging from rigorously controlled laboratory settings through high-fidelity simulations to naturalistic experiments in the wild. This research has revealed robust patterns that can be reliably identified and replicated in the field and reproduced in the lab. The purpose of this review is to cover the basics of what is known about these gaze behaviors, and some of their implications for understanding visually guided steering. The phenomena reviewed will be of interest to those working on any domain where visual guidance and control with similar task demands is involved (e.g., many sports). The paper is intended to be accessible to the non-specialist, without oversimplifying the complexity of real-world visual behavior. The literature reviewed will provide an information base useful for researchers working on oculomotor behaviors and physiology in the lab who wish to extend their research into more naturalistic locomotor tasks, or researchers in more applied fields (sports, transportation) who wish to bring aspects of the real-world ecology under experimental scrutiny. Part of a Research Topic on Gaze Strategies in Closed Self-paced tasks, this aspect of the driving task is discussed. It is in particular emphasized why it is important to carefully separate the visual strategies driving (quite closed and self-paced) from visual behaviors relevant to other forms of driver behavior (an open-ended menagerie of behaviors). There is always a balance to strike between ecological complexity and experimental control. One way to reconcile these demands is to look for natural, real-world tasks and behavior that are rich enough to be interesting yet sufficiently constrained and well-understood to be replicated in simulators and the lab. This ecological approach to driving as a model behavior and the way the connection between "lab" and "real world" can be spanned in this research is of interest to anyone keen to develop more ecologically representative designs for studying human gaze behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Otto Lappi
- Cognitive Science/TRU, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Goettker A, Gegenfurtner KR. A change in perspective: The interaction of saccadic and pursuit eye movements in oculomotor control and perception. Vision Res 2021; 188:283-296. [PMID: 34489101 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.08.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2021] [Revised: 07/26/2021] [Accepted: 08/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Due to the close relationship between oculomotor behavior and visual processing, eye movements have been studied in many different areas of research over the last few decades. While these studies have brought interesting insights, specialization within each research area comes at the potential cost of a narrow and isolated view of the oculomotor system. In this review, we want to expand this perspective by looking at the interactions between the two most important types of voluntary eye movements: saccades and pursuit. Recent evidence indicates multiple interactions and shared signals at the behavioral and neurophysiological level for oculomotor control and for visual perception during pursuit and saccades. Oculomotor control seems to be based on shared position- and velocity-related information, which leads to multiple behavioral interactions and synergies. The distinction between position- and velocity-related information seems to be also present at the neurophysiological level. In addition, visual perception seems to be based on shared efferent signals about upcoming eye positions and velocities, which are to some degree independent of the actual oculomotor response. This review suggests an interactive perspective on the oculomotor system, based mainly on different types of sensory input, and less so on separate subsystems for saccadic or pursuit eye movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Goettker
- Abteilung Allgemeine Psychologie and Center for Mind, Brain & Behavior, Justus-Liebig University Giessen, Germany.
| | - Karl R Gegenfurtner
- Abteilung Allgemeine Psychologie and Center for Mind, Brain & Behavior, Justus-Liebig University Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|