1
|
Alhaider R, Mahon M, Donlan C. The influence of language on the formation of number concepts: Evidence from preschool children who are bilingual in English and Arabic. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 246:105988. [PMID: 38901325 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105988] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2023] [Revised: 05/07/2024] [Accepted: 05/08/2024] [Indexed: 06/22/2024]
Abstract
We asked whether grammatical number marking has specific influence on the formation of early number concepts. In particular, does comprehension of dual case marking support young children's understanding of cardinality? We assessed number knowledge in 77 3-year-old Arabic-English bilingual children using the Give-a-Number task in both languages. Given recent concerns around the administration and scoring of the Give-a-Number task, we used two complementary approaches: one based on conceptual levels and the other based on overall test scores. We also tested comprehension of dual case marking in Arabic and number sequence knowledge in both languages. Regression analyses showed that dual case comprehension exerts a strong influence on cardinality tested in Arabic independent of age, general language skills, and number sequence knowledge. No such influence was found for cardinality tested in English, indicating a language-specific effect. Further analyses tested for transfer of cardinality knowledge between languages. These revealed, in addition to the findings outlined above, a powerful cross-linguistic transfer effect. Our findings are consistent with a model in which the direct effect of dual case marking is language specific, but concepts, once acquired, may be represented abstractly and transferred between languages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rima Alhaider
- Department of Language and Cognition, University College London, London WC1N 1PF, UK.
| | - Merle Mahon
- Department of Language and Cognition, University College London, London WC1N 1PF, UK
| | - Chris Donlan
- Department of Language and Cognition, University College London, London WC1N 1PF, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
James‐Brabham E, Loveridge T, Sella F, Wakeling P, Carroll DJ, Blakey E. How do socioeconomic attainment gaps in early mathematical ability arise? Child Dev 2023; 94:1550-1565. [PMID: 37248732 PMCID: PMC10953023 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13947] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/22/2021] [Revised: 02/24/2023] [Accepted: 03/21/2023] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Socioeconomic attainment gaps in mathematical ability are evident before children begin school, and widen over time. Little is known about why early attainment gaps emerge. Two cross-sectional correlational studies were conducted in 2018-2019 with socioeconomically diverse preschoolers, to explore four factors that might explain why attainment gaps arise: working memory, inhibitory control, verbal ability, and frequency of home mathematical activities (N = 304, 54% female; 84% White, 10% Asian, 1% black African, 1% Kurdish, 4% mixed ethnicity). Inhibitory control and verbal ability emerged as indirect factors in the relation between socioeconomic status and mathematical ability, but neither working memory nor home activities did. We discuss the implications this has for future research to understand, and work towards narrowing attainment gaps.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Toni Loveridge
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of SheffieldSheffieldUK
| | - Francesco Sella
- Centre for Mathematical Cognition and Centre for Early Mathematics Learning, Department of Mathematics EducationLoughborough UniversityLoughboroughUK
| | | | | | - Emma Blakey
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of SheffieldSheffieldUK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Gerbrand A, Gredebäck G, Lindskog M. Recognition of small numbers in subset knowers Cardinal knowledge in early childhood. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2023; 10:230474. [PMID: 37885983 PMCID: PMC10598441 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.230474] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2023] [Accepted: 10/06/2023] [Indexed: 10/28/2023]
Abstract
Previous research suggests that subset-knowers have an approximate understanding of small numbers. However, it is still unclear exactly what subset-knowers understand about small numbers. To investigate this further, we tested 133 participants, ages 2.6-4 years, on a newly developed eye-tracking task targeting cardinal recognition. Participants were presented with two sets differing in cardinality (1-4 items) and asked to find a specific cardinality. Our main finding showed that on a group level, subset-knowers could identify all presented targets at rates above chance, further supporting that subset-knowers understand several of the basic principles of small numbers. Exploratory analyses tentatively suggest that 1-knowers could identify the targets 1 and 2, but struggled when the target was 3 and 4, whereas 2-knowers and above could identify all targets at rates above chance. This might tentatively suggest that subset-knowers have an approximate understanding of numbers that is just (i.e. +1) above their current knower level. We discuss the implications of these results at length.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anton Gerbrand
- Uppsala Child and Babylab, Uppsala Universitet, Department of psychology, Sweden
| | - Gustaf Gredebäck
- Uppsala Child and Babylab, Uppsala Universitet, Department of psychology, Sweden
| | - Marcus Lindskog
- Uppsala Child and Babylab, Uppsala Universitet, Department of psychology, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Gilmore C. Understanding the complexities of mathematical cognition: A multi-level framework. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2023; 76:1953-1972. [PMID: 37129432 PMCID: PMC10466984 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231175325] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2022] [Revised: 01/16/2023] [Accepted: 01/17/2023] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
Mathematics skills are associated with future employment, well-being, and quality of life. However, many adults and children fail to learn the mathematics skills they require. To improve this situation, we need to have a better understanding of the processes of learning and performing mathematics. Over the past two decades, there has been a substantial growth in psychological research focusing on mathematics. However, to make further progress, we need to pay greater attention to the nature of, and multiple elements involved in, mathematical cognition. Mathematics is not a single construct; rather, overall mathematics achievement is comprised of proficiency with specific components of mathematics (e.g., number fact knowledge, algebraic thinking), which in turn recruit basic mathematical processes (e.g., magnitude comparison, pattern recognition). General cognitive skills and different learning experiences influence the development of each component of mathematics as well as the links between them. Here, I propose and provide evidence for a framework that structures how these components of mathematics fit together. This framework allows us to make sense of the proliferation of empirical findings concerning influences on mathematical cognition and can guide the questions we ask, identifying where we are missing both research evidence and models of specific mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camilla Gilmore
- Centre for Mathematical Cognition, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kwon N, Kim SY. Effects of intelligence and approximate number system on the non-symbolic division ability in preschoolers. Front Psychol 2023; 14:961140. [PMID: 37425165 PMCID: PMC10328115 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.961140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2022] [Accepted: 05/29/2023] [Indexed: 07/11/2023] Open
Abstract
Recently, it has become evident that cognitive abilities such as the approximate number system (ANS), number knowledge, and intelligence affect individuals' fundamental mathematical ability. However, it is unclear which of these cognitive abilities have the greatest impact on the non-symbolic division ability in preschoolers. Therefore, in the present study, we included 4- to 6-year-old Korean preschoolers without prior formal education of division in order to test their ability to solve non-symbolic division problems, ANS acuity, and intelligence, and to determine the interrelationships among those functions (N = 38). We used the Panamath Dot Comparison Paradigm to measure the ANS acuity, employed non-symbolic division tasks to measure the ability to solve non-symbolic division problems, and measured the intelligence using the Korean version of the WPPSI-IV (Wechsler Preschool Primary Scale of Intelligence-IV). Our results showed that, in all conditions of the non-symbolic division tasks, the 4- to 6-years old children were able to perform better than chance level. Additionally, in a relatively easy condition, the children's performance showed a significant positive correlation with full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) and ANS acuity; however, in a more complex condition, only FSIQ was significantly correlated with their performance. Overall, we found significant relationships between the children's performance in the non-symbolic division tasks and verbal comprehension, fluid reasoning, and processing speed index. Taken together, our findings demonstrate that preschoolers without formal education on the arithmetic problem solving can solve non-symbolic division problems. Moreover, we suggest that both FSIQ and ANS ability play essential roles in children's ability to solve non-symbolic division problems, highlighting the significance of intelligence on children's fundamental mathematical ability.
Collapse
|
6
|
O'Rear CD, Seip I, Azar J, Baroody AJ, McNeil NM. Features in children's counting books that lead dyads to both count and label sets during shared book reading. Child Dev 2023. [PMID: 36970843 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/18/2023]
Abstract
This study examined how book features influence talk during shared book reading. We used data from a study in which parent-child dyads (n = 157; child's Mage = 43.99 months; 88 girls, 69 boys; 91.72% of parents self-reported as white) were randomly assigned to read two number books. The focus was comparison talk (i.e., talk in which dyads count a set and also label its total), as this type of talk has been shown to promote children's understanding of cardinality. Replicating previous findings, dyads produced relatively low levels of comparison talk. However, book features influenced the talk. Books containing a greater number of numerical representations (e.g., number word, numeral, and non-symbolic set) and a greater word count elicited more comparison talk.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Connor D O'Rear
- Department of Human Development and Family Science, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, USA
| | - Isabella Seip
- Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana, USA
| | - Joanna Azar
- Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana, USA
| | - Arthur J Baroody
- University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA
| | - Nicole M McNeil
- Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Muñez D, Bull R, Lee K, Ruiz C. Heterogeneity in children at risk of math learning difficulties. Child Dev 2023. [PMID: 36919958 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13918] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2022] [Revised: 01/15/2023] [Accepted: 01/26/2023] [Indexed: 03/16/2023]
Abstract
This study recruited 428 Singaporean children at risk of math learning difficulties (MLD; Mage = 83.9 months, SDage = 4.35 months; 41% female). Using a factor mixture model that considered both quantitative and qualitative differences in math ability, two qualitatively different groups were identified: one with generalized difficulties across different math skills and the other with more focal difficulties in arithmetic fluency. Reading, working memory capacity, and numeracy (number line estimation skills and numerical discrimination) uniquely explained group membership. Children within each group differed in the extent of difficulties they exhibited, with numeracy variables differentially contributing to math ability in each group. Findings speak against a dimensional view of MLD and underscore the conceptual limitations of using basic numeracy performance to profile learning difficulties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Muñez
- Centre for Research in Child Development, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| | - Rebecca Bull
- Department of Educational Studies, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Kerry Lee
- Department of Early Childhood Education, The University of Education Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
| | - Carola Ruiz
- Department of Educational Studies, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
What Is the Source of the Correlation Between Reading and Mathematics Achievement? Two Meta-analytic Studies. EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2023. [DOI: 10.1007/s10648-023-09717-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
|
9
|
Mou Y, Zhang B, Hyde DC. Directionality in the interrelations between approximate number, verbal number, and mathematics in preschool-aged children. Child Dev 2023; 94:e67-e84. [PMID: 36528845 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13879] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
A fundamental question in numerical development concerns the directional relation between an early-emerging non-verbal approximate number system (ANS) and culturally acquired verbal number and mathematics knowledge. Using path models on longitudinal data collected in preschool children (Mage = 3.86 years; N = 216; 99 males; 80.8% White; 10.8% Multiracial, 3.8% Latino; 1.9% Black; collected 2013-2017) over 1 year, this study showed that earlier verbal number knowledge was associated with later ANS precision (average β = .32), even after controlling for baseline differences in numerical, general cognitive, and language abilities. In contrast, earlier ANS precision was not associated with later verbal number knowledge (β = -.07) or mathematics abilities (average β = .10). These results suggest that learning about verbal numbers is associated with a sharpening of pre-existing non-verbal numerical abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yi Mou
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, People's Republic of China
| | - Bo Zhang
- School of Labor and Employment Relations, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA.,Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA
| | - Daniel C Hyde
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Haman M, Lipowska K, Soltanlou M, Cipora K, Domahs F, Nuerk HC. The plural counts: Inconsistent grammatical number hinders numerical development in preschoolers - A cross-linguistic study. Cognition 2023; 235:105383. [PMID: 36753808 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105383] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2022] [Revised: 01/16/2023] [Accepted: 01/19/2023] [Indexed: 02/09/2023]
Abstract
The role of grammar in numerical development, and particularly the role of grammatical number inflection, has already been well-documented in toddlerhood. It is unclear, however, whether the influence of grammatical language structure further extends to more complex later stages of numerical development. Here, we addressed this question by exploiting differences between Polish, which has a complex grammatical number paradigm, leading to a partially inconsistent mapping between numerical quantities and grammatical number, and German, which has a comparatively easy verbal paradigm: 151 Polish-speaking and 123 German-speaking kindergarten children were tested using a symbolic numerical comparison task. Additionally, counting skills (Give-a-Number and count-list), and mapping between non-symbolic (dot sets) and symbolic representations of numbers, as well as working memory (Corsi blocks and Digit span) were assessed. Based on the Give-a-Number and mapping tasks, the children were divided into subset-knowers, CP-knowers-non-mappers, and CP-knowers-mappers. Linguistic background was related to performance in several ways: Polish-speaking children expectedly progressed to the CP-knowers stage later than German children, despite comparable non-numerical capabilities, and even after this stage was achieved, they fared worse in the numerical comparison task. There were also meaningful differences in spatial-numerical mapping between the Polish and German groups. Our findings are in line with the theory that grammatical number paradigms influence. the development of representations and processing of numbers, not only at the stage of acquiring the meaning of the first number-words but at later stages as well, when dealing with symbolic numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maciej Haman
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland.
| | | | - Mojtaba Soltanlou
- Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; LEAD Graduate School & Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; School of Psychology, University of Surrey, UK
| | - Krzysztof Cipora
- Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; LEAD Graduate School & Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; Centre for Mathematical Cognition, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK
| | - Frank Domahs
- Department of Linguistics, University of Erfurt, Erfurt, Germany
| | - Hans-Christoph Nuerk
- Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; LEAD Graduate School & Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Chen Y, Han C, Yu X, Yang X, Jiang J, Zhao Y. Contributions of cognitive flexibility, inhibition and number label knowledge to numerical equivalence in 3- to 5-year-old children. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 41:140-156. [PMID: 36577546 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/21/2021] [Revised: 10/30/2022] [Accepted: 12/14/2022] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
To investigate the contributions of cognitive flexibility, inhibition and number label knowledge to children's numerical equivalence, one hundred and one 3- to 5-year-olds were administered the dimensional change card sorting task, the day-night task and the give-a-number task. The numerical equivalence was assessed with the numerical matching task in three surface similarity conditions. Results showed that, in the high surface similarity condition, cognitive flexibility and label knowledge, rather than inhibition, were significant predictors of children's performance in numerical equivalence. In the low surface similarity and the cross-mapping conditions, only cognitive flexibility, rather than number label knowledge and inhibition, significantly explained the unique variance in numerical equivalence. Besides, cognitive flexibility explained more variation in numerical equivalence in the cross-mapping condition compared with the low surface similarity condition. These findings highlight different roles of cognitive flexibility, inhibition and number label knowledge in numerical equivalence in the three surface similarity conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yinghe Chen
- Faculty of Psychology, School of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Congcong Han
- School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan, China
| | - Xiao Yu
- Department of Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Beijing Forestry University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiujie Yang
- Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China.,State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Jiali Jiang
- Department of Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Beijing Forestry University, Beijing, China
| | - Yuxi Zhao
- Department of Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Beijing Forestry University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Tomasetto C, LeFevre JA, Passolunghi MC, De Vita C, Guardabassi V, Brunelli A, Ciotti F, Biasini G. With a little help from our pediatrician: An intervention to promote mathematics-related home activities through regular well-child visits. Front Psychol 2022; 13:1051822. [PMID: 36544453 PMCID: PMC9763055 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1051822] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2022] [Accepted: 11/11/2022] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Introduction Children's involvement in mathematics-related activities in the home environment is associated with the development of their early numeracy over the preschool years. Intervention studies to promote parents' awareness and provision of mathematics-related home activities are however scant. In this study we developed and tested the effectiveness of a non-intensive intervention program delivered by community pediatricians to promote mathematics-related activities in the home environment. Methods Parents of 204 Italian children were invited to report on the frequency of mathematics-related home activities when children attended the first preschool year (3 years, 8 months of age on average) and, subsequently, the third preschool year (5 years, 6 months of age on average). At both waves, children were also assessed on their early numeracy. In occasion of the routine well-child visit at age 5, parents who were randomly allocated to the intervention condition (vs. a business-as-usual control condition) received guidance on age-appropriate home mathematics-related practices to sustain children's numerical development. Results Results revealed that parents in the intervention group improved their provision of home mathematics-related activities at the post-intervention assessment (relative to baseline) to a greater extent than parents in the control condition. No effect was observed on children's early numeracy. Discussion Overall, results are promising in suggesting that community pediatricians may be a resource to promote home mathematics-related activities though non-intensive low-cost interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carlo Tomasetto
- Department of Psychology Renzo Canestrari, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Jo-Anne LeFevre
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Science, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | | | - Chiara De Vita
- Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
| | - Veronica Guardabassi
- Department of Psychology Renzo Canestrari, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | - Antonella Brunelli
- Azienda Unità Sanitaria Locale (AUSL) della Romagna, Cesena, Italy
- Associazione Culturale Pediatri – Romagna (ACPR), Cesena, Italy
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Eason SH, Scalise NR, Berkowitz T, Ramani GB, Levine SC. Widening the lens of family math engagement: A conceptual framework and systematic review. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2022.101046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
14
|
Elliott L, Bachman HJ, Carvalho Pereira J, Coulanges L, Duong S, Montue T, Miller P, Libertus M, Votruba-Drzal E. Self-regulation in toddlers and the emergence of pre-academic disparities. Infant Behav Dev 2022; 69:101779. [PMID: 36274356 PMCID: PMC10184574 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101779] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2022] [Revised: 10/01/2022] [Accepted: 10/09/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
A growing body of research has examined how children's self-regulation during early and middle childhood mediates SES disparities in academic achievement. Evidence suggests that these self-regulation skills begin developing even earlier, during the toddler years, but more work is needed examining how different measures of self-regulation relate to key constructs such as socioeconomic status (SES) and toddlers' pre-academic skills. In this online study, we examine multiple approaches to measuring self-regulation using confirmatory factor analyses and assess the extent to which self-regulatory skills help explain SES differences in early math and language skills among a sample of 158 two- and three-year-old children. Self-regulation was assessed through a battery of parent- and examiner-ratings. Children's counting, cardinality, and vocabulary skills were measured online through direct assessments and parent surveys. Two self-regulation factors emerged representing parent-reported and observational measures, and only observational measures of self-regulation mediated associations between SES and children's math and language skills. Parent-reported self-regulation was not uniquely related to SES or children's pre-academic skills, underscoring the need for careful consideration of how self-regulation is measured among toddlers when examining its associations with pre-academic skills.
Collapse
|
15
|
Jordan NC, Devlin BL, Botello M. Core foundations of early mathematics: refining the number sense framework. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
16
|
Silver AM, Libertus ME. Environmental influences on mathematics performance in early childhood. NATURE REVIEWS PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 1:407-418. [PMID: 36330081 PMCID: PMC9624502 DOI: 10.1038/s44159-022-00061-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/15/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
Math skills relate to lifelong career, health, and financial outcomes. Individuals' own cognitive abilities predict math performance and there is growing recognition that environmental influences including differences in culture and variability in math engagement also impact math skills. In this Review, we summarize evidence indicating that differences between languages, exposure to math-focused language, socioeconomic status, attitudes and beliefs about math, and engagement with math activities influence young children's math performance. These influences play out at the community and individual level. However, research on the role of these environmental influences for foundational number skills, including understanding of number words, is limited. Future research is needed to understand individual differences in the development of early emerging math skills such as number word skills, examining to what extent different types of environmental input are necessary and how children's cognitive abilities shape the impact of environmental input.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex M. Silver
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Rajagopal AA, Vandecruys F, De Smedt B. The effects of preschool and age on children’s early number skills. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101227] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
18
|
Marchand E, Lovelett JT, Kendro K, Barner D. Assessing the knower-level framework: How reliable is the Give-a-Number task? Cognition 2022; 222:104998. [PMID: 35144098 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104998] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2021] [Revised: 11/21/2021] [Accepted: 12/22/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The Give-a-Number task has become a gold standard of children's number word comprehension in developmental psychology. Recently, researchers have begun to use the task as a predictor of other developmental milestones. This raises the question of how reliable the task is, since test-retest reliability of any measure places an upper bound on the size of reliable correlations that can be found between it and other measures. In Experiment 1, we presented 81 2- to 5-year-old children with Wynn (1992) titrated version of the Give-a-Number task twice within a single session. We found that the reliability of this version of the task was high overall, but varied importantly across different assigned knower levels, and was very low for some knower levels. In Experiment 2, we assessed the test-retest reliability of the non-titrated version of the Give-a-Number task with another group of 81 children and found a similar pattern of results. Finally, in Experiment 3, we asked whether the two versions of Give-a-Number generated different knower levels within-subjects, by testing 75 children with both tasks. Also, we asked how both tasks relate to another commonly used test of number knowledge, the "What's-On-This-Card" task. We found that overall, the titrated and non-titrated versions of Give-a-Number yielded similar knower levels, though the non-titrated version was slightly more conservative than the titrated version, which produced modestly higher knower levels. Neither was more closely related to "What's-On-This-Card" than the other. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elisabeth Marchand
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America.
| | - Jarrett T Lovelett
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America
| | - Kelly Kendro
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America
| | - David Barner
- Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Cuder A, Vidoz M, De Vita C, Pellizzoni S, Passolunghi MC. Numerical Training Videos and Early Numerical Achievement: A Study on 3-Year-Old Preschoolers. Brain Sci 2022; 12:88. [PMID: 35053831 PMCID: PMC8773724 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12010088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2021] [Revised: 01/06/2022] [Accepted: 01/07/2022] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Early numerical abilities predict later math achievement and could be improved in children by using various training methods. As the literature on the use of training videos to develop numerical abilities is still surprisingly scant, the aim of the present study was to test the efficacy of a numerical training video on the development of counting and number line knowledge in 3-year-old preschoolers. Far transfer effects to cardinality and working memory were also examined. The study involved 86 children randomly assigned to two intervention groups: a numerical training group exposed to videos on counting and number lines; and a control group exposed to videos on colors and animal names in a foreign language. After the video training, there was an improvement in the numerical training group's counting skills, but not in their number line knowledge, and this improvement persisted six months later. The numerical training group also showed a far-transfer enhancement of cardinality six months after the intervention. Based on our results, numerical training videos could be effective in helping to enhance early numeracy skills in very young preschoolers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - Maria Chiara Passolunghi
- Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste, 34128 Trieste, Italy; (A.C.); (M.V.); (C.D.V.); (S.P.)
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Sokolowski HM, Merkley R, Kingissepp SSB, Vaikuntharajan P, Ansari D. Children's attention to numerical quantities relates to verbal number knowledge: An introduction to the Build-A-Train task. Dev Sci 2021; 25:e13211. [PMID: 34889002 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2021] [Revised: 10/24/2021] [Accepted: 12/02/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Which dimension of a set of objects is more salient to young children: number or size? The 'Build-A-Train' task was developed and used to examine whether children spontaneously use a number or physical size approach on an un-cued matching task. In the Build-A-Train task, an experimenter assembles a train using one to five blocks of a particular length and asks the child to build the same train. The child's blocks differ in length from the experimenter's blocks, causing the child to build a train that matches based on either the number of blocks or length of the train, as it is not possible to match on both. One hundred and nineteen children between 2 years 2 months and 6 years 0 months of age (M = 4.05, SD = 0.84) completed the Build-A-Train task, and the Give-a-Number task, a classic task used to assess children's conceptual knowledge of verbal number words. Across train lengths and verbal number knowledge levels, children used a number approach more than a size approach on the Build-A-Train task. However, children were especially likely to use a number approach over a size approach when they knew the verbal number word that corresponded to the quantity of blocks in the train, particularly for quantities smaller than four. Therefore, children's attention to number relates to their knowledge of verbal number words. The Build-A-Train task and findings from the current study set a foundation for future longitudinal research to investigate the causal relationship between children's acquisition of symbolic mathematical concepts and attention to number.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Moriah Sokolowski
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Hospital, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| | - Rebecca Merkley
- Department of Cognitive Science, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
| | - Sarah Samantha Bray Kingissepp
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| | - Praja Vaikuntharajan
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| | - Daniel Ansari
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Ünal ZE, Powell SR, Özel S, Scofield JE, Geary DC. Mathematics vocabulary differentially predicts mathematics achievement in eighth grade higher- versus lower- achieving students: Comparisons across two countries. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2021.102061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
22
|
Billard C, Jung C, Munnich A, Gassama S, Touzin M, Mirassou A, Willig TN. External Validation of BMT- i Computerized Test Battery for Diagnosis of Learning Disabilities. Front Pediatr 2021; 9:733713. [PMID: 34660490 PMCID: PMC8517505 DOI: 10.3389/fped.2021.733713] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2021] [Accepted: 08/31/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Learning disabilities (LDs) are a major public health issue, affecting cognitive functions and academic performance for 8% of children. If LDs are not detected early and addressed through appropriate interventions, they have a heavy impact on these children in the social, educational, and professional spheres, at great cost to society. The BMT-i (Batterie Modulable de Tests informatisée, or "computerized Adaptable Test Battery") enables fast, easy, reliable assessments for each cognitive domain. It has previously been validated in children ages 4-13 who had no prior complaints. The present study demonstrates the sensitivity of the BMT-i, relative to reference test batteries, for 191 children with cognitive difficulties. Materials and Methods: These 191 subjects were included in the study by the 14 pediatricians treating them for complaints in five cognitive domains: written language [60 (cases)]; mathematical cognition (40); oral language (60); handwriting, drawing, and visuospatial construction (45); and attention and executive functioning (45). In accordance with a predefined protocol, the children were administered BMT-i tests first, by their pediatricians, and reference tests later, by specialists to whom the BMT-i test results were not disclosed. Comparison of BMT-i and reference test results made it possible to evaluate sensitivity and agreement between tests. Results: For each of the five domains, the BMT-i was very sensitive (0.91-1), and normal BMT-i results were highly predictive of normal results for specialized reference tests [negative likelihood ratio (LR-): 0-0.16]. There was close agreement between BMT-i and reference tests in all domains except attention and executive functioning, for which only moderate agreement was observed. Conclusion: The BMT-i offers rapid, reliable, simple computerized assessments whose sensitivity and agreement with reference test batteries make it a suitable first-line instrument for LD screening in children 4-13 years old.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Catherine Billard
- Association pour la Recherche sur les Troubles des Apprentissages, Paris, France
| | - Camille Jung
- Clinical Research Center, Centre Hospitalier Intercommunal de Créteil, Créteil, France
| | - Arnold Munnich
- Université de Paris and Imagine Institute (INSERM UMR1163), Paris, France
| | - Sahawanatou Gassama
- Association pour la Recherche sur les Troubles des Apprentissages, Paris, France
- Centre Ressource sur les Troubles des Apprentissages, Paris Santé Réussite, Paris, France
| | - Monique Touzin
- Association pour la Recherche sur les Troubles des Apprentissages, Paris, France
- Centre Ressource sur les Troubles des Apprentissages, Paris Santé Réussite, Paris, France
| | - Anne Mirassou
- Association pour la Recherche sur les Troubles des Apprentissages, Paris, France
| | - Thiébaut-Noël Willig
- ELSAN & EvEnTAil Assessment Center, Toulouse, France
- Association Française de Pédiatrie Ambulatoire, Orléans, France
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Relations between Subdomains of Home Math Activities and Corresponding Math Skills in 4-Year-Old Children. EDUCATION SCIENCES 2021. [DOI: 10.3390/educsci11100594] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Most studies on the subject have investigated relations between home math activities and child math skills, without paying much attention to the specific skills that such activities foster and their alignment with children’s math assessments. The present study examined specific relations between subdomains of home math activities and children’s corresponding math skills (e.g., home counting/cardinality activities related to children’s counting/cardinality skills). Participants were 78 mostly middle-income, White parents and their four-year-old children (M age = 53.19 months; 45% girls). Parents completed a 24-item survey about the frequency of home activities supporting five subdomains of math: counting/cardinality, set comparison, number identification, adding/subtracting, and patterning. Children’s skills in these same five subdomains were assessed using the Preschool Early Numeracy Scale (PENS) and the Early Patterning Assessment. Specific relations were observed in set comparison, adding/subtracting, and patterning, such that higher frequency of home activities in these subdomains related to advanced child math skills in the corresponding subdomains. No specific relations were found in counting/cardinality and number identification. Overall home math activities averaged across the five math subdomains positively related to children’s overall math skills. Findings highlight the importance of engagement in specific math activities in the home environment and their significance for corresponding child math development.
Collapse
|
24
|
Intensifying Instruction to Address Treatment Resistance to Early Mathematics Interventions. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-021-02320-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
25
|
Bugden S, Park A, Mackey A, Brannon E. The neural basis of number word processing in children and adults. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2021; 51:101011. [PMID: 34562794 PMCID: PMC8476348 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2020] [Revised: 08/30/2021] [Accepted: 09/08/2021] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
The ability to map number words to their corresponding quantity representations is a gatekeeper for children's future math success (Spaepen et al., 2018). Without number word knowledge at school entry, children are at greater risk for developing math learning difficulties (Chu et al., 2019). In the present study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the neural basis for processing the meaning of spoken number words and its developmental trajectory in 4- to 10-year-old children, and in adults. In a number word-quantity mapping paradigm, participants listened to number words while simultaneously viewing quantities that were congruent or incongruent to the number word they heard. Whole brain analyses revealed that adults showed a neural congruity effect with greater neural activation for incongruent relative to congruent trials in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and left intraparietal sulcus (LIPS). In contrast, children did not show a significant neural congruity effect. However, a region of interest analysis in the child sample demonstrated age-related increases in the neural congruity effect, specifically in the LIPS. The positive correlation between neural congruity in LIPS and age was stronger in children who were already attending school, suggesting that developmental changes in LIPS function are experience-dependent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S. Bugden
- Department of Psychology, University of Winnipeg, 515 Portage Ave, Manitoba, R3B 2E9, Canada,Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, 425 S. University Ave, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA,Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Winnipeg, 515 Portage Ave, Manitoba, R3B 2E9, Canada.
| | - A.T. Park
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, 425 S. University Ave, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - A.P. Mackey
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, 425 S. University Ave, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - E.M. Brannon
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, 425 S. University Ave, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Preschoolers' mastery of advanced counting: The best predictor of addition skills 2 years later. J Exp Child Psychol 2021; 212:105252. [PMID: 34352661 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105252] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2020] [Revised: 06/30/2021] [Accepted: 07/02/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The current study addressed the following question: Among preschoolers' basic numerical abilities, what are the best predictors for the later addition skills? We measured numerical abilities at preschool age and used dominance analysis to determine the dominant predictor for addition skills 2 years later. We tested seven numerical specific predictors (counting, advanced counting, enumeration, Give-N, collection comparison, number-word comparison, and approximate addition). Both quantitative and qualitative aspects (accuracy, strategy choice, and fluency) of addition skills were measured. The results show that the predictor weights for addition skills were 39% (counting), 37% (advanced counting), and 25% (collection comparison). We concluded that counting ability and especially advanced counting measured in early preschool is the most robust predictor of addition skills 2 years later (even after controlling for global cognitive abilities). This study generalized the previous findings found for Western children to Vietnamese preschoolers (N = 157, Mage = 4.8 years); extended and highlighted the role of advanced counting (count from a number other than 1) to later addition performance, mature strategy, and calculation fluency; and suggested further implications.
Collapse
|
27
|
Vogel SE, De Smedt B. Developmental brain dynamics of numerical and arithmetic abilities. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2021; 6:22. [PMID: 34301948 PMCID: PMC8302738 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-021-00099-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2020] [Accepted: 06/24/2021] [Indexed: 05/07/2023]
Abstract
The development of numerical and arithmetic abilities constitutes a crucial cornerstone in our modern and educated societies. Difficulties to acquire these central skills can lead to severe consequences for an individual's well-being and nation's economy. In the present review, we describe our current broad understanding of the functional and structural brain organization that supports the development of numbers and arithmetic. The existing evidence points towards a complex interaction among multiple domain-specific (e.g., representation of quantities and number symbols) and domain-general (e.g., working memory, visual-spatial abilities) cognitive processes, as well as a dynamic integration of several brain regions into functional networks that support these processes. These networks are mainly, but not exclusively, located in regions of the frontal and parietal cortex, and the functional and structural dynamics of these networks differ as a function of age and performance level. Distinctive brain activation patterns have also been shown for children with dyscalculia, a specific learning disability in the domain of mathematics. Although our knowledge about the developmental brain dynamics of number and arithmetic has greatly improved over the past years, many questions about the interaction and the causal involvement of the abovementioned functional brain networks remain. This review provides a broad and critical overview of the known developmental processes and what is yet to be discovered.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephan E Vogel
- Educational Neuroscience, Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria.
| | - Bert De Smedt
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Berkowitz T, Gibson DJ, Levine SC. Parent math anxiety predicts early number talk. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2021; 22:523-536. [PMID: 34335106 DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2021.1926252] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Differences in children's math knowledge emerge as early as the start of kindergarten, and persist throughout schooling. Previous research implicates the importance of early parent number talk in the development of math competency. Yet we understand little about the factors that relate to variation in early parent number talk. The current study examined the relation of parent math anxiety and family socioeconomic status (SES) to parent number talk with children under the age of three (n = 36 dyads). For the first time, we show preliminary evidence that parent math anxiety (MA) predicts the amount of number talk children hear at home, beyond differences accounted for by SES. We also found a significant SES by parent MA interaction such that parent MA was predictive of higher-SES parents' number talk but not that of lower-SES parents. Furthermore, we found that these relations were specific to parents' cardinal number talk (but not counting), which has been shown to be particularly important in children's math development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Talia Berkowitz
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Dominic J Gibson
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Susan C Levine
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
When one size does not fit all: A latent profile analysis of low-income preschoolers' math skills. J Exp Child Psychol 2021; 209:105156. [PMID: 34089919 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105156] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2020] [Revised: 02/01/2021] [Accepted: 03/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
On average, preschoolers from lower-income households perform worse on symbolic numerical tasks than preschoolers from middle- and upper-income households. Although many recent studies have developed and tested mathematics interventions for low-income preschoolers, the variability within this population has received less attention. The goal of the current study was to describe the variability in low-income children's math skills using a person-centered analysis. We conducted a latent profile analysis on six measures of preschoolers' (N = 115, mean age = 4.6 years) numerical abilities (nonsymbolic magnitude comparison, verbal counting, object counting, cardinality, numeral identification, and symbolic magnitude comparison). The results showed different patterns of strengths and weaknesses and revealed four profiles of numerical skills: (a) poor math abilities on all numerical measures (n = 13), (b) strong math abilities on all numerical measures (n = 41), (c) moderate abilities on all numerical measures (n = 35), and (d) strong counting and numeral skills but poor magnitude skills (n = 26). Children's age, working memory, and inhibitory control significantly predicted their profile membership. We found evidence of quantitative and qualitative differences between profiles, such that some profiles were higher performing across tasks than others, but the overall patterns of performance varied across the different numerical skills assessed.
Collapse
|
30
|
Lago MO, Escudero A, Dopico C. The Relationship Between Confidence and Conformity in a Non-routine Counting Task With Young Children: Dedicated to the Memory of Purificación Rodríguez. Front Psychol 2021; 12:593509. [PMID: 34135796 PMCID: PMC8202410 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.593509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2020] [Accepted: 04/23/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Counting is a complex cognitive process that is paramount to arithmetical development at school. The improvement of counting skills of children depends on their understanding of the logical and conventional rules involved. While the logical rules are mandatory and related to one-to-one correspondence, stable order, and cardinal principles, conventional rules are optional and associated with social customs. This study contributes to unravel the conceptual understanding of counting rules of children. It explores, with a developmental approach, the performance of children on non-routine counting detection tasks, their confidence in their answers (metacognitive monitoring skills), and their ability to change a wrong answer by deferring to the opinion of a unanimous majority who justified or did not justify their claims. Hundred and forty nine children aged from 5 to 8 years were randomized to one of the experimental conditions of the testimony of teachers: with (n = 74) or without justification (n = 75). Participants judged the correctness of different types of counting procedures presented by a computerized detection task, such as (a) pseudoerrors that are correct counts where conventional rules are violated (e.g., first counting six footballs, followed by other six basketballs that were interspersed along the row), and (b) compensation errors that are incorrect counts where logical rules were broken twice (e.g., skipping the third element of the row and then labeling the sixth element with two number words, 5 and 6). Afterwards, children rated their confidence in their detection answer with a 5-point scale. Subsequently, they listened to the testimony of the teachers and showed either conformity or non-conformity. The participants considered both compensation errors and pseudoerrors as incorrect counts in the detection task. The analysis of the confidence of children in their responses suggested that they were not sensitive to their incorrect performance. Finally, children tended to conform more often after hearing a justification of the testimony than after hearing only the testimonies of the teachers. It can be concluded that the age range of the evaluated children failed to recognize the optional nature of conventional counting rules and were unaware of their misconceptions. Nevertheless, the reasoned justifications of the testimony, offered by a unanimous majority, promoted considerable improvement in the tendency of the children to revise those misconceptions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ma Oliva Lago
- Departamento de Investigación y Psicología en Educación, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Ana Escudero
- Departamento de Investigación y Psicología en Educación, Facultad de Educación, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Cristina Dopico
- Departamento de Investigación y Psicología en Educación, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Scalise NR, Ramani GB. Symbolic Magnitude Understanding Predicts Preschoolers’ Later Addition Skills. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2021.1888732] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole R. Scalise
- University of California, Irvine
- University of Maryland, College Park
| | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Schneider RM, Sullivan J, Guo K, Barner D. What Counts? Sources of Knowledge in Children's Acquisition of the Successor Function. Child Dev 2021; 92:e476-e492. [PMID: 33476044 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Although many U.S. children can count sets by 4 years, it is not until 5½-6 years that they understand how counting relates to number-that is, that adding 1 to a set necessitates counting up one number. This study examined two knowledge sources that 3½- to 6-year-olds (N = 136) may leverage to acquire this "successor function": (a) mastery of productive rules governing count list generation; and (b) training with "+1" math facts. Both productive counting and "+1" math facts were related to understanding that adding 1 to sets entails counting up one number in the count list; however, even children with robust successor knowledge struggled with its arithmetic expression, suggesting they do not generalize the successor function from "+1" math facts.
Collapse
|
33
|
Lê MLT, Noël MP. Transparent number-naming system gives only limited advantage for preschooler's numerical development: Comparisons of Vietnamese and French-speaking children. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0243472. [PMID: 33284824 PMCID: PMC7721146 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243472] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/10/2020] [Accepted: 11/21/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Several cross-sectional studies have suggested that the transparency of the number-naming system of East Asian languages (Chinese, Japanese) facilitates children's numerical development. The Vietnamese number-naming system also makes the base-10 system very explicit (eleven is "mười một," literally "ten-one," and thirty is "ba mươi," literally "three-ten"). In contrast, Western languages (English, French) include teen words (eleven to sixteen) and ten words (twenty to ninety) that make their counting systems less transparent. The main question addressed in this paper is: To what extent does a language's number-naming system impact preschoolers' numerical development? Our study participants comprised 104 Vietnamese and 104 French-speaking Belgian children between 3½ and 5½ years of age, as well as their parents. We tested the children on eight numerical tasks (counting, advanced counting, enumeration, Give-N, number-word comparison, collection comparison, addition, and approximate addition) and some general cognitive abilities (IQ and phonological loop by letter span). The parents completed a questionnaire on the frequency with which they stimulated their child's numeracy and literacy at home. The results indicated that Vietnamese children outperformed Belgian children only in counting. However, neither group differed in other symbolic or non-symbolic abilities, although Vietnamese parents tended to stimulate their child at home slightly more than Belgian parents. We concluded that the Vietnamese number-naming system's transparency led to faster acquisition of basic counting for preschoolers but did not support other more advanced numerical skills or non-symbolic numerical abilities. In addition, we extended the evidence that both transparent number-naming system and home numeracy influence young children's counting development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mai-Liên T. Lê
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
- Faculty of Psychology, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, National University of Vietnam in Ho Chi Minh City, Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam
| | - Marie-Pascale Noël
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Gibson DJ, Gunderson EA, Levine SC. Causal Effects of Parent Number Talk on Preschoolers' Number Knowledge. Child Dev 2020; 91:e1162-e1177. [PMID: 33164211 PMCID: PMC10683715 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13423] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Individual differences in children's number knowledge arise early and are associated with variation in parents' number talk. However, there exists little experimental evidence of a causal link between parent number talk and children's number knowledge. Parent number talk was manipulated by creating picture books which parents were asked to read with their children every day for 4 weeks. N = 100 two- to four-year olds and their parents were randomly assigned to read either Small Number (1-3), Large Number (4-6), or Control (non-numerical) books. Small Number books were particularly effective in promoting number knowledge relative to the Control books. However, children who began the study further along in their number development also benefited from reading the Large Number Books with their parents.
Collapse
|
35
|
Mutaf-Yıldız B, Sasanguie D, De Smedt B, Reynvoet B. Probing the Relationship Between Home Numeracy and Children's Mathematical Skills: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol 2020; 11:2074. [PMID: 33071838 PMCID: PMC7530373 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2020] [Accepted: 07/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The concept of home numeracy has been defined as parent–child interactions with numerical content. This concept started to receive increasing attention since the last decade. Most of the studies indicated that the more parents and their children engage in numerical experiences, the better children perform in mathematical tasks. However, there are also contrasting results indicating that home numeracy does not play a role or that there is a negative association between the parent–child interactions and children's mathematics performance. To shed light on these discrepancies, a systematic review searching for available articles examining the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills was conducted. Thirty-seven articles were retained and a p-curve analysis showed a true positive association between home numeracy and children's mathematical skills. A more qualitative investigation of the articles revealed five common findings: (1) Advanced home numeracy interactions but not basic ones are associated with children's mathematical skills. (2) Most participants in the studies were mothers, however, when both parents participated and were compared, only mothers' reports of formal home numeracy activities (i.e., explicit numeracy teaching) were linked to children's mathematical skill. (3) Formal home numeracy activities have been investigated more commonly than informal home numeracy activities (i.e., implicit numeracy teaching). (4) The number of studies that have used questionnaires to assess home numeracy is larger compared with the ones that have used observations. (5) The majority of the studies measured children's mathematical skills with comprehensive tests that index mathematical ability with one composite score rather than with specific numerical tasks. These five common findings might explain the contradictory results regarding the relationship between home numeracy and mathematical skills. Therefore, more research is necessary to draw quantitative conclusions about these five points.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Belde Mutaf-Yıldız
- Brain and Cognition, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | | | - Bert De Smedt
- Parenting and Special Education, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Bert Reynvoet
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven Kulak, Kortrijk, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Wong TTY, Liu D. The association between visual attention and arithmetic competence: The mediating role of enumeration. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 196:104864. [PMID: 32344115 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104864] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2019] [Revised: 03/29/2020] [Accepted: 03/30/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The current study aimed at clarifying the nature of relation between visual attention and arithmetic competence. A group of 301 Chinese second graders was assessed. Children's visual attention was measured using two versions of a visual search task, with efficient visual search (the similarity between the target and the distractors is low) tapping automatic, stimulus-driven visual attention and inefficient visual search (the similarity between the target and the distractors is high) tapping effortful, goal-directed visual attention. Children's arithmetic competence, enumeration skills (assessed in about half of the participants), and other domain-general cognitive abilities were also assessed. The results suggest that only inefficient visual search significantly predicted children's arithmetic competence, and such a relation was mediated through their enumeration skills. The findings highlight the role of fundamental cognitive capacities in mathematics learning and provide insights into potential interventions for improving children's arithmetic competence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Terry Tin-Yau Wong
- Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Pok Fu Lam, Hong Kong.
| | - Duo Liu
- Department of Special Education and Counseling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Ting Kok, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Fischer U, Suggate SP, Stoeger H. The Implicit Contribution of Fine Motor Skills to Mathematical Insight in Early Childhood. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1143. [PMID: 32581955 PMCID: PMC7283516 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01143] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2019] [Accepted: 05/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding number magnitude is an important prerequisite for children's mathematical development. One early experience that contributes to this understanding is the common practice of finger counting. Recent research suggested that through repeated finger counting, children internalize their fingers as representations of number magnitude. Furthermore, finger counting habits have been proposed to predict concurrent and future mathematical performance. However, little is known about how finger-based number representations are formed and by which processes they could influence mathematical development. Regarding the emergence of finger-based number representations, it is likely that they result from repeated practice of finger counting. Accordingly, children need sufficient fine motor skills (FMS) to successfully count on their fingers. However, the role that different types of FMS (such as dexterity and graphomotor skills) might play in the development of finger-based number representations is still unknown. In the current study, we investigated (a) whether children's FMS (dexterity and graphomotor skills) are associated with their emerging finger-based number representations (ordinal and cardinal), (b) whether FMS explain variance in children's finger-based number representations beyond the influence of general cognitive skills, and (c) whether the association between FMS and numerical skills is mediated by finger-based representations. We tested associations between preschool children's (N = 80) FMS (dexterity and graphomotor skills), finger-based number representations, and numerical skills. Furthermore, visuo-spatial working memory and nonverbal intelligence were controlled for. Dexterity was related to children's finger-based number representations as well as numerical skills after controlling for chronological age, but not after also controlling for cognitive skills. Moreover, the relationship between dexterity and numerical skills was mediated by finger-based number representations. No such associations were observed for graphomotor skills. These results suggest that dexterity plays a role in children's development of finger-based number representations, which in turn contribute to their numerical skills. Possible explanations are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ursula Fischer
- Department of Sport Science, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
- Thurgau University of Teacher Education, Kreuzlingen, Switzerland
- Department of Educational Science, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Sebastian P. Suggate
- Department of Educational Science, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Heidrun Stoeger
- Department of Educational Science, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Schneider RM, Sullivan J, Marušič F, Žaucer R, Biswas P, Mišmaš P, Plesničar V, Barner D. Do children use language structure to discover the recursive rules of counting? Cogn Psychol 2020; 117:101263. [PMID: 31901852 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.101263] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2019] [Revised: 10/25/2019] [Accepted: 12/09/2019] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
We test the hypothesis that children acquire knowledge of the successor function - a foundational principle stating that every natural number n has a successor n + 1 - by learning the productive linguistic rules that govern verbal counting. Previous studies report that speakers of languages with less complex count list morphology have greater counting and mathematical knowledge at earlier ages in comparison to speakers of more complex languages (e.g., Miller & Stigler, 1987). Here, we tested whether differences in count list transparency affected children's acquisition of the successor function in three languages with relatively transparent count lists (Cantonese, Slovenian, and English) and two languages with relatively opaque count lists (Hindi and Gujarati). We measured 3.5- to 6.5-year-old children's mastery of their count list's recursive structure with two tasks assessing productive counting, which we then related to a measure of successor function knowledge. While the more opaque languages were associated with lower counting proficiency and successor function task performance in comparison to the more transparent languages, a unique within-language analytic approach revealed a robust relationship between measures of productive counting and successor knowledge in almost every language. We conclude that learning productive rules of counting is a critical step in acquiring knowledge of recursive successor function across languages, and that the timeline for this learning varies as a function of count list transparency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rose M Schneider
- Psychology Department, University of California, San Diego, United States.
| | | | - Franc Marušič
- Center for Cognitive Science of Language, University of Nova Gorica, Slovenia
| | - Rok Žaucer
- Center for Cognitive Science of Language, University of Nova Gorica, Slovenia
| | - Priyanka Biswas
- Department of Linguistics, University of California, San Diego, United States
| | - Petra Mišmaš
- Center for Cognitive Science of Language, University of Nova Gorica, Slovenia
| | - Vesna Plesničar
- Center for Cognitive Science of Language, University of Nova Gorica, Slovenia
| | - David Barner
- Psychology Department, University of California, San Diego, United States; Department of Linguistics, University of California, San Diego, United States
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
McGonigle-Chalmers M, Kusel I. The Development of Size Sequencing Skills: An Empirical and Computational Analysis. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2019; 84:7-202. [PMID: 31713884 DOI: 10.1111/mono.12411] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
We explore a long-observed phenomenon in children's cognitive development known as size seriation. It is not until children are around 7 years of age that they spontaneously use a strict ascending or descending order of magnitude to organize sets of objects differing in size. Incomplete and inaccurate ordering shown by younger children has been thought to be related to their incomplete grasp of the mathematical concept of a unit. Piaget first brought attention to children's difficulties in solving ordering and size-matching tests, but his tasks and explanations have been progressively neglected due to major theoretical shifts in scholarship on developmental cognition. A cogent alternative to his account has never emerged, leaving size seriation and related abilities as an unexplained case of discontinuity in mental growth. In this monograph, we use a new training methodology, together with computational modeling of the data to offer a new explanation of size seriation development and the emergence of related skills. We describe a connected set of touchscreen tasks that measure the abilities of 5- and 7-year-old children to (a) learn a linear size sequence of five or seven items and (b) identify unique (unit) values within those same sets, such as second biggest and middle-sized. Older children required little or no training to succeed in the sequencing tasks, whereas younger children evinced trial-and-error performance. Marked age differences were found on ordinal identification tasks using matching-to-sample and other methods. Confirming Piaget's findings, these tasks generated learning data with which to develop a computational model of the change. Using variables to represent working and long-term memory (WM and LTM), the computational model represents the information processing of the younger child in terms of a perception-action feedback loop, resulting in a heuristic for achieving a correct sequence. To explain why older children do not require training on the size task, it was hypothesized that an increase in WM to a certain threshold level provides the information-processing capacity to allow the participant to start to detect a minimum interval between each item in the selection. The probabilistic heuristic is thus thought to be replaced during a transitional stage by a serial algorithm that guarantees success. The minimum interval discovery has the effect of controlling search for the next item in a principled monotonic direction. Through a minor additional processing step, this algorithm permits relatively easy identification of ordinal values. The model was tested by simulating the perceptual learning and action selection processes thought to be taking place during trial-and-error sequencing. Error distributions were generated across each item in the sequence and these were found to correspond to the error patterns shown by 5-year-olds. The algorithm that is thought to emerge from successful learning was also tested. It simulated high levels of success on seriation and also on ordinal identification tasks, as shown by 7-year-olds. An unexpected finding from the empirical studies was that, unlike adults, the 7-year-old children showed marked difficulty when they had to compute ordinal size values in tasks that did not permit the use of the serial algorithm. For example, when required to learn a non-monotonic sequence where the ordinal values were in a fixed random order such as "second biggest, middle-sized, smallest, second smallest, biggest," each item has to be found without reference to the "smallest difference" rule used by the algorithm. The difficulty evinced by 7-year-olds was consistent with the idea that the information in LTM is integrally tied to the search procedure itself as a search-and-stop based on a cumulative tally, as distinct from being accessed from a more permanent and atemporal store of stand-alone ordinal values in LTM. The implications of this possible constraint in understanding are discussed in terms of further developmental changes. We conclude that the seriation behavior shown by children at around 7 years represents a qualitative shift in their understanding but not in the sense that Piaget first proposed. We see the emergent algorithm as an information-reducing device, representing a default strategy for how humans come to deal with potentially complex sets of relations. We argue this with regard to counting behaviors in children and also with regard to how linear monotonic devices for resolving certain logical tasks endure into adulthood. Insofar as the monograph reprises any aspect of the Piagetian account, it is in his highlighting of an important cognitive discontinuity in logicomathematical understanding at around the age of 7, and his quest for understanding the transactions with the physical world that lead to it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Iain Kusel
- Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh. Now affiliated with the Sword Group
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Sella F, Lucangeli D. The knowledge of the preceding number reveals a mature understanding of the number sequence. Cognition 2019; 194:104104. [PMID: 31698183 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2018] [Revised: 08/05/2019] [Accepted: 10/08/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
There is an ongoing debate concerning how numbers acquire numerical meaning. On the one hand, it has been argued that symbols acquire meaning via a mapping to external numerosities as represented by the approximate number system (ANS). On the other hand, it has been proposed that the initial mapping of small numerosities to the corresponding number words and the knowledge of the properties of counting list, especially the order relation between symbols, lead to the understanding of the exact numerical magnitude associated with numerical symbols. In the present study, we directly compared these two hypotheses in a group of preschool children who could proficiently count (most of the children were cardinal principle knowers). We used a numerosity estimation task to assess whether children have created a mapping between the ANS and the counting list (i.e., ANS-to-word mapping). Children also completed a direction task to assess their knowledge of the directional property of the counting list. That is, adding one item to a set leads to he next number word in the sequence (i.e., successor knowledge) whereas removing one item leads to the preceding number word (i.e., predecessor knowledge). Similarly, we used a visual order task to assess the knowledge that successive and preceding numbers occupy specific spatial positions on the visual number line (i.e., preceding: [?], [13], [14]; successive: [12], [13], [?]). Finally, children's performance in comparing the magnitude of number words and Arabic numbers indexed the knowledge of exact symbolic numerical magnitude. Approximately half of the children in our sample have created a mapping between the ANS and the counting list. Most of the children mastered the successor knowledge whereas few of them could master the predecessor knowledge. Children revealed a strong tendency to respond with the successive number in the counting list even when an item was removed from a set or the name of the preceding number on the number line was asked. Crucially, we found evidence that both the mastering of the predecessor knowledge and the ability to name the preceding number in the number line relate to the performance in number comparison tasks. Conversely, there was moderate/anecdotal evidence for a relation between the ANS-to-word mapping and number comparison skills. Non-rote access to the number sequence relates to knowledge of the exact magnitude associated with numerical symbols, beyond the mastering of the cardinality principle and domain-general factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daniela Lucangeli
- Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padova, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Chu FW, vanMarle K, Hoard MK, Nugent L, Scofield JE, Geary DC. Preschool deficits in cardinal knowledge and executive function contribute to longer-term mathematical learning disability. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 188:104668. [PMID: 31430570 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104668] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2018] [Revised: 06/25/2019] [Accepted: 07/13/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
In a preschool through first grade longitudinal study, we identified groups of children with persistently low mathematics achievement (n = 14) and children with low achievement in preschool but average achievement in first grade (n = 23). The preschool quantitative developments of these respective groups of children with mathematical learning disability (MLD) and recovered children and a group of typically achieving peers (n = 35) were contrasted, as were their intelligence, executive function, and parental education levels. The core characteristics of the children with MLD were poor executive function and delayed understanding of the cardinal value of number words throughout preschool. These compounded into even more substantive deficits in number and arithmetic at the beginning of first grade. The recovered group had poor executive function and cardinal knowledge during the first year of preschool but showed significant gains during the second year. Despite these gains and average mathematics achievement, the recovered children had subtle deficits with accessing magnitudes associated with numerals and addition combinations (e.g., 5 + 6 = ?) in first grade. The study provides unique insight into domain-general and quantitative deficits in preschool that increase risk for long-term mathematical difficulties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felicia W Chu
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| | - Kristy vanMarle
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| | - Mary K Hoard
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| | - Lara Nugent
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| | - John E Scofield
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| | - David C Geary
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA; Interdisciplinary Neuroscience Program, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Hutchison JE, Ansari D, Zheng S, De Jesus S, Lyons IM. The relation between subitizable symbolic and non-symbolic number processing over the kindergarten school year. Dev Sci 2019; 23:e12884. [PMID: 31271687 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12884] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2018] [Revised: 06/07/2019] [Accepted: 06/24/2019] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
A long-standing debate in the field of numerical cognition concerns the degree to which symbolic and non-symbolic processing are related over the course of development. Of particular interest is the possibility that this link depends on the range of quantities in question. Behavioral and neuroimaging research with adults suggests that symbolic and non-symbolic quantities may be processed more similarly within, relative to outside of, the subitizing range. However, it remains unclear whether this unique link exists in young children at the outset of formal education. Further, no study has yet taken numerical size into account when investigating the longitudinal influence of these skills. To address these questions, we investigated the relation between symbolic and non-symbolic processing inside versus outside the subitizing range, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally, in 540 kindergarteners. Cross-sectionally, we found a consistently stronger relation between symbolic and non-symbolic number processing within versus outside the subitizing range at both the beginning and end of kindergarten. We also show evidence for a bidirectional relation over the course of kindergarten between formats within the subitizing range, and a unidirectional relation (symbolic → non-symbolic) for quantities outside of the subitizing range. These findings extend current theories on symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude development by suggesting that non-symbolic processing may in fact play a role in the development of symbolic number abilities, but that this influence may be limited to quantities within the subitizing range.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jane E Hutchison
- Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Daniel Ansari
- Department of Psychology and Brain & Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| | - Samuel Zheng
- Research and Development, Toronto District School Board, North York, Ontario, Canada
| | - Stefanie De Jesus
- Research and Development, Toronto District School Board, North York, Ontario, Canada
| | - Ian M Lyons
- Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
O'Rear CD, McNeil NM. Improved set-size labeling mediates the effect of a counting intervention on children's understanding of cardinality. Dev Sci 2019; 22:e12819. [PMID: 30779262 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12819] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/07/2017] [Revised: 02/02/2019] [Accepted: 02/12/2019] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
How does improving children's ability to label set sizes without counting affect the development of understanding of the cardinality principle? It may accelerate development by facilitating subsequent alignment and comparison of the cardinal label for a given set and the last word counted when counting that set (Mix et al., 2012). Alternatively, it may delay development by decreasing the need for a comprehensive abstract principle to understand and label exact numerosities (Piantadosi et al., 2012). In this study, preschoolers (N = 106, Mage = 4;8) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: (a) count-and-label, wherein children spent 6 weeks both counting and labeling sets arranged in canonical patterns like pips on a die; (b) label-first,wherein children spent the first 3 weeks learning to label the set sizes without counting before spending 3 weeks identical to the count-and-label condition; (c) print referencing control. Both counting conditions improved understanding of cardinality through increases in children's ability to label set sizes without counting. In addition to this indirect effect, there was a direct effect of the count-and-label condition on progress toward understanding of cardinality. Results highlight the roles of set labeling and equifinality in the development of children's understanding of number concepts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Connor D O'Rear
- Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana
| | - Nicole M McNeil
- Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Geary DC, vanMarle K, Chu FW, Hoard MK, Nugent L. Predicting Age of Becoming a Cardinal Principle Knower. JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2019; 111:256-267. [PMID: 37275456 PMCID: PMC10237038 DOI: 10.1037/edu0000277] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/27/2024]
Abstract
Children's first mathematics concept is their understanding of the quantities represented by number words (cardinal value), and the age at which they achieve this insight predicts their readiness for mathematics learning in school. We provide the first exploration of the factors that influence the age of becoming a cardinal principle knower (CPK), with a longitudinal study of 197 (94 boys) children from the beginning to the end of two years of preschool. Core symbolic and non-symbolic quantitative competencies at the beginning of preschool, as well as measures of intelligence, executive function, preliteracy skills, and parental education were used to predict timing of CPK status. Children who achieved early CPK status had higher IQ scores, knew more count words and numerals, and had a better intuitive understanding of relative quantity than their peers. Children who were delayed CPKs, in contrast, had deficits in executive function and poor preliteracy skills. The results add to our understanding of children's conceptual development in mathematics and have implications for the identification of at-risk children and design of interventions for them.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David C. Geary
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
- Interdisciplinary Neuroscience Program, University of Missouri
| | - Kristy vanMarle
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
| | - Felicia W. Chu
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
| | - Mary K. Hoard
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
| | - Lara Nugent
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Vanderwegen J, Van Nuffelen G, Elen R, De Bodt M. The Influence of Age, Sex, Visual Feedback, Bulb Position, and the Order of Testing on Maximum Anterior and Posterior Tongue Strength in Healthy Belgian Children. Dysphagia 2019; 34:834-851. [DOI: 10.1007/s00455-019-09976-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2018] [Accepted: 01/02/2019] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
|
46
|
Wilkey ED, Pollack C, Price GR. Dyscalculia and Typical Math Achievement Are Associated With Individual Differences in Number-Specific Executive Function. Child Dev 2018; 91:596-619. [PMID: 30597527 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13194] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Deficits in numerical magnitude perception characterize the mathematics learning disability developmental dyscalculia (DD), but recent studies suggest the relation stems from inhibitory control demands from incongruent visual cues in the nonsymbolic number comparison task. This study investigated the relation among magnitude perception during differing congruency conditions, executive function, and mathematics achievement measured longitudinally in children (n = 448) from ages 4 to 13. This relation was investigated across achievement groups and as it related to mathematics across the full range of achievement. Only performance on incongruent trials related to achievement. Findings indicate that executive function in a numerical context, beyond magnitude perception or executive function in a non-numerical context, relates to DD and mathematics across a wide range of achievement.
Collapse
|
47
|
Braham EJ, Elliott L, Libertus ME. Using Hierarchical Linear Models to Examine Approximate Number System Acuity: The Role of Trial-Level and Participant-Level Characteristics. Front Psychol 2018; 9:2081. [PMID: 30483169 PMCID: PMC6240605 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2018] [Accepted: 10/09/2018] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The ability to intuitively and quickly compare the number of items in collections without counting is thought to rely on the Approximate Number System (ANS). To assess individual differences in the precision of peoples' ANS representations, researchers often use non-symbolic number comparison tasks in which participants quickly choose the numerically larger of two arrays of dots. However, some researchers debate whether this task actually measures the ability to discriminate approximate numbers or instead measures the ability to discriminate other continuous magnitude dimensions that are often confounded with number (e.g., the total surface area of the dots or the convex hull of the dot arrays). In this study, we used hierarchical linear models (HLMs) to predict 132 adults' accuracy on each trial of a non-symbolic number comparison task from a comprehensive set of trial-level characteristics (including numerosity ratio, surface area, convex hull, and temporal and spatial variations in presentation format) and participant-level controls (including cognitive abilities such as visual-short term memory, working memory, and math ability) in order to gain a more nuanced understanding of how individuals complete this task. Our results indicate that certain trial-level characteristics of the dot arrays contribute to our ability to compare numerosities, yet numerosity ratio, the critical marker of the ANS, remains a highly significant predictor of accuracy above and beyond trial-level characteristics and across individuals with varying levels of math ability and domain-general cognitive abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily J. Braham
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Leanne Elliott
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Melissa E. Libertus
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Growth of symbolic number knowledge accelerates after children understand cardinality. Cognition 2018; 177:69-78. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2017] [Revised: 04/02/2018] [Accepted: 04/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
49
|
Zhang X, Räsänen P, Koponen T, Aunola K, Lerkkanen MK, Nurmi JE. Early Cognitive Precursors of Children's Mathematics Learning Disability and Persistent Low Achievement: A 5-Year Longitudinal Study. Child Dev 2018; 91:7-27. [PMID: 29998603 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Mathematical difficulties have been distinguished as mathematics learning disability (MLD) and persistent low achievement (LA). Based on 1,880 Finnish children who were followed from kindergarten (age 6) to fourth grade, this study examined the early risk factors for MLD and LA. Distinct groups of MLD (6.0% of the sample) and LA (25.7%) children were identified on the basis of their mathematics performance between first and fourth grades with latent class growth modeling. Impairment in the same set of cognitive skills, including language, spatial, and counting skills, was found to underlie MLD and LA. The finding highlights the importance of monitoring mathematical development across the early grades and identifying early cognitive precursors of MLD and LA for screening and intervention efforts.
Collapse
|
50
|
Abstract
This brief report addresses preschoolers' selective sustained attention (SSA) and early numeracy skills and knowledge. Past research indicates that children's attention and early numeracy are positively associated, yet some concerns have emerged about the age appropriateness of tools used to measure preschoolers' SSA. This study used a new measure-the Track-It Task-that demonstrates strong psychometric properties. In total, 31 at-risk preschoolers (Mage = 46.6 months) participated and were assessed on SSA, nonsymbolic quantity discrimination, and symbolic quantitative skills and knowledge. The ability to sustain attention in the face of distractions was positively correlated with preschoolers' verbal counting and one-to-one correspondence, Arabic numeral recognition, and cardinal principle knowledge. SSA was not significantly associated with child age or performance on a memory task. This study provides preliminary evidence that SSA may facilitate the process whereby young children become reliable counters and learn that the symbol system of numbers represents specific quantities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alicia Brueggemann
- Department of Nutrition and Exercise Physiology, College of Human Environmental Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| | - Sara Gable
- Department of Nutrition and Exercise Physiology, College of Human Environmental Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.
| |
Collapse
|