1
|
Kok EM, Niehorster DC, van der Gijp A, Rutgers DR, Auffermann WF, van der Schaaf M, Kester L, van Gog T. The effects of gaze-display feedback on medical students' self-monitoring and learning in radiology. ADVANCES IN HEALTH SCIENCES EDUCATION : THEORY AND PRACTICE 2024; 29:1689-1710. [PMID: 38555550 PMCID: PMC11549167 DOI: 10.1007/s10459-024-10322-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2023] [Accepted: 03/03/2024] [Indexed: 04/02/2024]
Abstract
Self-monitoring is essential for effectively regulating learning, but difficult in visual diagnostic tasks such as radiograph interpretation. Eye-tracking technology can visualize viewing behavior in gaze displays, thereby providing information about visual search and decision-making. We hypothesized that individually adaptive gaze-display feedback improves posttest performance and self-monitoring of medical students who learn to detect nodules in radiographs. We investigated the effects of: (1) Search displays, showing which part of the image was searched by the participant; and (2) Decision displays, showing which parts of the image received prolonged attention in 78 medical students. After a pretest and instruction, participants practiced identifying nodules in 16 cases under search-display, decision-display, or no feedback conditions (n = 26 per condition). A 10-case posttest, without feedback, was administered to assess learning outcomes. After each case, participants provided self-monitoring and confidence judgments. Afterward, participants reported on self-efficacy, perceived competence, feedback use, and perceived usefulness of the feedback. Bayesian analyses showed no benefits of gaze displays for post-test performance, monitoring accuracy (absolute difference between participants' estimated and their actual test performance), completeness of viewing behavior, self-efficacy, and perceived competence. Participants receiving search-displays reported greater feedback utilization than participants receiving decision-displays, and also found the feedback more useful when the gaze data displayed was precise and accurate. As the completeness of search was not related to posttest performance, search displays might not have been sufficiently informative to improve self-monitoring. Information from decision displays was rarely used to inform self-monitoring. Further research should address if and when gaze displays can support learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ellen M Kok
- Department of Education, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Diederick C Niehorster
- Lund University Humanities Lab, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
- Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Anouk van der Gijp
- Department of Radiology, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Dirk R Rutgers
- Department of Radiology, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | | | - Marieke van der Schaaf
- Utrecht Center for Research and Development in Health Professions Education, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Liesbeth Kester
- Department of Education, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Tamara van Gog
- Department of Education, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Anderson BA. Using aversive conditioning with near-real-time feedback to shape eye movements during naturalistic viewing. Behav Res Methods 2021; 53:993-1002. [PMID: 32918167 PMCID: PMC7947016 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-020-01476-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/31/2020] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Strategically shaping patterns of eye movements through training has manifold promising applications, with the potential to improve the speed and efficiency of visual search, improve the ability of humans to extract information from complex displays, and help correct disordered eye movement patterns. However, training how a person moves their eyes when viewing an image or scene is notoriously difficult, with typical approaches relying on explicit instruction and strategy, which have notable limitations. The present study introduces a novel approach to eye movement training using aversive conditioning with near-real-time feedback. Participants viewed indoor scenes (eight scenes presented over 48 trials) with the goal of remembering those scenes for a later memory test. During viewing, saccades meeting specific amplitude and direction criteria probabilistically triggered an aversive electric shock, which was felt within 50 ms after the eliciting eye movement, allowing for a close temporal coupling between an oculomotor behavior and the feedback intended to shape it. Results demonstrate a bias against performing an initial saccade in the direction paired with shock (Experiment 1) or generally of the amplitude paired with shock (Experiment 2), an effect that operates without apparent awareness of the relationship between shocks and saccades, persists into extinction, and generalizes to the viewing of novel images. The present study serves as a proof of concept concerning the implementation of near-real-time feedback in eye movement training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brian A Anderson
- Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, 4235 TAMU, College Station, TX, 77843-4235, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Rieger T, Heilmann L, Manzey D. Visual search behavior and performance in luggage screening: effects of time pressure, automation aid, and target expectancy. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2021; 6:12. [PMID: 33630179 PMCID: PMC7907401 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-021-00280-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2020] [Accepted: 02/12/2021] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
Visual inspection of luggage using X-ray technology at airports is a time-sensitive task that is often supported by automated systems to increase performance and reduce workload. The present study evaluated how time pressure and automation support influence visual search behavior and performance in a simulated luggage screening task. Moreover, we also investigated how target expectancy (i.e., targets appearing in a target-often location or not) influenced performance and visual search behavior. We used a paradigm where participants used the mouse to uncover a portion of the screen which allowed us to track how much of the stimulus participants uncovered prior to their decision. Participants were randomly assigned to either a high (5-s time per trial) or a low (10-s time per trial) time-pressure condition. In half of the trials, participants were supported by an automated diagnostic aid (85% reliability) in deciding whether a threat item was present. Moreover, within each half, in target-present trials, targets appeared in a predictable location (i.e., 70% of targets appeared in the same quadrant of the image) to investigate effects of target expectancy. The results revealed better detection performance with low time pressure and faster response times with high time pressure. There was an overall negative effect of automation support because the automation was only moderately reliable. Participants also uncovered a smaller amount of the stimulus under high time pressure in target-absent trials. Target expectancy of target location improved accuracy, speed, and the amount of uncovered space needed for the search.Significance Statement Luggage screening is a safety-critical real-world visual search task which often has to be done under time pressure. The present research found that time pressure compromises performance and increases the risk to miss critical items even with automation support. Moreover, even highly reliable automated support may not improve performance if it does not exceed the manual capabilities of the human screener. Lastly, the present research also showed that heuristic search strategies (e.g., areas where targets appear more often) seem to guide attention also in luggage screening.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tobias Rieger
- Department of Psychology and Ergonomics, Chair of Work, Engineering, and Organizational Psychology, F7, Technische Universität Berlin, Marchstr. 12, 10587, Berlin, Germany.
| | - Lydia Heilmann
- Department of Psychology and Ergonomics, Chair of Work, Engineering, and Organizational Psychology, F7, Technische Universität Berlin, Marchstr. 12, 10587, Berlin, Germany
| | - Dietrich Manzey
- Department of Psychology and Ergonomics, Chair of Work, Engineering, and Organizational Psychology, F7, Technische Universität Berlin, Marchstr. 12, 10587, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Abstract
Cancer diagnosis frequently relies on the interpretation of medical images such as chest X-rays and mammography. This process is error prone; misdiagnoses can reach a rate of 15% or higher. Of particular interest are false negatives-tumors that are present but missed. Previous research has identified several perceptual and attentional problems underlying inaccurate perception of these images. But how might these problems be reduced? The psychological literature has shown that presenting multiple, duplicate images can improve performance. Here we explored whether redundant image presentation can improve target detection in simulated X-ray images, by presenting four identical or similar images concurrently. Displays with redundant images, including duplicates of the same image, showed reduced false-negative rates, compared with displays with a single image. This effect held both when the target's prevalence rate was high and when it was low. Eye tracking showed that fixating on two or more images in the redundant condition speeded target detection and prolonged search, and that the latter effect was the key to reducing false negatives. The redundancy gain may result from both perceptual enhancement and an increase in the search quitting threshold.
Collapse
|
5
|
Affiliation(s)
| | - Stephen R. Mitroff
- Department of Psychology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wu CC, Wolfe JM. Eye Movements in Medical Image Perception: A Selective Review of Past, Present and Future. Vision (Basel) 2019; 3:E32. [PMID: 31735833 PMCID: PMC6802791 DOI: 10.3390/vision3020032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2019] [Revised: 06/09/2019] [Accepted: 06/18/2019] [Indexed: 12/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The eye movements of experts, reading medical images, have been studied for many years. Unlike topics such as face perception, medical image perception research needs to cope with substantial, qualitative changes in the stimuli under study due to dramatic advances in medical imaging technology. For example, little is known about how radiologists search through 3D volumes of image data because they simply did not exist when earlier eye tracking studies were performed. Moreover, improvements in the affordability and portability of modern eye trackers make other, new studies practical. Here, we review some uses of eye movements in the study of medical image perception with an emphasis on newer work. We ask how basic research on scene perception relates to studies of medical 'scenes' and we discuss how tracking experts' eyes may provide useful insights for medical education and screening efficiency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Chien Wu
- Visual Attention Lab, Department of Surgery, Brigham & Women’s Hospital, 65 Landsdowne St, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Jeremy M. Wolfe
- Visual Attention Lab, Department of Surgery, Brigham & Women’s Hospital, 65 Landsdowne St, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
- Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Donnelly N, Muhl-Richardson A, Godwin HJ, Cave KR. Using Eye Movements to Understand how Security Screeners Search for Threats in X-Ray Baggage. Vision (Basel) 2019; 3:vision3020024. [PMID: 31735825 PMCID: PMC6802782 DOI: 10.3390/vision3020024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2019] [Revised: 05/17/2019] [Accepted: 06/01/2019] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
There has been an increasing drive to understand failures in searches for weapons and explosives in X-ray baggage screening. Tracking eye movements during the search has produced new insights into the guidance of attention during the search, and the identification of targets once they are fixated. Here, we review the eye-movement literature that has emerged on this front over the last fifteen years, including a discussion of the problems that real-world searchers face when trying to detect targets that could do serious harm to people and infrastructure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nick Donnelly
- Department of Psychology, Liverpool Hope University, Liverpool L16 9JD, UK
| | | | - Hayward J. Godwin
- Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton SO17 1BJ, UK
- Correspondence:
| | - Kyle R. Cave
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kramer MR, Porfido CL, Mitroff SR. Evaluation of strategies to train visual search performance in professional populations. Curr Opin Psychol 2019; 29:113-118. [PMID: 30731261 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2018] [Revised: 12/07/2018] [Accepted: 01/04/2019] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Visual search, the act of finding targets amongst distractors, is central to many professions with life-or-death implications including aviation security, radiology, lifeguarding, military, and more. As such, every effort should be taken to improve visual search performance. One potential path to improvement is to ensure that workforces are optimally trained. Broadly, there are three general components to train: (1) specific use of the machinery and user interface (i.e. 'knobology'), (2) target and distractor identification, and (3) search strategy. The current review considers the cognitive psychology aspects of these three components; each is evaluated in light of short-term and long-term training goals, as well as profession-specific constraints.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michelle R Kramer
- The George Washington University, Department of Psychology, 2125 G Street NW, Washington, DC 20052, United States.
| | - Courtney L Porfido
- The George Washington University, Department of Psychology, 2125 G Street NW, Washington, DC 20052, United States
| | - Stephen R Mitroff
- The George Washington University, Department of Psychology, 2125 G Street NW, Washington, DC 20052, United States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Drew T, Williams LH. Simple eye-movement feedback during visual search is not helpful. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2017; 2:44. [PMID: 29214205 PMCID: PMC5698384 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-017-0082-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2017] [Accepted: 10/06/2017] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Searching for targets in the visual world, or visual search, is something we all do every day. We frequently make 'false-negative' errors, wherein we erroneously conclude a target was absent when one was, in fact, present. These sorts of errors can have tremendous costs, as when signs of cancers are missed in diagnostic radiology. Prior research has characterized the cause of many of these errors as being due to failure to completely search the area where targets may be present; indeed, roughly one-third of chest nodules missed in lung cancer screening are never fixated (Drew, Võ, Olwal, Jacobson, Seltzer and Wolfe, Journal of Vision 13:3, 2013). This suggests that observers do not have a good representation of what areas have and have not been searched prior to declaring an area target free. Therefore, in six experiments, we sought to examine the utility of reducing the uncertainty with respect to what areas had been examined via online eye-tracking feedback. We hypothesized that providing information about what areas had or had not been examined would lead to lower rates of false negatives or more efficient search, namely faster response times with no cost on target detection accuracy. Neither of these predictions held true. Over six experiments, online eye-tracking feedback did not yield any reliable performance benefits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Trafton Drew
- Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 380 S 1530 E Beh S 1003, Salt Lake City, UT 84112 USA
| | - Lauren H Williams
- Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 380 S 1530 E Beh S 1003, Salt Lake City, UT 84112 USA
| |
Collapse
|