1
|
Shestopalova LB, Petropavlovskaia EA, Salikova DA, Semenova VV. Temporal integration of sound motion: Motion-onset response and perception. Hear Res 2024; 441:108922. [PMID: 38043403 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2023.108922] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2023] [Revised: 11/14/2023] [Accepted: 11/20/2023] [Indexed: 12/05/2023]
Abstract
The purpose of our study was to estimate the time interval required for integrating the acoustical changes related to sound motion using both psychophysical and EEG measures. Healthy listeners performed direction identification tasks under dichotic conditions in the delayed-motion paradigm. Minimal audible movement angle (MAMA) has been measured over the range of velocities from 60 to 360 deg/s. We also measured minimal duration of motion, at which the listeners could identify its direction. EEG was recorded in the same group of subjects during passive listening. Motion onset responses (MOR) were analyzed. MAMA increased linearly with motion velocity. Minimum audible angle (MAA) calculated from this linear function was about 2 deg. For higher velocities of the delayed motion, we found 2- to 3-fold better spatial resolution than the one previously reported for motion starting at the sound onset. The time required for optimal discrimination of motion direction was about 34 ms. The main finding of our study was that both direction identification time obtained in the behavioral task and cN1 latency behaved like hyperbolic functions of the sound's velocity. Direction identification time decreased asymptotically to 8 ms, which was considered minimal integration time for the instantaneous shift detection. Peak latency of cN1 also decreased with increasing velocity and asymptotically approached 137 ms. This limit corresponded to the latency of response to the instantaneous sound shift and was 37 ms later than the latency of the sound-onset response. The direction discrimination time (34 ms) was of the same magnitude as the additional time required for motion processing to be reflected in the MOR potential. Thus, MOR latency can be viewed as a neurophysiological index of temporal integration. Based on the findings obtained, we may assume that no measurable MOR would be evoked by slowly moving stimuli as they would reach their MAMAs in a time longer than the optimal integration time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lidia B Shestopalova
- Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Makarova emb., 6, 199034, Saint Petersburg, Russia.
| | | | - Diana A Salikova
- Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Makarova emb., 6, 199034, Saint Petersburg, Russia
| | - Varvara V Semenova
- Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Makarova emb., 6, 199034, Saint Petersburg, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Park WJ, Fine I. The perception of auditory motion in sighted and early blind individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2023; 120:e2310156120. [PMID: 38015842 PMCID: PMC10710053 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2310156120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2023] [Accepted: 10/29/2023] [Indexed: 11/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Motion perception is a fundamental sensory task that plays a critical evolutionary role. In vision, motion processing is classically described using a motion energy model with spatiotemporally nonseparable filters suited for capturing the smooth continuous changes in spatial position over time afforded by moving objects. However, it is still not clear whether the filters underlying auditory motion discrimination are also continuous motion detectors or infer motion from comparing discrete sound locations over time (spatiotemporally separable). We used a psychophysical reverse correlation paradigm, where participants discriminated the direction of a motion signal in the presence of spatiotemporal noise, to determine whether the filters underlying auditory motion discrimination were spatiotemporally separable or nonseparable. We then examined whether these auditory motion filters were altered as a result of early blindness. We found that both sighted and early blind individuals have separable filters. However, early blind individuals show increased sensitivity to auditory motion, with reduced susceptibility to noise and filters that were more accurate in detecting motion onsets/offsets. Model simulations suggest that this reliance on separable filters is optimal given the limited spatial resolution of auditory input.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Woon Ju Park
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA98195
| | - Ione Fine
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA98195
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Fine I, Park WJ. Do you hear what I see? How do early blind individuals experience object motion? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20210460. [PMID: 36511418 PMCID: PMC9745882 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0460] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2022] [Accepted: 09/13/2022] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
One of the most important tasks for 3D vision is tracking the movement of objects in space. The ability of early blind individuals to understand motion in the environment from noisy and unreliable auditory information is an impressive example of cortical adaptation that is only just beginning to be understood. Here, we compare visual and auditory motion processing, and discuss the effect of early blindness on the perception of auditory motion. Blindness leads to cross-modal recruitment of the visual motion area hMT+ for auditory motion processing. Meanwhile, the planum temporale, associated with auditory motion in sighted individuals, shows reduced selectivity for auditory motion. We discuss how this dramatic shift in the cortical basis of motion processing might influence the perceptual experience of motion in early blind individuals. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'New approaches to 3D vision'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ione Fine
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195-1525, USA
| | - Woon Ju Park
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195-1525, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Bertonati G, Amadeo MB, Campus C, Gori M. Auditory speed processing in sighted and blind individuals. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0257676. [PMID: 34551010 PMCID: PMC8457492 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0257676] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2021] [Accepted: 09/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Multisensory experience is crucial for developing a coherent perception of the world. In this context, vision and audition are essential tools to scaffold spatial and temporal representations, respectively. Since speed encompasses both space and time, investigating this dimension in blindness allows deepening the relationship between sensory modalities and the two representation domains. In the present study, we hypothesized that visual deprivation influences the use of spatial and temporal cues underlying acoustic speed perception. To this end, ten early blind and ten blindfolded sighted participants performed a speed discrimination task in which spatial, temporal, or both cues were available to infer moving sounds' velocity. The results indicated that both sighted and early blind participants preferentially relied on temporal cues to determine stimuli speed, by following an assumption that identified as faster those sounds with a shorter duration. However, in some cases, this temporal assumption produces a misperception of the stimulus speed that negatively affected participants' performance. Interestingly, early blind participants were more influenced by this misleading temporal assumption than sighted controls, resulting in a stronger impairment in the speed discrimination performance. These findings demonstrate that the absence of visual experience in early life increases the auditory system's preference for the time domain and, consequentially, affects the perception of speed through audition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giorgia Bertonati
- Unit for Visually Impaired People (U-VIP), Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
- Department of Informatics, Bioengineering, Robotics and Systems Engineering (DIBRIS), Università degli Studi di Genova, Genova, Italy
| | - Maria Bianca Amadeo
- Unit for Visually Impaired People (U-VIP), Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
| | - Claudio Campus
- Unit for Visually Impaired People (U-VIP), Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
| | - Monica Gori
- Unit for Visually Impaired People (U-VIP), Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Auditory motion perception emerges from successive sound localizations integrated over time. Sci Rep 2019; 9:16437. [PMID: 31712688 PMCID: PMC6848124 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-52742-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2019] [Accepted: 10/11/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans rely on auditory information to estimate the path of moving sound sources. But unlike in vision, the existence of motion-sensitive mechanisms in audition is still open to debate. Psychophysical studies indicate that auditory motion perception emerges from successive localization, but existing models fail to predict experimental results. However, these models do not account for any temporal integration. We propose a new model tracking motion using successive localization snapshots but integrated over time. This model is derived from psychophysical experiments on the upper limit for circular auditory motion perception (UL), defined as the speed above which humans no longer identify the direction of sounds spinning around them. Our model predicts ULs measured with different stimuli using solely static localization cues. The temporal integration blurs these localization cues rendering them unreliable at high speeds, which results in the UL. Our findings indicate that auditory motion perception does not require motion-sensitive mechanisms.
Collapse
|
6
|
Anobile G, Guerrini G, Burr DC, Monti M, Del Lucchese B, Cicchini GM. Spontaneous perception of numerosity in pre-school children. Proc Biol Sci 2019; 286:20191245. [PMID: 31288698 PMCID: PMC6650702 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1245] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
There is strong evidence that humans can make rough estimates of the numerosity of a set of items, almost from birth. However, as numerosity covaries with many non-numerical variables, the idea of a direct number sense has been challenged. Here we applied two different psychophysical paradigms to demonstrate the spontaneous perception of numerosity in a cohort of young pre-school children. The results of both tasks showed that even at that early developmental stage, humans spontaneously base the perceptual choice on numerosity, rather than on area or density. Precision in one of these tasks predicted mathematical abilities. The results reinforce strongly the idea of a primary number sense and provide further evidence linking mathematical skills to the sensory precision of the spontaneous number sense, rather than to mechanisms involved in handling explicit numerosity judgements or extensive exposure to mathematical teaching.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Anobile
- 1 Department of Developmental Neuroscience, IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation , Pisa , Italy
| | - G Guerrini
- 2 Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, University of Florence , Florence , Italy
| | - D C Burr
- 2 Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, University of Florence , Florence , Italy.,3 Institute of Neuroscience, National Research Council , Pisa , Italy
| | - M Monti
- 2 Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, University of Florence , Florence , Italy
| | - B Del Lucchese
- 1 Department of Developmental Neuroscience, IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation , Pisa , Italy
| | - G M Cicchini
- 3 Institute of Neuroscience, National Research Council , Pisa , Italy
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Senna I, Parise CV, Ernst MO. Modulation frequency as a cue for auditory speed perception. Proc Biol Sci 2018; 284:rspb.2017.0673. [PMID: 28701558 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2017.0673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2017] [Accepted: 06/16/2017] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Unlike vision, the mechanisms underlying auditory motion perception are poorly understood. Here we describe an auditory motion illusion revealing a novel cue to auditory speed perception: the temporal frequency of amplitude modulation (AM-frequency), typical for rattling sounds. Naturally, corrugated objects sliding across each other generate rattling sounds whose AM-frequency tends to directly correlate with speed. We found that AM-frequency modulates auditory speed perception in a highly systematic fashion: moving sounds with higher AM-frequency are perceived as moving faster than sounds with lower AM-frequency. Even more interestingly, sounds with higher AM-frequency also induce stronger motion aftereffects. This reveals the existence of specialized neural mechanisms for auditory motion perception, which are sensitive to AM-frequency. Thus, in spatial hearing, the brain successfully capitalizes on the AM-frequency of rattling sounds to estimate the speed of moving objects. This tightly parallels previous findings in motion vision, where spatio-temporal frequency of moving displays systematically affects both speed perception and the magnitude of the motion aftereffects. Such an analogy with vision suggests that motion detection may rely on canonical computations, with similar neural mechanisms shared across the different modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene Senna
- Applied Cognitive Psychology, Ulm University, 89081 Ulm, Germany
| | - Cesare V Parise
- Oculus Research, Redmond, WA 98052, USA.,CITEC, Bielefeld University, 33619 Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Marc O Ernst
- Applied Cognitive Psychology, Ulm University, 89081 Ulm, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Freeman TCA, Culling JF, Akeroyd MA, Brimijoin WO. Auditory compensation for head rotation is incomplete. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2017; 43:371-380. [PMID: 27841453 PMCID: PMC5289217 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000321] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2016] [Revised: 08/24/2016] [Accepted: 09/01/2016] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Hearing is confronted by a similar problem to vision when the observer moves. The image motion that is created remains ambiguous until the observer knows the velocity of eye and/or head. One way the visual system solves this problem is to use motor commands, proprioception, and vestibular information. These "extraretinal signals" compensate for self-movement, converting image motion into head-centered coordinates, although not always perfectly. We investigated whether the auditory system also transforms coordinates by examining the degree of compensation for head rotation when judging a moving sound. Real-time recordings of head motion were used to change the "movement gain" relating head movement to source movement across a loudspeaker array. We then determined psychophysically the gain that corresponded to a perceptually stationary source. Experiment 1 showed that the gain was small and positive for a wide range of trained head speeds. Hence, listeners perceived a stationary source as moving slightly opposite to the head rotation, in much the same way that observers see stationary visual objects move against a smooth pursuit eye movement. Experiment 2 showed the degree of compensation remained the same for sounds presented at different azimuths, although the precision of performance declined when the sound was eccentric. We discuss two possible explanations for incomplete compensation, one based on differences in the accuracy of signals encoding image motion and self-movement and one concerning statistical optimization that sacrifices accuracy for precision. We then consider the degree to which such explanations can be applied to auditory motion perception in moving listeners. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Michael A Akeroyd
- Medical Research Council Institute of Hearing Research, University of Nottingham
| | - W Owen Brimijoin
- Medical Research Council/Chief Scientist Office Institute of Hearing Research-Scottish Section, Glasgow Royal Infirmary
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Neuhoff JG. Looming sounds are perceived as faster than receding sounds. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2016; 1:15. [PMID: 28180166 PMCID: PMC5256440 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-016-0017-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2016] [Accepted: 09/23/2016] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Each year thousands of people are killed by looming motor vehicles. Throughout our evolutionary history looming objects have posed a threat to survival and perceptual systems have evolved unique solutions to confront these environmental challenges. Vision provides an accurate representation of time-to-contact with a looming object and usually allows us to interact successfully with the object if required. However, audition functions as a warning system and yields an anticipatory representation of arrival time, indicating that the object has arrived when it is still some distance away. The bias provides a temporal margin of safety that allows more time to initiate defensive actions. In two studies this bias was shown to influence the perception of the speed of looming and receding sound sources. Listeners heard looming and receding sound sources and judged how fast they were moving. Listeners perceived the speed of looming sounds as faster than that of equivalent receding sounds. Listeners also showed better discrimination of the speed of looming sounds than receding sounds. Finally, close sounds were perceived as faster than distant sounds. The results suggest a prioritization of the perception of the speed of looming and receding sounds that mirrors the level of threat posed by moving objects in the environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John G Neuhoff
- Department of Psychology, The College of Wooster, Wooster, OH 44691 USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Locke SM, Leung J, Carlile S. Sensitivity to Auditory Velocity Contrast. Sci Rep 2016; 6:27725. [PMID: 27291488 PMCID: PMC4904411 DOI: 10.1038/srep27725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2015] [Accepted: 05/23/2016] [Indexed: 02/02/2023] Open
Abstract
A natural auditory scene often contains sound moving at varying velocities. Using a velocity contrast paradigm, we compared sensitivity to velocity changes between continuous and discontinuous trajectories. Subjects compared the velocities of two stimulus intervals that moved along a single trajectory, with and without a 1 second inter stimulus interval (ISI). We found thresholds were threefold larger for velocity increases in the instantaneous velocity change condition, as compared to instantaneous velocity decreases or thresholds for the delayed velocity transition condition. This result cannot be explained by the current static "snapshot" model of auditory motion perception and suggest a continuous process where the percept of velocity is influenced by previous history of stimulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shannon M. Locke
- School of Medical Sciences, University of Sydney, NSW 2006 Australia
- Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Johahn Leung
- School of Medical Sciences, University of Sydney, NSW 2006 Australia
| | - Simon Carlile
- School of Medical Sciences, University of Sydney, NSW 2006 Australia
- Starkey Hearing Research Center, 2110 Shattuck st#408, Berkeley, CA 94704 USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Carlile S, Leung J. The Perception of Auditory Motion. Trends Hear 2016; 20:2331216516644254. [PMID: 27094029 PMCID: PMC4871213 DOI: 10.1177/2331216516644254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2015] [Revised: 03/22/2016] [Accepted: 03/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The growing availability of efficient and relatively inexpensive virtual auditory display technology has provided new research platforms to explore the perception of auditory motion. At the same time, deployment of these technologies in command and control as well as in entertainment roles is generating an increasing need to better understand the complex processes underlying auditory motion perception. This is a particularly challenging processing feat because it involves the rapid deconvolution of the relative change in the locations of sound sources produced by rotational and translations of the head in space (self-motion) to enable the perception of actual source motion. The fact that we perceive our auditory world to be stable despite almost continual movement of the head demonstrates the efficiency and effectiveness of this process. This review examines the acoustical basis of auditory motion perception and a wide range of psychophysical, electrophysiological, and cortical imaging studies that have probed the limits and possible mechanisms underlying this perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simon Carlile
- School of Medical Sciences, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia Starkey Hearing Research Center, Berkeley, CA, USA
| | - Johahn Leung
- School of Medical Sciences, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Sankaran N, Leung J, Carlile S. Effects of virtual speaker density and room reverberation on spatiotemporal thresholds of audio-visual motion coherence. PLoS One 2014; 9:e108437. [PMID: 25269061 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108437] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2014] [Accepted: 08/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study examined the effects of spatial sound-source density and reverberation on the spatiotemporal window for audio-visual motion coherence. Three different acoustic stimuli were generated in Virtual Auditory Space: two acoustically "dry" stimuli via the measurement of anechoic head-related impulse responses recorded at either 1° or 5° spatial intervals (Experiment 1), and a reverberant stimulus rendered from binaural room impulse responses recorded at 5° intervals in situ in order to capture reverberant acoustics in addition to head-related cues (Experiment 2). A moving visual stimulus with invariant localization cues was generated by sequentially activating LED's along the same radial path as the virtual auditory motion. Stimuli were presented at 25°/s, 50°/s and 100°/s with a random spatial offset between audition and vision. In a 2AFC task, subjects made a judgment of the leading modality (auditory or visual). No significant differences were observed in the spatial threshold based on the point of subjective equivalence (PSE) or the slope of psychometric functions (β) across all three acoustic conditions. Additionally, both the PSE and β did not significantly differ across velocity, suggesting a fixed spatial window of audio-visual separation. Findings suggest that there was no loss in spatial information accompanying the reduction in spatial cues and reverberation levels tested, and establish a perceptual measure for assessing the veracity of motion generated from discrete locations and in echoic environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Narayan Sankaran
- Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory, School of Medical Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Johahn Leung
- Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory, School of Medical Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Simon Carlile
- Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory, School of Medical Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|