1
|
Flanagan T, Georgiou NC, Scassellati B, Kushnir T. School-age children are more skeptical of inaccurate robots than adults. Cognition 2024; 249:105814. [PMID: 38763071 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105814] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2023] [Revised: 05/10/2024] [Accepted: 05/13/2024] [Indexed: 05/21/2024]
Abstract
We expect children to learn new words, skills, and ideas from various technologies. When learning from humans, children prefer people who are reliable and trustworthy, yet children also forgive people's occasional mistakes. Are the dynamics of children learning from technologies, which can also be unreliable, similar to learning from humans? We tackle this question by focusing on early childhood, an age at which children are expected to master foundational academic skills. In this project, 168 4-7-year-old children (Study 1) and 168 adults (Study 2) played a word-guessing game with either a human or robot. The partner first gave a sequence of correct answers, but then followed this with a sequence of wrong answers, with a reaction following each one. Reactions varied by condition, either expressing an accident, an accident marked with an apology, or an unhelpful intention. We found that older children were less trusting than both younger children and adults and were even more skeptical after errors. Trust decreased most rapidly when errors were intentional, but only children (and especially older children) outright rejected help from intentionally unhelpful partners. As an exception to this general trend, older children maintained their trust for longer when a robot (but not a human) apologized for its mistake. Our work suggests that educational technology design cannot be one size fits all but rather must account for developmental changes in children's learning goals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa Flanagan
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, 417 Chapel Drive, Durham, NC 27701, United States of America.
| | - Nicholas C Georgiou
- Department of Computer Science, Yale University, 51 Prospect Street, New Haven, CT 06511, United States of America
| | - Brian Scassellati
- Department of Computer Science, Yale University, 51 Prospect Street, New Haven, CT 06511, United States of America
| | - Tamar Kushnir
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, 417 Chapel Drive, Durham, NC 27701, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Vasil J, Capoot C, Tomasello M. Effects of group entitativity on young English-speaking children's interpretation of inclusive We. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0306556. [PMID: 38980888 PMCID: PMC11232990 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0306556] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/12/2023] [Accepted: 06/19/2024] [Indexed: 07/11/2024] Open
Abstract
The pronoun we can be used to refer to various collections of people depending on various pragmatic factors. This article reports the results of two online experiments that investigated children's interpretation of inclusive we, in which the child-listener is part of the intended referent of we. 128 2- and 4-year-olds collaborated with three partners in a coloring task. Before they played together, one partner informed participants that, e.g., "we can color!" Participants had their own markers and had to choose to how many partners to distribute (virtual) markers. In the first experiment, the partners appeared more like an aggregation of individuals than a collaborative group. The second experiment flipped this so that the partners appeared more like a collaborative group. Contrary to expectations, there was relatively little evidence for development in children's interpretation of we. Additionally, participants did not sharply distinguish their interpretations of we from those of we both or we all. Rather, participants were more likely to choose group interpretations when contextual cues indicated that their partners were a collaborative group than an aggregation of individuals. Interestingly, this interpretational distinction was sharpest for the pragmatically ambiguous we, compared to the relatively unambiguous we both and we all. These results are informative about the kinds of cues that shape young children's interpretation of pragmatically ambiguous pronominal reference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jared Vasil
- Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
| | - Camryn Capoot
- Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
| | - Michael Tomasello
- Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
- Max Planck Institute of Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Chuey A, Boyce V, Cao A, Frank MC. Conducting Developmental Research Online vs. In-Person: A Meta-Analysis. Open Mind (Camb) 2024; 8:795-808. [PMID: 38957506 PMCID: PMC11219065 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00147] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2023] [Accepted: 05/01/2024] [Indexed: 07/04/2024] Open
Abstract
An increasing number of psychological experiments with children are being conducted using online platforms, in part due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Individual replications have compared the findings of particular experiments online and in-person, but the general effect of data collection method on data collected from children is still unknown. Therefore, the goal of the current meta-analysis is to estimate the average difference in effect size for developmental studies conducted online compared to the same studies conducted in-person. Our pre-registered analysis includes 211 effect sizes calculated from 30 papers with 3282 children, ranging in age from four months to six years. The estimated effect size for studies conducted online was slightly smaller than for their counterparts conducted in-person, a difference of d = -.05, but this difference was not significant, 95% CI = [-.17, .07]. We examined several potential moderators of the effect of online testing, including the role of dependent measure (looking vs verbal), online study method (moderated vs unmoderated), and age, but none of these were significant. The literature to date thus suggests-on average-small differences in results between in-person and online experimentation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron Chuey
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Veronica Boyce
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Anjie Cao
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Michael C. Frank
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Nguyen DKL, Fitzpatrick N, Floccia C. Adapting language development research paradigms to online testing: Data from preferential looking, word learning and vocabulary assessment in toddlers. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2024:1-33. [PMID: 38433469 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/05/2024]
Abstract
During the recent pandemic, it became necessary to adapt lab-based studies to online experiments. To investigate the impact of online testing on the quality of data, we focus on three paradigms widely used in infant research: a word recognition task using the Intermodal Preferential Looking Paradigm, a word learning task using the Switch task, and a language assessment tool (WinG) where children identify a target word amongst a set of picture cards. Our results for synchronous and asynchronous studies provide support for the robustness of online testing. In Experiment 1, robust word recognition was found in 24-month-old toddlers. In Experiment 2, 17-month-old infants consistently learned a new word. Finally, Experiment 3 demonstrated that 19- to 26-month-old children performed well on a language assessment test administered online. Overall, effect sizes or language scores were found to be higher than in lab-based studies. These experiments point to promising possibilities for reaching out to families around the world.
Collapse
|
5
|
Lapidow E, Bonawitz E. What's in the Box? Preschoolers Consider Ambiguity, Expected Value, and Information for Future Decisions in Explore-Exploit Tasks. Open Mind (Camb) 2023; 7:855-878. [PMID: 37946850 PMCID: PMC10631797 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/02/2023] [Accepted: 09/16/2023] [Indexed: 11/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Self-directed exploration in childhood appears driven by a desire to resolve uncertainties in order to learn more about the world. However, in adult decision-making, the choice to explore new information rather than exploit what is already known takes many factors beyond uncertainty (such as expected utilities and costs) into account. The evidence for whether young children are sensitive to complex, contextual factors in making exploration decisions is limited and mixed. Here, we investigate whether modifying uncertain options influences explore-exploit behavior in preschool-aged children (48-68 months). Over the course of three experiments, we manipulate uncertain options' ambiguity, expected value, and potential to improve epistemic state for future exploration in a novel forced-choice design. We find evidence that young children are influenced by each of these factors, suggesting that early, self-directed exploration involves sophisticated, context-sensitive decision-making under uncertainty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth Lapidow
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Blankenship TL, Kibbe MM. "Plan chunking" expands 3-year-olds' ability to complete multiple-step plans. Child Dev 2023; 94:1330-1339. [PMID: 37092570 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2022] [Revised: 02/15/2023] [Accepted: 02/17/2023] [Indexed: 04/25/2023]
Abstract
The ability to use knowledge to guide the completion of goals is a critical cognitive skill, but 3-year-olds struggle to complete goals that require multiple steps. This study asked whether 3-year-olds could benefit from "plan chunking" to complete multistep goals. Thirty-two U.S. children (range = 35.75-46.59 months; 18 girls; 9 white, 3 mixed race, 20 unknown; tested between July 2020 and April 2021) were asked to complete "treasure maps," retrieving four colored map pieces by pressing specific buttons on a "rainbow box." Children completed more of the four-step sequence correctly when the steps were presented in a way that encouraged chunking the steps into pairs. These findings suggest a potential mechanism supporting memory-guided planning abilities in early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Melissa M Kibbe
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Hammack J, Sharma M, Riera-Gomez L, Gvirts HZ, Wilcox T. When I move, you move: Associations between automatic and person-coded measures of infant-mother synchrony during free-play using virtual in-home data collection. Infant Behav Dev 2023; 72:101869. [PMID: 37562176 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101869] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2023] [Revised: 07/27/2023] [Accepted: 07/29/2023] [Indexed: 08/12/2023]
Abstract
The relationship between movement synchrony and global scores of behavioral synchrony were investigated during a naturalistic free-play between 33 infants (ages 12- to 24-months) and their mothers using a video-conferencing platform. We assessed movement synchrony by applying an automatic tool, motion-energy analysis (MEA), to the obtained video data. Dyadic movement synchrony was associated with higher levels of maternal sensitivity, infant involvement, dyadic reciprocity, and a child-led interaction pattern. This demonstrates the feasibility of using MEA as an automatic tool for assessing movement synchrony in mother-infant dyads and its application for investigating naturalistic at-home free play sessions in a remote setting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mini Sharma
- Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ariel University, Israel
| | | | - Hila Z Gvirts
- Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ariel University, Israel
| | - Teresa Wilcox
- Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Bursalıoğlu A, Michalak A, Guy MW. Intersensory redundancy impedes face recognition in 12-month-old infants. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1210132. [PMID: 37529309 PMCID: PMC10389088 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1210132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2023] [Accepted: 06/26/2023] [Indexed: 08/03/2023] Open
Abstract
This study examined the role of intersensory redundancy on 12-month-old infants' attention to and processing of face stimuli. Two experiments were conducted. In Experiment 1, 72 12-month-olds were tested using an online platform called Lookit. Infants were familiarized with two videos of an actor reciting a children's story presented simultaneously. A soundtrack either matched one of the videos (experimental condition) or neither of the videos (control condition). Visual-paired comparison (VPC) trials were completed to measure looking preferences for the faces presented synchronously and asynchronously during familiarization and for novel faces. Neither group displayed looking preferences during the VPC trials. It is possible that the complexity of the familiarization phase made the modality-specific face properties (i.e., facial characteristics and configuration) difficult to process. In Experiment 2, 56 12-month-old infants were familiarized with the video of only one actor presented either synchronously or asynchronously with the soundtrack. Following familiarization, participants completed a VPC procedure including the familiar face and a novel face. Results from Experiment 2 showed that infants in the synchronous condition paid more attention during familiarization than infants in the asynchronous condition. Infants in the asynchronous condition demonstrated recognition of the familiar face. These findings suggest that the competing face stimuli in the Experiment 1 were too complex for the facial characteristics to be processed. The procedure in Experiment 2 led to increased processing of the face in the asynchronous presentation. These results indicate that intersensory redundancy in the presentation of synchronous audiovisual faces is very salient, discouraging the processing of modality-specific visual properties. This research contributes to the understanding of face processing in multimodal contexts, which have been understudied, although a great deal of naturalistic face exposure occurs multimodally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aslı Bursalıoğlu
- Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Shore MJ, Bukovsky DL, Pinheiro SGV, Hancock BM, Liptrot EM, Kuhlmeier VA. A survey on the challenges, limitations, and opportunities of online testing of infants and young children during the COVID-19 pandemic: using our experiences to improve future practices. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1160203. [PMID: 37384169 PMCID: PMC10296766 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1160203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2023] [Accepted: 05/18/2023] [Indexed: 06/30/2023] Open
Abstract
In developmental psychology, the widespread adoption of new methods for testing children does not typically occur over a matter of months. Yet, the COVID-19 pandemic and its associated social distancing requirements created a sudden need among many research groups to use a new method with which they had little or no experience: online testing. Here, we report results from a survey of 159 researchers detailing their early experiences with online testing. The survey approach allowed us to create a general picture of the challenges, limitations, and opportunities of online research, and it identified aspects of the methods that have the potential to impact interpretations of findings. We use the survey results to present considerations to improve online research practices.
Collapse
|
10
|
Ahl RE, Hannan K, Amir D, Baker A, Sheskin M, McAuliffe K. Tokens of virtue: Replicating incentivized measures of children’s prosocial behavior with online methods and virtual resources. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2023. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101313] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/05/2023]
|
11
|
Orena AJ, Mader AS, Werker JF. Learning to Recognize Unfamiliar Voices: An Online Study With 12- and 24-Month-Olds. Front Psychol 2022; 13:874411. [PMID: 35558718 PMCID: PMC9088808 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.874411] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2022] [Accepted: 03/18/2022] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Young infants are attuned to the indexical properties of speech: they can recognize highly familiar voices and distinguish them from unfamiliar voices. Less is known about how and when infants start to recognize unfamiliar voices, and to map them to faces. This skill is particularly challenging when portions of the speaker’s face are occluded, as is the case with masking. Here, we examined voice−face recognition abilities in infants 12 and 24 months of age. Using the online Lookit platform, children saw and heard four different speakers produce words with sonorous phonemes (high talker information), and words with phonemes that are less sonorous (low talker information). Infants aged 24 months, but not 12 months, were able to learn to link the voices to partially occluded faces of unfamiliar speakers, and only when the words were produced with high talker information. These results reveal that 24-month-old infants can encode and retrieve indexical properties of an unfamiliar speaker’s voice, and they can access this information even when visual access to the speaker’s mouth is blocked.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adriel John Orena
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.,Department of Evaluation and Research Services, Fraser Health Authority, Surrey, BC, Canada
| | - Asia Sotera Mader
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | - Janet F Werker
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Zaadnoordijk L, Cusack R. Online testing in developmental science: A guide to design and implementation. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2022; 62:93-125. [PMID: 35249687 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2022.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
At present, most developmental psychology experiments use participants from a mere subsection of the world's population. Moreover, like other fields of psychology, many studies in developmental psychology suffer from low statistical power due to small samples and limited observations. Online testing holds promise as a way to achieve more representative and robust, better powered experiments. As participants do not have to visit in person, it is easier to access populations living further away from a developmental lab, enabling testing of more diverse populations (e.g., urban vs rural areas, various different nationalities or geographies), both within and beyond the researcher's home country. Furthermore, due to the codified nature of browser-based online testing, it is possible for multiple labs to carry out the exact same study, allowing for better replications. Because of these advantages, developmental researchers have started to move experiments online so that caregivers and their children can participate from their home environments. However, the transition from traditional lab testing to remote online testing brings many challenges. Laboratory studies of infant and child development are typically conducted under highly standardized conditions to control factors, such as distractors, distance to the screen, movement, and lighting, and often rely on specialized equipment for measuring behavior. In this chapter, we provide a guide for researchers considering online testing of a developmental population. The different sections comprise an overview of the decision-making processes and the state-of-the-art advances associated with, as well as tangible recommendations for, online data collection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lorijn Zaadnoordijk
- Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
| | - Rhodri Cusack
- Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland.
| |
Collapse
|