1
|
Luef EM. Obsolescence effects in second language phonological networks. Mem Cognit 2024; 52:771-792. [PMID: 38049676 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01500-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/21/2023] [Indexed: 12/06/2023]
Abstract
Phonological networks are representations of word forms and their phonological relationships with other words in a given language lexicon. A principle underlying the growth (or evolution) of those networks is preferential attachment, or the "rich-gets-richer" mechanisms, according to which words with many phonological neighbors (or links) are the main beneficiaries of future growth opportunities. Due to their limited number of words, language lexica constitute node-constrained networks where growth cannot keep increasing in a linear way; hence, preferential attachment is likely mitigated by certain factors. The present study investigated obsolescence effects (i.e., a word's finite timespan of being active in terms of growth) in an evolving phonological network of English as a second language. It was found that phonological neighborhoods are constructed by one large initial lexical spurt, followed by sublinear growth spurts that eventually lead to very limited growth in later lexical spurts during network evolution. First-language-given neighborhood densities are rarely reached even by the most advanced language learners. An analysis of the strength of phonological relationships between phonological word forms revealed a tendency to incorporate phonetically more distant phonological neighbors at earlier acquisition stages. Overall, the findings suggest an obsolescence effect in growth that favors younger words. Implications for the second-language lexicon include leveraged learning mechanisms and learning bouts focused on a smaller range of phonological segments, and involve questions concerning lexical processing in aging networks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva Maria Luef
- Institute of English and American Studies, University of Hamburg, Von-Melle-Park 6, 20146, Hamburg, Germany.
- Department of English and ELT Methodology, Faculty of Arts, Charles University, nám. Jana Palacha 2, 116 38, Prague 1, Czech Republic.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Roivainen E. Age of Acquisition of Personality Terms: Implications for Personality Theory. EUROPES JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 18:132-141. [PMID: 36348698 PMCID: PMC9632550 DOI: 10.5964/ejop.2987] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2020] [Accepted: 12/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Analysis of the age of acquisition (AoA) of personality terms represents a genetic method for the study of the individual personality lexicon and offers a potential alternative to correlational analysis for identifying the fundamental personality descriptors among the thousands of terms that appear in language. In the present study, the relationship between AoA, word frequency, word desirability, and factor loading in the Big Five and Hexaco models of 274 and 408 personality adjectives was analyzed. It was found that young children (2nd graders or younger) acquire personality terms that represent traits at the core of the broad personality factors in the Big Five and Hexaco models slightly earlier than words that represent more peripheral traits. In older children beyond second grade, the correlation between factor loading and AoA is weak. Words that describe the broad openness and stability/emotionality aspects of personality are learned later than words for the other broad factors. Word frequency (in book texts) and desirability have a weak negative correlation with AoA. It is hypothesized that the AoA of a personality term reflects the importance of the corresponding trait for children and may be used as one criterion for ranking facet level traits independent of the broad factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eka Roivainen
- Department of Medical Rehabilitation, Oulu University Hospital, Oulu, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
The Croatian psycholinguistic database: Estimates for 6000 nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. Behav Res Methods 2021; 53:1799-1816. [PMID: 33904142 PMCID: PMC8367916 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-020-01533-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/21/2020] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Psycholinguistic databases containing ratings of concreteness, imageability, age of acquisition, and subjective frequency are used in psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic studies which require words as stimuli. Linguistic characteristics (e.g. word length, corpus frequency) are frequently coded, but word class is seldom systematically treated, although there are indications of its significance for imageability and concreteness. This paper presents the Croatian Psycholinguistic Database (CPD; available at: https://doi.org/10.17234/megahr.2019.hpb ), containing 6000 Croatian nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs, rated for concreteness, imageability, age of acquisition, and subjective frequency. Moreover, we present computationally obtained extrapolations of concreteness and imageability to the remainder of the Croatian lexicon (available at: https://github.com/megahr/lexicon/blob/master/predictions/hr_c_i.predictions.txt ). In the two studies presented here, we explore the significance of word class for concreteness and imageability in human and computationally obtained ratings. The observed correlations in the CPD indicate correspondences between psycholinguistic measures expected from the literature. Word classes exhibit differences in subjective frequency, age of acquisition, concreteness and imageability, with significant differences between nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. In the computational study which focused on concreteness and imageability, concreteness obtained higher correlations with human ratings than imageability, and the system underpredicted the concreteness of nouns, and overpredicted the concreteness of adjectives and adverbs. Overall, this suggests that word class contains schematic conceptual and distributional information. Schematic conceptual content seems to be more significant in human ratings of concreteness and less significant in computationally obtained ratings, where distributional information seems to play a more significant role. This suggests that word class differences should be theoretically explored.
Collapse
|
4
|
Catling JC, Elsherif MM. The hunt for the age of acquisition effect: It's in the links! Acta Psychol (Amst) 2020; 209:103138. [PMID: 32805435 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2020] [Revised: 07/07/2020] [Accepted: 07/08/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The Age of Acquisition (AoA) effect is such that words acquired early in life are processed more quickly than later-acquired words. One explanation for the AoA effects is the arbitrary mapping hypothesis (Ellis & Lambon-Ralph, 2000), stating that the AoA effects are more likely to occur in items that have an arbitrary, rather than a systematic, nature between input and output. Previous behavioural findings have shown that the AoA effects are larger in pictorial than word items. However, no behavioural studies have attempted to directly assess the AoA effects in relation to the connections between representations. In the first two experiments, 48 participants completed a word-picture verification task (Experiments 1A and 2A), together with a spoken (Experiment 1B) or written (Experiment 2B) picture naming task. In the third and fourth experiments, 48 participants complete a picture-word verification task (Experiments 3A and 4A), together with a spoken (Experiment 3B) or written (Experiment 4B) word naming task. For each pair of experiments the subtraction of the naming latencies from the verification tasks for each item per participant was calculated (Experiments 1-4C; e.g. Santiago, Mackay, Palma & Rho, 2000). Results showed that early-acquired items were responded to more quickly than late-acquired ones for all experiments, except for Experiment 3B (spoken word naming) where the AoA effect was shown for only low-frequency words. In addition, the subtraction results for pictorial stimuli demonstrated strong AoA effects. This strengthens the case for the AM hypothesis, also suggesting the AoA effect resides in the connections between representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J C Catling
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - M M Elsherif
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Karimi H, Diaz M. When phonological neighborhood density both facilitates and impedes: Age of acquisition and name agreement interact with phonological neighborhood during word production. Mem Cognit 2020; 48:1061-1072. [PMID: 32372366 PMCID: PMC7787263 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-020-01042-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The number of similar-sounding words that a target word has, or its Phonological Neighborhood Density (PND), has been shown to influence word production. However, reported results are sometimes inconsistent, with studies showing facilitation, inhibition, and null effects of phonological neighbors. These mixed results may be due to the influence of other factors beyond PND. We investigated the potential interactions between a broad measure of PND (bPND), and age of acquisition (AoA), frequency, and name agreement in order to see if the effect of bPND varies as a function of these three variables. We examined the effect of bPND on the latency of picture naming and observed significant interactions between bPND and AoA such that bPND facilitated lexical retrieval for words that were acquired early, but inhibited retrieval for words acquired later in life. We hypothesize that lexical retrieval difficulty ultimately depends on the activation level of the target word's phonological representations relative to the activation levels of its neighbors' phonological representations. When phonological features of the target word are weakly activated (i.e., late AoA), and bPND is high, the neighbors' activation may overshadow the target's, impeding target retrieval. However, when the target's phonological representation is strongly activated, the activation of the neighbors might not exceed that of the target, thereby supporting phonological retrieval. We also observed interactions between bPND and name agreement such that increasing bPND led to faster reaction times (RTs), particularly when name agreement was lower, suggesting that bPND may also facilitate word retrieval when lexical competition is high.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hossein Karimi
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, 356 Moore Building, University Park, PA, USA
| | - Michele Diaz
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, 356 Moore Building, University Park, PA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Two words as one: A multi-naming investigation of the age-of-acquisition effect in compound-word processing. Mem Cognit 2019; 48:511-525. [PMID: 31755026 PMCID: PMC7242258 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-019-00986-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has shown that early-acquired words are produced faster than late-acquired words. Juhasz and colleagues (Juhasz, Lai & Woodcock, Behavior Research Methods, 47 (4), 1004-1019, 2015; Juhasz, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1-10, 2018) argue that the Age-of-Acquisition (AoA) loci for complex words, specifically compound words, are found at the lexical/semantic level. In the current study, two experiments were conducted to evaluate this claim and investigate the influence of AoA in reading compound words aloud. In Experiment 1, 48 participants completed a word naming task. Using general linear mixed modelling, we found that the age at which the compound word was learned significantly affected the naming latencies beyond the other psycholinguistic properties measured. The second experiment required 48 participants to name the compound word when the two morphemes were presented with a space in-between (combinatorial naming, e.g. air plane). We found that the age at which the compound word was learned, as well as the AoA of the individual morphemes that formed the compound word, significantly influenced combinatorial naming latency. These findings are discussed in relation to theories of the AoA in language processing.
Collapse
|
7
|
Łuniewska M, Wodniecka Z, Miller CA, Smolík F, Butcher M, Chondrogianni V, Hreich EK, Messarra C, A. Razak R, Treffers-Daller J, Yap NT, Abboud L, Talebi A, Gureghian M, Tuller L, Haman E. Age of acquisition of 299 words in seven languages: American English, Czech, Gaelic, Lebanese Arabic, Malay, Persian and Western Armenian. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0220611. [PMID: 31393919 PMCID: PMC6687123 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220611] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2019] [Accepted: 07/19/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
We present a new set of subjective Age of Acquisition (AoA) ratings for 299 words (158 nouns, 141 verbs) in seven languages from various language families and cultural settings: American English, Czech, Scottish Gaelic, Lebanese Arabic, Malaysian Malay, Persian, and Western Armenian. The ratings were collected from a total of 173 participants and were highly reliable in each language. We applied the same method of data collection as used in a previous study on 25 languages which allowed us to create a database of fully comparable AoA ratings of 299 words in 32 languages. We found that in the seven languages not included in the previous study, the words are estimated to be acquired at roughly the same age as in the previously reported languages, i.e. mostly between the ages of 1 and 7 years. We also found that the order of word acquisition is moderately to highly correlated across all 32 languages, which extends our previous conclusion that early words are acquired in similar order across a wide range of languages and cultures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Zofia Wodniecka
- Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Krakow, Poland
- Pennsylvania State University, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University Park, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Carol A. Miller
- Pennsylvania State University, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University Park, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Filip Smolík
- Czech Academy of Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Prague, Czech Republic
| | - Morna Butcher
- University of Edinburgh, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom
| | - Vasiliki Chondrogianni
- University of Edinburgh, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom
| | | | - Camille Messarra
- Saint Joseph University of Beirut, High Institute of Speech and Language Therapy, Beirut, Lebanon
| | - Rogayah A. Razak
- Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia, Faculty of Health Science, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
| | - Jeanine Treffers-Daller
- University of Reading, Department of English Language and Applied Linguistics, Reading, United Kingdom
| | - Ngee Thai Yap
- Universiti Putra Malaysia, Faculty of Modern Languages and Communication, Serdang, Malaysia
| | - Layal Abboud
- Saint Joseph University of Beirut, Faculty of Medicine, Beirut, Lebanon
| | - Ali Talebi
- Allameh Tabatabai University, Department of Linguistics and Teaching Persian to Speakers of Other Languages, Teheran, Iran
| | - Maribel Gureghian
- Saint Joseph University of Beirut, Faculty of Medicine, Beirut, Lebanon
| | | | - Ewa Haman
- University of Warsaw, Faculty of Psychology, Warsaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Navarrete E, Arcara G, Mondini S, Penolazzi B. Italian norms and naming latencies for 357 high quality color images. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0209524. [PMID: 30794543 PMCID: PMC6386297 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2018] [Accepted: 02/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In the domain of cognitive studies on the lexico-semantic representational system, one of the most important means of ensuring effective experimental designs is using ecological stimulus sets accompanied by normative data on the most relevant variables affecting the processing of their items. In the context of image sets, color photographs are particularly suited to this purpose as they reduce the difficulty of visual decoding processes that may emerge with traditional image sets of line drawings. This is especially so in clinical populations. In this study we provide Italian norms for a set of 357 high quality image-items belonging to 23 semantic subcategories from the Moreno-Martínez and Montoro database. Data from several variables affecting image processing were collected from a sample of 255 Italian-speaking participants: age of acquisition, familiarity, lexical frequency, manipulability, name agreement, typicality and visual complexity. Lexical frequency data were derived from the CoLFIS corpus. Furthermore, we collected data on image oral naming latencies to explore how the variance in these latencies could be explained by these critical variables. Multiple regression analyses on the naming latencies show classical psycholinguistic phenomena, such as the effects of age of acquisition and name agreement. In addition, manipulability was also a significant predictor. The described Italian normative data and naming latencies are available for download as supplementary material.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eduardo Navarrete
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | | - Sara Mondini
- Department of General Psychology, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
- Human Inspired Technologies Research Centre-HIT, Padova, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Montefinese M, Vinson D, Vigliocco G, Ambrosini E. Italian Age of Acquisition Norms for a Large Set of Words (ItAoA). Front Psychol 2019; 10:278. [PMID: 30814969 PMCID: PMC6381031 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00278] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2018] [Accepted: 01/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Age of acquisition (AoA) is an important psycholinguistic variable that affects the performance of healthy individuals and patients in a large variety of cognitive tasks. For this reason, it becomes more and more compelling to collect new AoA norms for a large set of stimuli in order to allow better control and manipulation of AoA in future research. An important motivation of the present study is to extend previous Italian norms by collecting AoA ratings for a much larger range of Italian words for which concreteness and semantic-affective norms are now available thus ensuring greater coverage of words varying along these dimensions. In the present study, we collected AoA ratings for 1,957 Italian content words (adjectives, nouns, and verbs), by asking healthy adult participants to estimate the age at which they thought they had learned the word in a Web survey procedure. First, we found high split-half correlation within our sample, suggesting strong internal reliability. Second, our data indicate that the ratings collected in this study are as valid and reliable as those collected in previous studies for Italian across different age populations (adult and children) and other languages. Finally, we analyzed the relation between AoA ratings and other lexical-semantic variables (e.g., word frequency, imageability, valence, arousal) and showed that these correlations were generally consistent with the correlations reported in other normative studies for Italian and other languages. Therefore, our new AoA norms are a valuable source of information for future research in the Italian language. The full database is available at the Open Science Framework (osf.io/3trg2).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Montefinese
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| | - David Vinson
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Gabriella Vigliocco
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Ettore Ambrosini
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Marful A, Díez-Álamo AM, Plaza-Navas S, Fernandez A. A normative study for photographs of celebrities in Spain. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0197554. [PMID: 29768497 PMCID: PMC5955507 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197554] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2017] [Accepted: 05/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Research on familiar faces has recurrently been conducted in different domains, such as, psycholinguistics, memory, attention, face processing, aging studies, etc. In general, photographs of celebrities, their proper names, or their occupations have been the materials mainly employed in those types of studies. These stimuli are, however, very constrained by the geographic and sociocultural contexts in which the studies are conducted, and, in spite of their relevance for psychological research, there are no normative studies for celebrities in Spain. With the aim of filling this gap, the photographs and names of the 118 most frequently produced celebrities in Spain were collected. For each celebrity, values for 13 different indices (including psycholinguistic properties, naming times, and emotional indicators) were obtained from a young adult Spanish sample. Regression analyses on the data indicated that the main determinant in naming times and ToTs was the percentage of correct responses. Face agreement was also a significant predictor of ToTs. Results were compared with previous celebrity norms in other languages, and discussed in relation to the current models of face processing. These norms are likely to make a useful contribution to the design of more controlled research and applied tools in Psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alejandra Marful
- Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Jaén, Jaén, Spain
- Centro de Investigación Mente, Cerebro, y Comportamiento (CIMCYC), Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain
- * E-mail:
| | - Antonio M. Díez-Álamo
- Departamento de Psicología Básica, Psicobiología y Metodología de las Ciencias del Comportamiento, Universidad de Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
- Instituto de Integración en la Comunidad (INICO), Universidad de Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
| | | | - Angel Fernandez
- Departamento de Psicología Básica, Psicobiología y Metodología de las Ciencias del Comportamiento, Universidad de Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
- Instituto de Integración en la Comunidad (INICO), Universidad de Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Colombo L, Navarrete E, Arfé B. Acquisition of nouns and verbs in Italian pre-school children. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2017; 44:1362-1393. [PMID: 27995818 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000916000593] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Noun and verb acquisition was investigated in three- and five-year-old Italian children by means of picture naming of objects and actions, selected from Druks and Masterson (2000). The aim was to examine the previously reported advantage of nouns compared to verbs. Older children were faster than younger children, and naming latencies were faster for object pictures than for action pictures. For errors, the advantage of objects over actions was greater for younger children. A qualitative analysis of errors was carried out according to a classification derived by Masterson, Druks, and Gallienne (2008). Overall, 25% of the errors reflected a complete lack of knowledge of the names or of the meanings of the pictures. Most errors, however, were likely to be due to a not yet fully developed knowledge of the meaning of words labelling the pictures, or to an incomplete conceptual representation, and this pattern was more marked for action concepts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucia Colombo
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale,Università di Padova
| | - Eduardo Navarrete
- Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione,Università di Padova
| | - Barbara Arfé
- Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione,Università di Padova
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Räling R, Hanne S, Schröder A, Keßler C, Wartenburger I. Judging the animacy of words: The influence of typicality and age of acquisition in a semantic decision task. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2016; 70:2094-2104. [PMID: 27550541 PMCID: PMC6159778 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1223704] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The age at which members of a semantic category are learned (age of acquisition),
the typicality they demonstrate within their corresponding category, and the
semantic domain to which they belong (living, non-living) are known to influence
the speed and accuracy of lexical/semantic processing. So far, only a few
studies have looked at the origin of age of acquisition and its interdependence
with typicality and semantic domain within the same experimental design. Twenty
adult participants performed an animacy decision task in which nouns were
classified according to their semantic domain as being living or non-living.
Response times were influenced by the independent main effects of each
parameter: typicality, age of acquisition, semantic domain, and frequency.
However, there were no interactions. The results are discussed with respect to
recent models concerning the origin of age of acquisition effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Romy Räling
- a Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics , University of Potsdam , Potsdam , Germany
| | - Sandra Hanne
- a Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics , University of Potsdam , Potsdam , Germany
| | - Astrid Schröder
- a Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics , University of Potsdam , Potsdam , Germany.,b Duden Institute für Lerntherapie , Berlin , Germany
| | - Carla Keßler
- a Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics , University of Potsdam , Potsdam , Germany
| | - Isabell Wartenburger
- a Cognitive Sciences, Department of Linguistics , University of Potsdam , Potsdam , Germany
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Marful A, Gómez-Ariza CJ, Barbón A, Bajo T. Forgetting "Novel" but Not "Dragon": The Role of Age of Acquisition on Intentional and Incidental Forgetting. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0155110. [PMID: 27163698 PMCID: PMC4862635 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2015] [Accepted: 04/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Two experiments studied how the age at which words are acquired (Age of Acquisition, AoA) modulates forgetting. Experiment 1 employed the retrieval-practice paradigm to test the effect of AoA on the incidental forgetting that emerges after solving competition during retrieval (i.e., retrieval-induced forgetting, RIF). Standard RIF appeared with late-acquired words, but this effect disappeared with early-acquired words. Experiment 2 evaluated the effect of AoA on intentional forgetting by employing the list-method directed forgetting paradigm. Results showed a standard directed forgetting effect only when the to-be-forgotten words were late-acquired words. These findings point to the prominent role of AoA in forgetting processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Analía Barbón
- International University of La Rioja, Logroño, Spain
| | - Teresa Bajo
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| |
Collapse
|