1
|
Blinch J, Gooch CE, Clark AR, Murrin B, Bayouth K. Reaction time distribution analysis of bimanual movements with spatial and symbolic cues. Exp Brain Res 2025; 243:58. [PMID: 39907809 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-025-07010-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2024] [Accepted: 01/23/2025] [Indexed: 02/06/2025]
Abstract
The movement preparation of bimanual asymmetric reaching movements is longer than bimanual symmetric movements. This bimanual asymmetric cost is small for spatially cued movements and large with symbolic cues. Previous research on these bimanual asymmetric costs has relied on mean reaction time. The goal of the present study was to better understand the sensorimotor mechanisms of spatially and symbolically cued bimanual asymmetric costs by employing reaction time distribution analysis. Reaction time distributions were described with the ex-Gaussian parameters of [Formula: see text], [Formula: see text], and [Formula: see text]. Forty-eight young adults made bimanual symmetric and asymmetric reaching movements that were spatially or symbolically cued. With spatial cues, there was a small cost to [Formula: see text] for bimanual asymmetric movements compared to symmetric ones. This was depicted as a 4.8 ms rightward shift to the reaction time distribution. The bimanual asymmetric cost with spatial cues is likely caused by the temporal coupling of bimanual asymmetric movements. With symbolic cues, there was a large cost to [Formula: see text] and small costs to [Formula: see text] and [Formula: see text]. This was depicted as the Gaussian and exponential components of the distribution having longer and more disperse reaction time for asymmetric movements. The bimanual asymmetric cost with symbolic cues is likely caused by two factors: stimulus-response translation of two different symbolic cues and temporal coupling of bimanual asymmetric movements. The bimanual asymmetric cost to µ with symbolic cues is likely a combination of both factors, with stimulus-response translation contributing more than temporal coupling. The bimanual asymmetric costs to σ and τ are exclusively caused by stimulus-response translation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jarrod Blinch
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Box 43011, Lubbock, TX, 79409, USA.
| | - Catherine E Gooch
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Box 43011, Lubbock, TX, 79409, USA
| | - Allison R Clark
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Box 43011, Lubbock, TX, 79409, USA
| | - Bryce Murrin
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Box 43011, Lubbock, TX, 79409, USA
| | - Kathryn Bayouth
- Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas Tech University, Box 43011, Lubbock, TX, 79409, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kürten J, Raettig T, Huestegge L. Erroneous saccade co-execution during manual action control is independent of oculomotor stimulus-response translation ease. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2024; 88:2376-2388. [PMID: 39080022 PMCID: PMC11522101 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01989-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2023] [Accepted: 06/08/2024] [Indexed: 11/01/2024]
Abstract
Recent multiple action control studies have demonstrated difficulties with single-action (vs. dual-action) execution when accompanied by the requirement to inhibit a prepotent additional response (e.g., a highly automatic eye movement). Such a dual-action performance benefit is typically characterized by frequent false-positive executions of the currently unwarranted response. Here, we investigated whether the frequency of false-positive saccades is affected by the ease of translating a stimulus into a spatial oculomotor response (S-R translation ease): Is it harder to inhibit a saccade that is more automatically triggered via the stimulus? Participants switched on a trial-by-trial basis between executing a single saccade, a single manual button press, and a saccadic-manual dual action in response to a single visual stimulus. Importantly, we employed three different stimulus modes that varied in oculomotor S-R translation ease (peripheral square > central arrow > central shape). The hierarchy of S-R translation ease was reflected by increasing saccade and manual reaction times. Critically, however, the frequency of false-positive saccades in single manual trials was not substantially affected by the stimulus mode. Our results rule out explanations related to limited capacity sharing (between inhibitory control and S-R translation demands) as well as accounts related to the time available for the completion of saccade inhibition. Instead, the findings suggest that the erroneous co-activation of the oculomotor system was elicited by the mere execution of a (frequently associated) manual response (action-based co-activation).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jens Kürten
- Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Röntgenring 11, 97070, Würzburg, Germany.
| | - Tim Raettig
- Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Röntgenring 11, 97070, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Lynn Huestegge
- Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Röntgenring 11, 97070, Würzburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Coco M, Di Pasquale F, Pellicano A. Knowledge over Time of Action Codes for Perceived Objects: An Exploratory Study on Developmental Children. Brain Sci 2024; 14:854. [PMID: 39335350 PMCID: PMC11429757 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14090854] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2024] [Revised: 08/16/2024] [Accepted: 08/21/2024] [Indexed: 09/30/2024] Open
Abstract
Over the past 20 years, there has been a growing interest in the processing of tool objects, and in the spatial S-R correspondence effects obtained with pictures of manipulable objects. Beyond the original affordance activation account, a location coding account has been proposed for such behavioral effects, which states that the location of the visually salient portion of an object speeds up spatially aligned motor responses and slows down misaligned ones. Furthermore, an additional action coding account has been proposed, according to which it is the direction of the action of the perceived object (e.g., "pouring tea leftward" when the spout of a teapot is leftward-oriented) that biases motor performance. We investigated this action coding account of S-R correspondence effects by comparing the reaction time (RT) performance of two groups: younger (6 to 9 years old) and older (10 to 13 years old) children. We assumed that knowledge of tool objects and the activation of action codes from object perception is proportional to age. Therefore, a larger correspondence effect was hypothesized for the older relative to the younger children's group. Consistently, a 34 ms correspondence effect was observed in the older children relative to the younger ones (18 ms). The results support the view that action direction is a constitutive feature of tool objects, which is subject to experience, and thus, to increasing knowledge over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marinella Coco
- Department of Educational Sciences, University of Catania, 95100 Catania, Italy
- B.R.I.T. (Bio-Nanotech Research and Innovation Tower) Service Center, University of Catania, 95100 Catania, Italy
| | | | - Antonello Pellicano
- Department of Educational Sciences, University of Catania, 95100 Catania, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Nucci L, Miraglia F, Alù F, Pappalettera C, Judica E, Manenti R, Rossini PM, Vecchio F. Reaction time and cognitive strategies: The role of education in task performance. LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2023. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lmot.2023.101884] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/13/2023]
|
5
|
The Simon Effect Asymmetry for Left- and Right-Dominant Persons. J Cogn 2023; 6:17. [PMID: 36874910 PMCID: PMC9983503 DOI: 10.5334/joc.265] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2022] [Accepted: 02/17/2023] [Indexed: 03/06/2023] Open
Abstract
When participants respond to a task-relevant stimulus attribute by pressing a left or right key with the respective index finger, reaction time is shorter if task-irrelevant left-right stimulus location corresponds to that of the response key than if it does not. For right-handers, this Simon effect is larger for right-located than left-located stimuli; for left-handers this Simon-effect asymmetry is reversed. A similar asymmetry has been found for right-footers pressing pedals with their feet. For analyses that separate stimulus- and response-location factors, these asymmetries appear as a main effect of response location, with responses being faster with the dominant effector. If the Simon-effect asymmetry is strictly a function of effector dominance, it should reverse for left-footers responding with their feet. In Experiment 1, left-dominant persons showed faster responses with the left than right hand but with the right than left foot, a finding consistent with prior research on tapping actions. Right-dominant persons also showed the right-foot asymmetry but, unexpectedly, not the typical asymmetry with hand responses. To evaluate whether hand-presses yield results distinct from finger-presses, in Experiment 2 participants performed the Simon task with finger-presses and hand-presses. The opposing asymmetries for right- and left-dominant persons were evident for both response modes. Our results are consistent with the view that the Simon effect asymmetry is primarily due to differences in effector efficiency, usually but not always favoring the dominant effector.
Collapse
|
6
|
A diffusion model for the congruency sequence effect. Psychon Bull Rev 2022; 29:2034-2051. [PMID: 35676612 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-022-02119-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/03/2022] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Two-choice reaction tasks for which stimuli differ on irrelevant and relevant dimensions (e.g., Simon, flanker, and Stroop tasks) show congruency effects. The diffusion model for conflict tasks (DMC) has provided a quantitative account of the mechanisms underlying decisions in such conflict tasks, but it has not been applied to the congruency sequence effect (CSE) for which the congruency on the prior trial influences performance on the current trial. The present study expands analysis of the reaction time (RT) distributions reflected by delta plots to the CSE, and then extends the DMC to simulate the results. With increasing RT: (1) the spatial Simon effect was almost unchanged following congruent trials but initially became smaller and finally reversed following incongruent trials; (2) the arrow-based Simon effects increased following both congruent and incongruent trials, but more so for the former than the latter; (3) the flanker congruency effect varied quadratically following congruent trials but increased linearly following incongruent trials. These results were modeled by the CSE-DMC, extended from the DMC with two additional assumptions: (1) feature integration influences only the controlled processes; (2) following incongruent trials, the automatic process is weakened. The results fit better with the CSE-DMC than with two variants that separately had only one of the two additional assumptions. These findings indicate that the CSEs for different conflict tasks have disparate RT distributions and that these disparities are likely due to the controlled and automatic processes being influenced differently for each trial sequence.
Collapse
|
7
|
Delta plots for conflict tasks: An activation-suppression race model. Psychon Bull Rev 2021; 28:1776-1795. [PMID: 34327678 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-021-01900-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We describe a mathematically simple yet precise model of activation suppression that can explain the negative-going delta plots often observed in standard Simon tasks. The model postulates a race between the identification of the relevant stimulus attribute and the suppression of irrelevant location-based activation, with the irrelevant activation only having an effect if the irrelevant activation is still present at the moment when central processing of the relevant attribute starts. The model can be fitted by maximum likelihood to observed distributions of RTs in congruent and incongruent trials, and it provides good fits to two previously-reported data sets with plausible parameter values. R and MATLAB software for use with the model is provided.
Collapse
|
8
|
Dudarev V, Iarocci G, Enns JT. A Joint Simon effect in children diagnosed with ASD is expressed differently from neurotypical children and adults. VISUAL COGNITION 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2021.1958039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Veronica Dudarev
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| | - Grace Iarocci
- Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, Canada
| | - James T. Enns
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Korko M, Coulson M, Jones A, de Mornay Davies P. Types of interference and their resolution in monolingual word production. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2021; 214:103251. [PMID: 33485153 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103251] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2020] [Revised: 07/10/2020] [Accepted: 01/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
There is growing evidence that speakers recruit inhibitory control in situations of high within-language interference, e.g., when selecting from among competing lexical entries or when tailoring utterances to the communicative needs of the addressee. However, little is known about the types of cognitive control mechanisms that are involved in the speech production process. This study examines the relative contribution of various forms of interference arising at different stages of information processing as well as their control to object naming under conditions of prepotent and underdetermined competition. Eighty-nine unimpaired native English speakers completed three inhibitory control tasks (arrow flanker, Simon arrow and anti-saccade) and two object naming tasks (picture-word interference, PWI, and name agreement, NA). Analyses of mean RT and RT distribution (delta plots) showed that only the flanker effect was a significant predictor of the PWI but not NA effect, while the remaining inhibitory measures made no significant contribution to either the PWI or NA effect. Participants with smaller flanker effects, indicative of better resolution of representational conflict, were faster to name objects in the face of competing stimuli. The pattern of results suggests that delays in production can be an outcome of inefficient resolution of interference traced to intermediate rather than late stages of processing, at least as far as the PWI task is concerned.
Collapse
|
10
|
Xiong A, Proctor RW, Xu Y, Zelaznik HN. Visual salience of 3D and 2D spoons determines S-R mapping and flanker effects. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2020; 74:241-253. [PMID: 33063606 DOI: 10.1177/1747021820959599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
This study tested the hypothesis that affordances for grasping with the corresponding hand are activated more strongly by three-dimensional (3D) real objects than by two-dimensional (2D) pictures of the objects. In Experiment 1, participants made left and right keypress responses to the handle or functional end (tip) of an eating utensil using compatible and incompatible mappings. In one session, stimuli were spoons mounted horizontally on a blackboard with the sides to which the handle and tip pointed varying randomly. In the other, stimuli were pictures of spoons displayed on a black computer screen. Three-dimensional and 2D sessions showed a similar benefit for compatible mapping when the tip was relevant and a small cost of compatible mapping when the handle was relevant. Experiment 2 used a flanker task in which participants responded compatibly to the location of the handle or the tip, and spoons located above and below the target spoon could have congruent or incongruent orientations. The difference between 3D and 2D displays was not obtained in the flanker effect for reaction time. There was little evidence that 3D objects activate grasping affordances that 2D images do not. Instead, we argue that visual salience of the tip is the critical factor determining these correspondence effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aiping Xiong
- College of Information Sciences and Technology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| | | | - Yaqi Xu
- Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Luo C, Proctor RW. Word- and arrow-based Simon effects emerge for eccentrically presented location words and arrows. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2020; 85:816-827. [PMID: 31956922 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-019-01280-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2019] [Accepted: 12/20/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments examined whether the location-based Simon effect and word- or arrow-based Simon effects, and their interaction, emerge in the same task situations by presenting location words and (left and right, Experiment 1) or single-headed arrows (left and right pointing, Experiment 2) in the left-right visual field. These tasks include two attributes of task-irrelevant location information, physical location and either location word (Experiment 1) or arrow direction (Experiment 2), when they vary jointly for a single stimulus. Moreover, the location-based Simon effect in these tasks was compared to that obtained in a pure location-based Simon task. Results showed that (1) the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects occurred on both mean RT and delta plots; (2) the word- and arrow-based Simon effects interacted with the location-based Simon effect on mean RT; (3) the Simon effect in the pure location-based Simon task differed little from the location-based Simon effect in the two joint Simon tasks. These results indicate that different task-irrelevant spatial attributes can influence responses in the same task, and that one of them can influence the effect of the other on responses. This latter result offers evidence that the different attributes do not provide separate sources of activation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chunming Luo
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, 16 Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing, 100101, China.
| | - Robert W Proctor
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
Task-irrelevant stimulus location can influence the response performance to task-relevant attributes, generating the location-based Simon effect. Using a Monte Carlo study and other methods, we examined whether the ex-Gaussian distribution provides a good fit to empirical reaction time (RT) distributions in the Simon task and whether reliable Simon effects occur on the ex-Gaussian parameters: (a) the mean (μ), (b) the standard deviation (σ) of the normal distribution, and (c) the tail (τ). Results showed that the ex-Gaussian function fits well to empirical RT distributions, and that these ex-Gaussian parameters are reliable between two trial blocks at the group level. At the individual level, correlation analysis showed that the Simon effect was reliable on the μ parameter but not on σ and τ. Moreover, a partial correlation analysis, with μs of the two blocks as controlling variables, showed that the Simon effect on τ was reliable. These results provide evidence that the ex-Gaussian function is a valuable tool for analyzing the Simon effect and can be considered as an alternative for analyzing RT distributions in Simon-type tasks.
Collapse
|
13
|
Luo C, Proctor RW. Shared mechanisms underlying the location-, word- and arrow-based Simon effects. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2019; 84:1655-1667. [PMID: 30941493 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-019-01175-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2018] [Accepted: 03/26/2019] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
A left or right keypress response to a relevant stimulus attribute (e.g., color) is faster when irrelevant left or right stimulus-location information corresponds with the correct response than when it does not. This phenomenon, known as the Simon effect, is obtained not only for physical locations, but also location words "left" and "right" and left- or right-pointing arrows. However, these location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects show different patterns in the reaction-time (RT) distributions, as evident in delta plots. In the present study, we employed procedures, analysis of survival curves and divergence point analysis, which have not previously been applied to the Simon effect, to investigate differences in time course of these various Simon effects in more detail. Also, we examined whether the diffusion model for conflict tasks (DMC), which assumes that automatic activation of task-irrelevant information occurs in a pulse-like function, can capture not only features of the RT distributions for the location-based Simon effect, to which it has been fit previously, but also features of the word- and arrow-based Simon effects, to which it has not. Results showed different survival curves and earliest, maximum, and latest divergence points for the three Simon effects, but DMC was able to capture the basic features of the RT distributions reflected by delta plot and survival curves for all effects. The results imply that the location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects have shared mechanisms, although they have different RT distributions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chunming Luo
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, 16 Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing, 100101, China.
| | - Robert W Proctor
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Luo C, Proctor RW. How different direct association routes influence the indirect route in the same Simon-like task. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 83:1733-1748. [PMID: 29761377 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1024-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2018] [Accepted: 05/05/2018] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
Abstract
The location-, word-, and arrow-based Simon effects are usually attributed to the result of a direct route (the spatially corresponding stimulus-response association, activated automatically) that interferes with an indirect route (the association of task-relevant information and response, activated in accordance with the instructed stimulus-response mapping). We examined whether and how distinct direct routes (stimulus-location-response position and location word-response position or arrow direction-response position associations) affect responding on the basis of the same indirect route (a stimulus color-response association) in a Simon-like task. For this task, left-right keypresses were made to indicate the ink colors of location words or left- or right-pointing arrows, presented eccentrically in left or right locations. The location-based Simon effect occurred at the levels of mean reaction time (RT) and RT distribution in the word Simon-like task, whereas the word-based Simon effect only occurred at the level of RT distribution. In the arrow Simon-like task, the location-based Simon effect did not occur at the level of mean RT, but did at the level of RT distribution, whereas the opposite pattern occurred for the arrow-based Simon effect. These results could imply that one direct route influences the effects of the other direct route on the responses, depending on the task context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chunming Luo
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, 16 Lincui Road, Chaoyang District, Beijing, 100101, China.
| | - Robert W Proctor
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
| |
Collapse
|