451
|
Drnec K, Marathe AR, Lukos JR, Metcalfe JS. From Trust in Automation to Decision Neuroscience: Applying Cognitive Neuroscience Methods to Understand and Improve Interaction Decisions Involved in Human Automation Interaction. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:290. [PMID: 27445741 PMCID: PMC4927573 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00290] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2015] [Accepted: 05/30/2016] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Human automation interaction (HAI) systems have thus far failed to live up to expectations mainly because human users do not always interact with the automation appropriately. Trust in automation (TiA) has been considered a central influence on the way a human user interacts with an automation; if TiA is too high there will be overuse, if TiA is too low there will be disuse. However, even though extensive research into TiA has identified specific HAI behaviors, or trust outcomes, a unique mapping between trust states and trust outcomes has yet to be clearly identified. Interaction behaviors have been intensely studied in the domain of HAI and TiA and this has led to a reframing of the issues of problems with HAI in terms of reliance and compliance. We find the behaviorally defined terms reliance and compliance to be useful in their functionality for application in real-world situations. However, we note that once an inappropriate interaction behavior has occurred it is too late to mitigate it. We therefore take a step back and look at the interaction decision that precedes the behavior. We note that the decision neuroscience community has revealed that decisions are fairly stereotyped processes accompanied by measurable psychophysiological correlates. Two literatures were therefore reviewed. TiA literature was extensively reviewed in order to understand the relationship between TiA and trust outcomes, as well as to identify gaps in current knowledge. We note that an interaction decision precedes an interaction behavior and believe that we can leverage knowledge of the psychophysiological correlates of decisions to improve joint system performance. As we believe that understanding the interaction decision will be critical to the eventual mitigation of inappropriate interaction behavior, we reviewed the decision making literature and provide a synopsis of the state of the art understanding of the decision process from a decision neuroscience perspective. We forward hypotheses based on this understanding that could shape a research path toward the ability to mitigate interaction behavior in the real world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kim Drnec
- Human Research and Engineering Directorate, U.S. Army Research Laboratory Aberdeen, MD, USA
| | - Amar R Marathe
- Human Research and Engineering Directorate, U.S. Army Research Laboratory Aberdeen, MD, USA
| | - Jamie R Lukos
- Advanced Concepts and Applied Research Branch, Space and Naval Warfare Systems Center Pacific San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Jason S Metcalfe
- Human Research and Engineering Directorate, U.S. Army Research Laboratory Aberdeen, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
452
|
Rich EL, Wallis JD. Decoding subjective decisions from orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2016; 19:973-80. [PMID: 27273768 PMCID: PMC4925198 DOI: 10.1038/nn.4320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 202] [Impact Index Per Article: 25.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2016] [Accepted: 05/05/2016] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
Abstract
When making a subjective choice, the brain must compute a value for each option and compare those values to make a decision. The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is critically involved in this process, but the neural mechanisms remain obscure, in part due to limitations in our ability to measure and control the internal deliberations that can alter the dynamics of the decision process. Here, we tracked the dynamics by recovering temporally precise neural states from multi-dimensional data in OFC. During individual choices, OFC alternated between states associated with the value of two available options, with dynamics that predicted whether a subject would decide quickly or vacillate between the two alternatives. Ensembles of value-encoding neurons contributed to these states, with individual neurons shifting activity patterns as the network evaluated each option. Thus, the mechanism of subjective decision-making involves the dynamic activation of OFC states associated with each choice alternative.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin L Rich
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California, USA
| | - Jonathan D Wallis
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California, USA.,Department of Psychology, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
453
|
Orbitofrontal Cortex Value Signals Depend on Fixation Location during Free Viewing. Neuron 2016; 90:1299-1311. [PMID: 27263972 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2015] [Revised: 01/30/2016] [Accepted: 04/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
In the natural world, monkeys and humans judge the economic value of numerous competing stimuli by moving their gaze from one object to another, in a rapid series of eye movements. This suggests that the primate brain processes value serially, and that value-coding neurons may be modulated by changes in gaze. To test this hypothesis, we presented monkeys with value-associated visual cues and took the unusual step of allowing unrestricted free viewing while we recorded neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). By leveraging natural gaze patterns, we found that a large proportion of OFC cells encode gaze location and, that in some cells, value coding is amplified when subjects fixate near the cue. These findings provide the first cellular-level mechanism for previously documented behavioral effects of gaze on valuation and suggest a major role for gaze in neural mechanisms of valuation and decision-making under ecologically realistic conditions.
Collapse
|
454
|
Van Duijvenvoorde ACK, Figner B, Weeda WD, Van der Molen MW, Jansen BRJ, Huizenga HM. Neural Mechanisms Underlying Compensatory and Noncompensatory Strategies in Risky Choice. J Cogn Neurosci 2016; 28:1358-73. [PMID: 27167399 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00975] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Individuals may differ systematically in their applied decision strategies, which has critical implications for decision neuroscience but is yet scarcely studied. Our study's main focus was therefore to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying compensatory versus noncompensatory strategies in risky choice. Here, we compared people using a compensatory expected value maximization with people using a simplified noncompensatory loss-minimizing choice strategy. To this end, we used a two-choice paradigm including a set of "simple" items (e.g., simple condition), in which one option was superior on all attributes, and a set of "conflict" items, in which one option was superior on one attribute but inferior on other attributes. A binomial mixture analysis of the decisions elicited by these items differentiated between decision-makers using either a compensatory or a noncompensatory strategy. Behavioral differences were particularly pronounced in the conflict condition, and these were paralleled by neural results. That is, we expected compensatory decision-makers to use an integrated value comparison during choice in the conflict condition. Accordingly, the compensatory group tracked the difference in expected value between choice options reflected in neural activation in the parietal cortex. Furthermore, we expected noncompensatory, compared with compensatory, decision-makers to experience increased conflict when attributes provided conflicting information. Accordingly, the noncompensatory group showed greater dorsomedial PFC activation only in the conflict condition. These pronounced behavioral and neural differences indicate the need for decision neuroscience to account for individual differences in risky choice strategies and to broaden its scope to noncompensatory risky choice strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Wouter D Weeda
- Leiden University.,Leiden Institute for Brain & Cognition
| | | | - Brenda R J Jansen
- University of Amsterdam.,Radboud University Nijmegen.,Amsterdam Brain & Cognition Center
| | - Hilde M Huizenga
- University of Amsterdam.,Radboud University Nijmegen.,Amsterdam Brain & Cognition Center
| |
Collapse
|
455
|
Konovalov A, Krajbich I. Over a Decade of Neuroeconomics: What Have We Learned? ORGANIZATIONAL RESEARCH METHODS 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/1094428116644502] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
At its inception, neuroeconomics promised to revolutionize economics. That promise has not yet been realized, and neuroeconomics has seen limited penetration into mainstream economics. Nevertheless, it would be a mistake to declare that neuroeconomics has failed. Quite to the contrary, the yearly rate of neuroeconomics papers has roughly doubled since 2005. While the number of direct applications to economics remains limited, due to the infancy of the field, we have learned an amazing amount about how the brain makes decisions. In this article, we review some of the major topics that have emerged in neuroeconomics and highlight findings that we believe will form the basis for future applications to economics. When possible, we focus on existing applications to economics and future directions for that research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arkady Konovalov
- Department of Economics, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
| | - Ian Krajbich
- Department of Economics, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
456
|
Graphs versus numbers: How information format affects risk aversion in gambling. JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING 2016. [DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500003077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
AbstractIn lottery gambling, the common phenomenon of risk aversion shows up as preference of the option with the higher win probability, even if a riskier alternative offers a greater expected value. Because riskier choices would optimize profitability in such cases, the present study investigates the visual format, with which lotteries are conveyed, as potential instrument to modulate risk attitudes. Previous research has shown that enhanced attention to graphical compared to numerical probabilities can increase risk aversion, but evidence for the reverse effect — reduced risk aversion through a graphical display of outcomes — is sparse. We conducted three experiments, in which participants repeatedly selected one of two lotteries. Probabilities and outcomes were either presented numerically or in a graphical format that consisted of pie charts (Experiment 1) or icon arrays (Experiment 2 and 3). Further, expected values were either higher in the safer or in the riskier lottery, or they did not differ between the options. Despite a marked risk aversion in all experiments, our results show that presenting outcomes as graphs can reduce — albeit not eliminate — risk aversion (Experiment 3). Yet, not all formats prove suitable, and non-intuitive outcome graphs can even enhance risk aversion (Experiment 1). Joint analyses of choice proportions and response times (RTs) further uncovered that risk aversion leads to safe choices particularly in fast decisions. This pattern is expressed under graphical probabilities, whereas graphical outcomes can weaken the rapid dominance of risk aversion and the variability over RTs (Experiment 1 and 2). Together, our findings demonstrate the relevance of information format for risky decisions.
Collapse
|
457
|
Neural substrates of cognitive biases during probabilistic inference. Nat Commun 2016; 7:11393. [PMID: 27116102 PMCID: PMC4853436 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11393] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2015] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Decision making often requires simultaneously learning about and combining evidence from various sources of information. However, when making inferences from these sources, humans show systematic biases that are often attributed to heuristics or limitations in cognitive processes. Here we use a combination of experimental and modelling approaches to reveal neural substrates of probabilistic inference and corresponding biases. We find systematic deviations from normative accounts of inference when alternative options are not equally rewarding; subjects' choice behaviour is biased towards the more rewarding option, whereas their inferences about individual cues show the opposite bias. Moreover, inference bias about combinations of cues depends on the number of cues. Using a biophysically plausible model, we link these biases to synaptic plasticity mechanisms modulated by reward expectation and attention. We demonstrate that inference relies on direct estimation of posteriors, not on combination of likelihoods and prior. Our work reveals novel mechanisms underlying cognitive biases and contributions of interactions between reward-dependent learning, decision making and attention to high-level reasoning. Humans are often biased in estimating the precise influence of probabilistic events on their decisions. Here, Khorsand and colleagues report a behavioural task that produces these biases in inference and describe a biophysically-plausible model that captures these behavioural deviations from optimal decision making.
Collapse
|
458
|
Bault N, Wydoodt P, Coricelli G. Different Attentional Patterns for Regret and Disappointment: An Eye-tracking Study. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2016; 29:194-205. [PMID: 30122806 PMCID: PMC6084306 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1938] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2014] [Revised: 10/21/2015] [Accepted: 12/01/2015] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
The unfavorable comparison between the obtained and expected outcomes of our choices may elicit disappointment. When the comparison is made with the outcome of alternative actions, emotions like regret can serve as a learning signal. Previous work showed that both anticipated disappointment and regret influence decisions. In addition, experienced regret is associated with higher emotional responses than disappointment. Yet it is not clear whether this amplification is due to additive effects of disappointment and regret when the outcomes of alternative actions are available, or whether it reflects the learning feature of regret signals. In this perspective, we used eye-tracking to measure the visual pattern of information acquisition in a probabilistic lottery task. In the partial feedback condition, only the outcome of the chosen lottery was revealed, while in the complete feedback condition, participants could compare their outcome with that of the non-chosen lottery, giving them the opportunity to experience regret. During the decision phase, visual patterns of information acquisition were consistent with the assessment of anticipated regret, in addition to a clear assessment of lotteries' expected values. During the feedback phase, subjective ratings and eye-tracking results confirmed that participants compared their outcome with the outcome of the non-chosen lottery in the complete feedback condition, particularly after a loss, and ignored the non-realized outcome of the chosen option. Moreover, participants who made more visual saccades consistent with counterfactual comparisons during the feedback period anticipated regret more in their decisions. These results are consistent with the proposed adaptive function of regret.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nadège Bault
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (Cimec)University of TrentoTrentoItaly
| | | | - Giorgio Coricelli
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (Cimec)University of TrentoTrentoItaly
- Department of EconomicsUniversity of Southern CaliforniaLos AngelesCAUSA
| |
Collapse
|
459
|
Ratcliff R, Smith PL, Brown SD, McKoon G. Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History. Trends Cogn Sci 2016; 20:260-281. [PMID: 26952739 PMCID: PMC4928591 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 711] [Impact Index Per Article: 88.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2015] [Revised: 01/15/2016] [Accepted: 01/26/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
There is growing interest in diffusion models to represent the cognitive and neural processes of speeded decision making. Sequential-sampling models like the diffusion model have a long history in psychology. They view decision making as a process of noisy accumulation of evidence from a stimulus. The standard model assumes that evidence accumulates at a constant rate during the second or two it takes to make a decision. This process can be linked to the behaviors of populations of neurons and to theories of optimality. Diffusion models have been used successfully in a range of cognitive tasks and as psychometric tools in clinical research to examine individual differences. In this review, we relate the models to both earlier and more recent research in psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roger Ratcliff
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA.
| | - Philip L Smith
- Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, Level 12, Redmond Barry Building 115, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia
| | - Scott D Brown
- School of Psychology, University of Newcastle, Australia, Aviation Building, Callaghan, NSW 2308, Australia
| | - Gail McKoon
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA
| |
Collapse
|
460
|
Brody CD, Hanks TD. Neural underpinnings of the evidence accumulator. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2016; 37:149-157. [PMID: 26878969 PMCID: PMC5777584 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 94] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/31/2015] [Accepted: 01/05/2016] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
Abstract
Gradual accumulation of evidence favoring one or another choice is considered a core component of many different types of decisions, and has been the subject of many neurophysiological studies in non-human primates. But its neural circuit mechanisms remain mysterious. Investigating it in rodents has recently become possible, facilitating perturbation experiments to delineate the relevant causal circuit, as well as the application of other tools more readily available in rodents. In addition, advances in stimulus design and analysis have aided studying the relevant neural encoding. In complement to ongoing non-human primate studies, these newly available model systems and tools place the field at an exciting time that suggests that the dynamical circuit mechanisms underlying accumulation of evidence could soon be revealed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carlos D Brody
- Howard Hughes Medical Institute, USA; Princeton Neuroscience Institute and Department of Molecular Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA.
| | - Timothy D Hanks
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California Davis, Davis, CA 95618, USA; Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA 95817, USA
| |
Collapse
|
461
|
Ashby NJS, Johnson JG, Krajbich I, Wedel M. Applications and Innovations of Eye-movement Research in Judgment and Decision Making. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2016. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1956] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
462
|
Tsetsos K, Moran R, Moreland J, Chater N, Usher M, Summerfield C. Economic irrationality is optimal during noisy decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113:3102-7. [PMID: 26929353 PMCID: PMC4801289 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1519157113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
According to normative theories, reward-maximizing agents should have consistent preferences. Thus, when faced with alternatives A, B, and C, an individual preferring A to B and B to C should prefer A to C. However, it has been widely argued that humans can incur losses by violating this axiom of transitivity, despite strong evolutionary pressure for reward-maximizing choices. Here, adopting a biologically plausible computational framework, we show that intransitive (and thus economically irrational) choices paradoxically improve accuracy (and subsequent economic rewards) when decision formation is corrupted by internal neural noise. Over three experiments, we show that humans accumulate evidence over time using a "selective integration" policy that discards information about alternatives with momentarily lower value. This policy predicts violations of the axiom of transitivity when three equally valued alternatives differ circularly in their number of winning samples. We confirm this prediction in a fourth experiment reporting significant violations of weak stochastic transitivity in human observers. Crucially, we show that relying on selective integration protects choices against "late" noise that otherwise corrupts decision formation beyond the sensory stage. Indeed, we report that individuals with higher late noise relied more strongly on selective integration. These findings suggest that violations of rational choice theory reflect adaptive computations that have evolved in response to irreducible noise during neural information processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Konstantinos Tsetsos
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3UD, United Kingdom; Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom;
| | - Rani Moran
- School of Psychology, University of Tel Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel; Sagol School of Neuroscience, University of Tel Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
| | - James Moreland
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
| | - Nick Chater
- Behavioural Science Group, Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, United Kingdom
| | - Marius Usher
- School of Psychology, University of Tel Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel; Sagol School of Neuroscience, University of Tel Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
| | | |
Collapse
|
463
|
Bhatia S, Mullett TL. The dynamics of deferred decision. Cogn Psychol 2016; 86:112-51. [PMID: 26970689 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2015] [Revised: 02/10/2016] [Accepted: 02/16/2016] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Decision makers are often unable to choose between the options that they are offered. In these settings they typically defer their decision, that is, delay the decision to a later point in time or avoid the decision altogether. In this paper, we outline eight behavioral findings regarding the causes and consequences of choice deferral that cognitive theories of decision making should be able to capture. We show that these findings can be accounted for by a deferral-based time limit applied to existing sequential sampling models of preferential choice. Our approach to modeling deferral as a time limit in a sequential sampling model also makes a number of novel predictions regarding the interactions between choice probabilities, deferral probabilities, and decision times, and we confirm these predictions in an experiment. Choice deferral is a key feature of everyday decision making, and our paper illustrates how established theoretical approaches can be used to understand the cognitive underpinnings of this important behavioral phenomenon.
Collapse
|
464
|
Krastev S, McGuire JT, McNeney D, Kable JW, Stolle D, Gidengil E, Fellows LK. Do Political and Economic Choices Rely on Common Neural Substrates? A Systematic Review of the Emerging Neuropolitics Literature. Front Psychol 2016; 7:264. [PMID: 26941703 PMCID: PMC4766282 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2015] [Accepted: 02/10/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The methods of cognitive neuroscience are beginning to be applied to the study of political behavior. The neural substrates of value-based decision-making have been extensively examined in economic contexts; this might provide a powerful starting point for understanding political decision-making. Here, we asked to what extent the neuropolitics literature to date has used conceptual frameworks and experimental designs that make contact with the reward-related approaches that have dominated decision neuroscience. We then asked whether the studies of political behavior that can be considered in this light implicate the brain regions that have been associated with subjective value related to "economic" reward. We performed a systematic literature review to identify papers addressing the neural substrates of political behavior and extracted the fMRI studies reporting behavioral measures of subjective value as defined in decision neuroscience studies of reward. A minority of neuropolitics studies met these criteria and relatively few brain activation foci from these studies overlapped with regions where activity has been related to subjective value. These findings show modest influence of reward-focused decision neuroscience on neuropolitics research to date. Whether the neural substrates of subjective value identified in economic choice paradigms generalize to political choice thus remains an open question. We argue that systematically addressing the commonalities and differences in these two classes of value-based choice will be important in developing a more comprehensive model of the brain basis of human decision-making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sekoul Krastev
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Joseph T McGuire
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Denver McNeney
- Centre for the Study of Democratic Citizenship, Department of Political Science, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Joseph W Kable
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Dietlind Stolle
- Centre for the Study of Democratic Citizenship, Department of Political Science, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Elisabeth Gidengil
- Centre for the Study of Democratic Citizenship, Department of Political Science, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Lesley K Fellows
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
465
|
Sali AW, Anderson BA, Courtney SM. Information processing biases in the brain: Implications for decision-making and self-governance. NEUROETHICS-NETH 2016; 11:259-271. [PMID: 30555600 DOI: 10.1007/s12152-016-9251-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
To make behavioral choices that are in line with our goals and our moral beliefs, we need to gather and consider information about our current situation. Most information present in our environment is not relevant to the choices we need or would want to make and thus could interfere with our ability to behave in ways that reflect our underlying values. Certain sources of information could even lead us to make choices we later regret, and thus it would be beneficial to be able to ignore that information. Our ability to exert successful self-governance depends on our ability to attend to sources of information that we deem important to our decision-making processes. We generally assume that, at any moment, we have the ability to choose what we pay attention to. However, recent research indicates that what we pay attention to is influenced by our prior experiences, including reward history and past successes and failures, even when we are not aware of this history. Even momentary distractions can cause us to miss or discount information that should have a greater influence on our decisions given our values. Such biases in attention thus raise questions about the degree to which the choices that we make may be poorly informed and not truly reflect our ability to otherwise exert self-governance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anthony W Sali
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University.,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University
| | - Brian A Anderson
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University
| | - Susan M Courtney
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University.,Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine.,F.M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Kennedy Krieger Institute
| |
Collapse
|
466
|
Abstract
An experiment is presented in which subjects were tested on both one-choice and two-choice driving tasks and on non-driving versions of them. Diffusion models for one- and two-choice tasks were successful in extracting model-based measures from the response time and accuracy data. These include measures of the quality of the information from the stimuli that drove the decision process (drift rate in the model), the time taken up by processes outside the decision process and, for the two-choice model, the speed/accuracy decision criteria that subjects set. Drift rates were only marginally different between the driving and non-driving tasks, indicating that nearly the same information was used in the two kinds of tasks. The tasks differed in the time taken up by other processes, reflecting the difference between them in response processing demands. Drift rates were significantly correlated across the two two-choice tasks showing that subjects that performed well on one task also performed well on the other task. Nondecision times were correlated across the two driving tasks, showing common abilities on motor processes across the two tasks. These results show the feasibility of using diffusion modeling to examine decision making in driving and so provide for a theoretical examination of factors that might impair driving, such as extreme aging, distraction, sleep deprivation, and so on.
Collapse
|
467
|
Yang SCH, Lengyel M, Wolpert DM. Active sensing in the categorization of visual patterns. eLife 2016; 5. [PMID: 26880546 PMCID: PMC4764587 DOI: 10.7554/elife.12215] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/10/2015] [Accepted: 12/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Interpreting visual scenes typically requires us to accumulate information from multiple locations in a scene. Using a novel gaze-contingent paradigm in a visual categorization task, we show that participants' scan paths follow an active sensing strategy that incorporates information already acquired about the scene and knowledge of the statistical structure of patterns. Intriguingly, categorization performance was markedly improved when locations were revealed to participants by an optimal Bayesian active sensor algorithm. By using a combination of a Bayesian ideal observer and the active sensor algorithm, we estimate that a major portion of this apparent suboptimality of fixation locations arises from prior biases, perceptual noise and inaccuracies in eye movements, and the central process of selecting fixation locations is around 70% efficient in our task. Our results suggest that participants select eye movements with the goal of maximizing information about abstract categories that require the integration of information from multiple locations. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12215.001 To interact with the world around us, we need to decide how best to direct our eyes and other senses to extract relevant information. When viewing a scene, people fixate on a sequence of locations by making fast eye movements to shift their gaze between locations. Previous studies have shown that these fixations are not random, but are actively chosen so that they depend on both the scene and the task. For example, in order to determine the gender or emotion from a face, we fixate around the eyes or the nose, respectively. Previous studies have only analyzed whether humans choose the optimal fixation locations in very simple situations, such as searching for a square among a set of circles. Therefore, it is not known how efficient we are at optimizing our rapid eye movements to extract high-level information from visual scenes, such as determining whether an image of fur belongs to a cheetah or a zebra. Yang, Lengyel and Wolpert developed a mathematical model that determines the amount of information that can be extracted from an image by any set of fixation locations. The model could also work out the next best fixation location that would maximize the amount of information that could be collected. This model shows that humans are about 70% efficient in planning each eye movement. Furthermore, it suggests that the inefficiencies are largely caused by imperfect vision and inaccurate eye movements. Yang, Lengyel and Wolpert’s findings indicate that we combine information from multiple locations to direct our eye movements so that we can maximize the information we collect from our surroundings. The next challenge is to extend this mathematical model and experimental approach to even more complex visual tasks, such as judging an individual’s intentions, or working out the relationships between people in real-life settings. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12215.002
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Scott Cheng-Hsin Yang
- Computational and Biological Learning Lab, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Máté Lengyel
- Computational and Biological Learning Lab, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom.,Department of Cognitive Science, Central European University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Daniel M Wolpert
- Computational and Biological Learning Lab, Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
468
|
Transcranial Stimulation over Frontopolar Cortex Elucidates the Choice Attributes and Neural Mechanisms Used to Resolve Exploration-Exploitation Trade-Offs. J Neurosci 2016; 35:14544-56. [PMID: 26511245 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2322-15.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Optimal behavior requires striking a balance between exploiting tried-and-true options or exploring new possibilities. Neuroimaging studies have identified different brain regions in humans where neural activity is correlated with exploratory or exploitative behavior, but it is unclear whether this activity directly implements these choices or simply reflects a byproduct of the behavior. Moreover, it remains unknown whether arbitrating between exploration and exploitation can be influenced with exogenous methods, such as brain stimulation. In our study, we addressed these questions by selectively upregulating and downregulating neuronal excitability with anodal or cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over right frontopolar cortex during a reward-learning task. This caused participants to make slower, more exploratory or faster, more exploitative decisions, respectively. Bayesian computational modeling revealed that stimulation affected how much participants took both expected and obtained rewards into account when choosing to exploit or explore: Cathodal stimulation resulted in an increased focus on the option expected to yield the highest payout, whereas anodal stimulation led to choices that were less influenced by anticipated payoff magnitudes and were more driven by recent negative reward prediction errors. These findings suggest that exploration is triggered by a neural mechanism that is sensitive to prior less-than-expected choice outcomes and thus pushes people to seek out alternative courses of action. Together, our findings establish a parsimonious neurobiological mechanism that causes exploration and exploitation, and they provide new insights into the choice features used by this mechanism to direct decision-making.
Collapse
|
469
|
Oud B, Krajbich I, Miller K, Cheong JH, Botvinick M, Fehr E. Irrational time allocation in decision-making. Proc Biol Sci 2016; 283:20151439. [PMID: 26763695 PMCID: PMC4721081 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2015] [Accepted: 12/01/2015] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Time is an extremely valuable resource but little is known about the efficiency of time allocation in decision-making. Empirical evidence suggests that in many ecologically relevant situations, decision difficulty and the relative reward from making a correct choice, compared to an incorrect one, are inversely linked, implying that it is optimal to use relatively less time for difficult choice problems. This applies, in particular, to value-based choices, in which the relative reward from choosing the higher valued item shrinks as the values of the other options get closer to the best option and are thus more difficult to discriminate. Here, we experimentally show that people behave sub-optimally in such contexts. They do not respond to incentives that favour the allocation of time to choice problems in which the relative reward for choosing the best option is high; instead they spend too much time on problems in which the reward difference between the options is low. We demonstrate this by showing that it is possible to improve subjects' time allocation with a simple intervention that cuts them off when their decisions take too long. Thus, we provide a novel form of evidence that organisms systematically spend their valuable time in an inefficient way, and simultaneously offer a potential solution to the problem.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bastiaan Oud
- Department of Economics and Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research, University of Zurich, Blümlisalpstrasse 10, Zurich 8006, Switzerland
| | - Ian Krajbich
- Department of Economics and Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research, University of Zurich, Blümlisalpstrasse 10, Zurich 8006, Switzerland Department of Economics and Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| | - Kevin Miller
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA
| | - Jin Hyun Cheong
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA
| | - Matthew Botvinick
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA
| | - Ernst Fehr
- Department of Economics and Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research, University of Zurich, Blümlisalpstrasse 10, Zurich 8006, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
470
|
Holmes WR, Trueblood JS, Heathcote A. A new framework for modeling decisions about changing information: The Piecewise Linear Ballistic Accumulator model. Cogn Psychol 2016; 85:1-29. [PMID: 26760448 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.11.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2015] [Revised: 09/25/2015] [Accepted: 11/28/2015] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
In the real world, decision making processes must be able to integrate non-stationary information that changes systematically while the decision is in progress. Although theories of decision making have traditionally been applied to paradigms with stationary information, non-stationary stimuli are now of increasing theoretical interest. We use a random-dot motion paradigm along with cognitive modeling to investigate how the decision process is updated when a stimulus changes. Participants viewed a cloud of moving dots, where the motion switched directions midway through some trials, and were asked to determine the direction of motion. Behavioral results revealed a strong delay effect: after presentation of the initial motion direction there is a substantial time delay before the changed motion information is integrated into the decision process. To further investigate the underlying changes in the decision process, we developed a Piecewise Linear Ballistic Accumulator model (PLBA). The PLBA is efficient to simulate, enabling it to be fit to participant choice and response-time distribution data in a hierarchal modeling framework using a non-parametric approximate Bayesian algorithm. Consistent with behavioral results, PLBA fits confirmed the presence of a long delay between presentation and integration of new stimulus information, but did not support increased response caution in reaction to the change. We also found the decision process was not veridical, as symmetric stimulus change had an asymmetric effect on the rate of evidence accumulation. Thus, the perceptual decision process was slow to react to, and underestimated, new contrary motion information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William R Holmes
- Department of Physics and Astronomy, Vanderbilt University, 37212, United States.,Department of Mathematics, University of Melbourne, Australia
| | - Jennifer S Trueblood
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 37212, United States.,Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, 92697, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
471
|
Vu TMH, Tu VP, Duerrschmid K. Design factors influence consumers’ gazing behaviour and decision time in an eye-tracking test: A study on food images. Food Qual Prefer 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.05.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
472
|
Testing necessary regional frontal contributions to value assessment and fixation-based updating. Nat Commun 2015; 6:10120. [PMID: 26658289 PMCID: PMC4682105 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2015] [Accepted: 11/05/2015] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Value-based decisions are biased by the time people spend viewing each option: Options fixated longer are chosen more often, even when previously rated as less appealing. This bias is thought to reflect ‘value updating' as new evidence is accumulated. Prior work has shown that ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC) carries a fixation-dependent value comparison signal, while other studies implicate dorsomedial PFC in representing the value of alternative options. Here, we test whether these regions are necessary for fixation-related value updating in 33 people with frontal lobe damage and 27 healthy controls performing a simple choice task. We show that damage to dorsomedial PFC leads to an exaggerated influence of fixations on choice, while damage to ventromedial or lateral PFC has no effect on this bias. These findings suggest a critical role for dorsomedial, and not ventromedial PFC, in mediating the relative influence of current fixations and a priori value on choice. In value-based decisions, the longer one fixates on an option, the more likely one is to choose it. Here, the authors compare the performance of people with focal frontal lobe damage in a simple choice task and show that damage to the dorsomedial PFC leads to exaggerated fixation-related value updating.
Collapse
|
473
|
Hunt LT, Behrens TEJ, Hosokawa T, Wallis JD, Kennerley SW. Capturing the temporal evolution of choice across prefrontal cortex. eLife 2015; 4. [PMID: 26653139 PMCID: PMC4718814 DOI: 10.7554/elife.11945] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2015] [Accepted: 11/18/2015] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Activity in prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been richly described using economic models of choice. Yet such descriptions fail to capture the dynamics of decision formation. Describing dynamic neural processes has proven challenging due to the problem of indexing the internal state of PFC and its trial-by-trial variation. Using primate neurophysiology and human magnetoencephalography, we here recover a single-trial index of PFC internal states from multiple simultaneously recorded PFC subregions. This index can explain the origins of neural representations of economic variables in PFC. It describes the relationship between neural dynamics and behaviour in both human and monkey PFC, directly bridging between human neuroimaging data and underlying neuronal activity. Moreover, it reveals a functionally dissociable interaction between orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral PFC in guiding cost-benefit decisions. We cast our observations in terms of a recurrent neural network model of choice, providing formal links to mechanistic dynamical accounts of decision-making. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11945.001 In 1848, a railroad worker named Phineas Gage suffered an accident that was to secure him a place in neuroscience lore. While constructing a new railway line, a mistimed explosion propelled an iron bar into the base of his skull, where it passed behind his left eye before exiting through the top of his head. Gage survived the accident, but those who knew him reported significant changes in his personality and behaviour. Gage’s ability to make decisions was particularly impaired by his injury. Decision-making involves weighing up the costs and benefits associated with alternative courses of action. It entails looking into the future to decide whether an anticipated reward will justify the effort or expense necessary to obtain it. This process is dependent on a region of the brain called the prefrontal cortex, the area that sustained the most damage in Phineas Gage. While many studies have shown correlations between activity in particular parts of prefrontal cortex and the outcome of decisions, little is known about how this activity evolves over time as a decision is made. To explore this process, Hunt et al. trained macaque monkeys to choose between pairs of images that were associated with specific rewards (quantities of fruit juice) and costs (either amounts of work or fixed delays). Electrode recordings revealed changes in prefrontal activity that varied over time as the monkeys deliberated over each pair of images, choosing for example between a large reward after a long delay versus a smaller reward immediately. This activity was consistent with a mathematical model of decision-making, which also explains data from brain imaging experiments in humans. This provides an important link between human data and electrode recordings in animals. However, some of the patterns of activity observed in both macaques and humans appeared to reflect the speed at which decisions were made, rather than the outcome of the decisions themselves. By extracting information about decision speed on each decision from each region, it was shown that communication between regions of prefrontal cortex changes when choices are between two different amounts of work, as opposed to two different delays. Further experiments are needed to explore this phenomenon and to determine how other brain regions interact with the prefrontal cortex to support the decision-making process. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11945.002
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurence T Hunt
- Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Timothy E J Behrens
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Oxford University, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Takayuki Hosokawa
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States.,Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Jonathan D Wallis
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States
| | - Steven W Kennerley
- Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States
| |
Collapse
|
474
|
|
475
|
Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Spaanjaars NL, Witteman CLM. The (In)visibility of Psychodiagnosticians' Expertise. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1925] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Schulte-Mecklenbeck
- Department of Business Administration; University of Bern; Switzerland
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development; Berlin Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
476
|
Mitsuda T. Preference modulates smelling behaviour in olfactory decision tasks. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1108323] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
477
|
van Giesen RI, Fischer ARH, van Dijk H, van Trijp HCM. Tracing Attitude Expressions: An Eye-Tracking Study. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1920] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Heleen van Dijk
- Marketing and Consumer Behavior Group; Wageningen University; Wageningen NL
| | | |
Collapse
|
478
|
Stewart N, Gächter S, Noguchi T, Mullett TL. Eye Movements in Strategic Choice. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015; 29:137-156. [PMID: 27513881 PMCID: PMC4959529 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2014] [Revised: 07/22/2015] [Accepted: 07/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
In risky and other multiattribute choices, the process of choosing is well described by random walk or drift diffusion models in which evidence is accumulated over time to threshold. In strategic choices, level‐k and cognitive hierarchy models have been offered as accounts of the choice process, in which people simulate the choice processes of their opponents or partners. We recorded the eye movements in 2 × 2 symmetric games including dominance‐solvable games like prisoner's dilemma and asymmetric coordination games like stag hunt and hawk–dove. The evidence was most consistent with the accumulation of payoff differences over time: we found longer duration choices with more fixations when payoffs differences were more finely balanced, an emerging bias to gaze more at the payoffs for the action ultimately chosen, and that a simple count of transitions between payoffs—whether or not the comparison is strategically informative—was strongly associated with the final choice. The accumulator models do account for these strategic choice process measures, but the level‐k and cognitive hierarchy models do not. © 2015 The Authors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Collapse
|
479
|
A Common Mechanism Underlying Food Choice and Social Decisions. PLoS Comput Biol 2015; 11:e1004371. [PMID: 26460812 PMCID: PMC4604207 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004371] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2014] [Accepted: 05/27/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
People make numerous decisions every day including perceptual decisions such as walking through a crowd, decisions over primary rewards such as what to eat, and social decisions that require balancing own and others’ benefits. The unifying principles behind choices in various domains are, however, still not well understood. Mathematical models that describe choice behavior in specific contexts have provided important insights into the computations that may underlie decision making in the brain. However, a critical and largely unanswered question is whether these models generalize from one choice context to another. Here we show that a model adapted from the perceptual decision-making domain and estimated on choices over food rewards accurately predicts choices and reaction times in four independent sets of subjects making social decisions. The robustness of the model across domains provides behavioral evidence for a common decision-making process in perceptual, primary reward, and social decision making. One critical question that concerns all disciplines involved in the study of human decision-making is whether different types of decisions are made in different ways, or whether there exists a common decision mechanism that underlies human choices. If the latter, what are the properties of that mechanism? Here we characterize a dynamical model of decision making that was initially fit to subjects making food choices but was later able to accurately predict choices and reaction times of separate groups of subjects making social decisions. The robustness of the model across different subjects, tasks, and environments supports the idea that the brain uses a consistent process for making decisions.
Collapse
|
480
|
|
481
|
Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex behavioral condition with onset during early childhood and a lifelong course in the vast majority of cases. To date, no behavioral, genetic, brain imaging, or electrophysiological test can specifically validate a clinical diagnosis of ASD. However, these medical procedures are often implemented in order to screen for syndromic forms of the disorder (i.e., autism comorbid with known medical conditions). In the last 25 years a good deal of information has been accumulated on the main components of the "endocannabinoid (eCB) system", a rather complex ensemble of lipid signals ("endocannabinoids"), their target receptors, purported transporters, and metabolic enzymes. It has been clearly documented that eCB signaling plays a key role in many human health and disease conditions of the central nervous system, thus opening the avenue to the therapeutic exploitation of eCB-oriented drugs for the treatment of psychiatric, neurodegenerative, and neuroinflammatory disorders. Here we present a modern view of the eCB system, and alterations of its main components in human patients and animal models relevant to ASD. This review will thus provide a critical perspective necessary to explore the potential exploitation of distinct elements of eCB system as targets of innovative therapeutics against ASD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bhismadev Chakrabarti
- Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
| | - Antonio Persico
- Center of Integrated Research and School of Medicine, Campus Bio-Medico University of Rome, Via Alvaro del Portillo 21, 00128, Rome, Italy
- Mafalda Luce Center for Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Milan, Italy
| | - Natalia Battista
- Faculty of Bioscience and Technology for Food, Agriculture and Environment, University of Teramo, Teramo, Italy
| | - Mauro Maccarrone
- Center of Integrated Research and School of Medicine, Campus Bio-Medico University of Rome, Via Alvaro del Portillo 21, 00128, Rome, Italy.
- European Center for Brain Research (CERC)/Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
482
|
Kwak Y, Payne JW, Cohen AL, Huettel SA. The Rational Adolescent: Strategic Information Processing during Decision Making Revealed by Eye Tracking. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2015; 36:20-30. [PMID: 26388664 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
Adolescence is often viewed as a time of irrational, risky decision-making - despite adolescents' competence in other cognitive domains. In this study, we examined the strategies used by adolescents (N=30) and young adults (N=47) to resolve complex, multi-outcome economic gambles. Compared to adults, adolescents were more likely to make conservative, loss-minimizing choices consistent with economic models. Eye-tracking data showed that prior to decisions, adolescents acquired more information in a more thorough manner; that is, they engaged in a more analytic processing strategy indicative of trade-offs between decision variables. In contrast, young adults' decisions were more consistent with heuristics that simplified the decision problem, at the expense of analytic precision. Collectively, these results demonstrate a counter-intuitive developmental transition in economic decision making: adolescents' decisions are more consistent with rational-choice models, while young adults more readily engage task-appropriate heuristics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Youngbin Kwak
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA, 01003
| | - John W Payne
- The Fuqua School of Business, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA, 27708
| | - Andrew L Cohen
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA, 01003
| | - Scott A Huettel
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA, 27708 ; Duke Center for Interdisciplinary Decision Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA, 27708 ; Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA, 27708
| |
Collapse
|
483
|
A Neurocomputational Model of Altruistic Choice and Its Implications. Neuron 2015; 87:451-62. [PMID: 26182424 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 147] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2014] [Revised: 03/17/2015] [Accepted: 06/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
We propose a neurocomputational model of altruistic choice and test it using behavioral and fMRI data from a task in which subjects make choices between real monetary prizes for themselves and another. We show that a multi-attribute drift-diffusion model, in which choice results from accumulation of a relative value signal that linearly weights payoffs for self and other, captures key patterns of choice, reaction time, and neural response in ventral striatum, temporoparietal junction, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. The model generates several novel insights into the nature of altruism. It explains when and why generous choices are slower or faster than selfish choices, and why they produce greater response in TPJ and vmPFC, without invoking competition between automatic and deliberative processes or reward value for generosity. It also predicts that when one's own payoffs are valued more than others', some generous acts may reflect mistakes rather than genuinely pro-social preferences.
Collapse
|
484
|
Forstmann BU, Ratcliff R, Wagenmakers EJ. Sequential Sampling Models in Cognitive Neuroscience: Advantages, Applications, and Extensions. Annu Rev Psychol 2015; 67:641-66. [PMID: 26393872 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033645] [Citation(s) in RCA: 277] [Impact Index Per Article: 30.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Sequential sampling models assume that people make speeded decisions by gradually accumulating noisy information until a threshold of evidence is reached. In cognitive science, one such model--the diffusion decision model--is now regularly used to decompose task performance into underlying processes such as the quality of information processing, response caution, and a priori bias. In the cognitive neurosciences, the diffusion decision model has recently been adopted as a quantitative tool to study the neural basis of decision making under time pressure. We present a selective overview of several recent applications and extensions of the diffusion decision model in the cognitive neurosciences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- B U Forstmann
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center, University of Amsterdam, 1018 WS Amsterdam, The Netherlands;
| | - R Ratcliff
- Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio 43210
| | - E-J Wagenmakers
- Department of Methodology, University of Amsterdam, 1018 WV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
485
|
Pärnamets P, Johansson R, Gidlöf K, Wallin A. How Information Availability Interacts with Visual Attention during Judgment and Decision Tasks. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1902] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Philip Pärnamets
- Lund University Cognitive Science; Lund Sweden
- Division of Psychology; Karolinska Institutet; Solna Sweden
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
486
|
Abstract
Research on the dynamics of reward-based, goal-directed decision making has largely focused on simple choice, where participants decide among a set of unitary, mutually exclusive options. Recent work suggests that the deliberation process underlying simple choice can be understood in terms of evidence integration: Noisy evidence in favor of each option accrues over time, until the evidence in favor of one option is significantly greater than the rest. However, real-life decisions often involve not one, but several steps of action, requiring a consideration of cumulative rewards and a sensitivity to recursive decision structure. We present results from two experiments that leveraged techniques previously applied to simple choice to shed light on the deliberation process underlying multistep choice. We interpret the results from these experiments in terms of a new computational model, which extends the evidence accumulation perspective to multiple steps of action.
Collapse
|
487
|
Franco‐Watkins AM, Mattson RE, Jackson MD. Now or Later? Attentional Processing and Intertemporal Choice. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1895] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
488
|
Gluth S, Sommer T, Rieskamp J, Büchel C. Effective Connectivity between Hippocampus and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Controls Preferential Choices from Memory. Neuron 2015; 86:1078-1090. [PMID: 25996135 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2014] [Revised: 02/27/2015] [Accepted: 04/20/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Although many preferential choices in everyday life require remembering relevant information, the interplay of neural systems mediating decisions and memory has rarely been studied. We addressed this question by combining a task, in which choice options had to be retrieved from memory, with cognitive modeling and fMRI. We found that memory-guided decisions are captured by established process models of choice (sequential sampling models) but constrained by forgetting. People are biased toward remembered options and reject them only if they are very unattractive. Using a Bayesian modeling approach, we determined the posterior probability that options were remembered given the observed choices. This probability correlated with hippocampal activation during encoding. During decision making, the bias toward remembered options was linked to increased connectivity between hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Our results provide insights into the dependency of decisions on memory constraints and show that memory-related activation can be inferred from decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Gluth
- Department of Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistrasse 52, D-20246, Hamburg, Germany; Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Missionsstrasse 62a, CH-4055, Basel, Switzerland.
| | - Tobias Sommer
- Department of Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistrasse 52, D-20246, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Jörg Rieskamp
- Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Missionsstrasse 62a, CH-4055, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Christian Büchel
- Department of Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistrasse 52, D-20246, Hamburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
489
|
Ding L. Distinct dynamics of ramping activity in the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 2015. [PMID: 26224774 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00395.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The prefronto-striatal network is involved in many cognitive functions, including perceptual decision making and reward-modulated behaviors. For well-trained subjects, neural responses frequently show similar patterns in the prefrontal cortex and striatum, making it difficult to tease apart distinct regional contributions. Here I show that, despite similar mean firing rate patterns, prefrontal and striatal responses differ in other temporal dynamics for both perceptual and reward-based tasks. Compared with simulation results, the temporal dynamics of prefrontal activity are consistent with an accumulation of sensory evidence used to solve a perceptual task but not with an accumulation of reward context-related information used for the development of a reward bias. In contrast, the dynamics of striatal activity is consistent with an accumulation of reward context-related information and with an accumulation of sensory evidence during early stimulus viewing. These results suggest that prefrontal and striatal neurons may have specialized functions for different tasks even with similar average activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Long Ding
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
| |
Collapse
|
490
|
Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference. Nat Commun 2015; 6:7455. [PMID: 26135809 PMCID: PMC4500827 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms8455] [Citation(s) in RCA: 132] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2015] [Accepted: 05/11/2015] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Do people intuitively favour certain actions over others? In some dual-process research, reaction-time (RT) data have been used to infer that certain choices are intuitive. However, the use of behavioural or biological measures to infer mental function, popularly known as ‘reverse inference', is problematic because it does not take into account other sources of variability in the data, such as discriminability of the choice options. Here we use two example data sets obtained from value-based choice experiments to demonstrate that, after controlling for discriminability (that is, strength-of-preference), there is no evidence that one type of choice is systematically faster than the other. Moreover, using specific variations of a prominent value-based choice experiment, we are able to predictably replicate, eliminate or reverse previously reported correlations between RT and selfishness. Thus, our findings shed crucial light on the use of RT in inferring mental processes and strongly caution against using RT differences as evidence favouring dual-process accounts. In cognitive neuroscience, it is common practice to use reaction time data to infer whether decisions are intuitive or deliberate. Here the authors demonstrate that they can replicate, eliminate and reverse previously reported correlations between selfishness and reaction time.
Collapse
|
491
|
Abstract
Rewards are crucial objects that induce learning, approach behavior, choices, and emotions. Whereas emotions are difficult to investigate in animals, the learning function is mediated by neuronal reward prediction error signals which implement basic constructs of reinforcement learning theory. These signals are found in dopamine neurons, which emit a global reward signal to striatum and frontal cortex, and in specific neurons in striatum, amygdala, and frontal cortex projecting to select neuronal populations. The approach and choice functions involve subjective value, which is objectively assessed by behavioral choices eliciting internal, subjective reward preferences. Utility is the formal mathematical characterization of subjective value and a prime decision variable in economic choice theory. It is coded as utility prediction error by phasic dopamine responses. Utility can incorporate various influences, including risk, delay, effort, and social interaction. Appropriate for formal decision mechanisms, rewards are coded as object value, action value, difference value, and chosen value by specific neurons. Although all reward, reinforcement, and decision variables are theoretical constructs, their neuronal signals constitute measurable physical implementations and as such confirm the validity of these concepts. The neuronal reward signals provide guidance for behavior while constraining the free will to act.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wolfram Schultz
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
492
|
Abstract
Multiple attribute search is a central feature of economic life: we consider much more than price when purchasing a home, and more than wage when choosing a job. An experiment is conducted in order to explore the effects of cognitive limitations on choice in these rich settings, in accordance with the predictions of a new model of search memory load. In each task, subjects are made to search the same information in one of two orders, which differ in predicted memory load. Despite standard models of choice treating such variations in order of acquisition as irrelevant, lower predicted memory load search orders are found to lead to substantially fewer choice errors. An implication of the result for search behavior, more generally, is that in order to reduce memory load (thus choice error) a limited memory searcher ought to deviate from the search path of an unlimited memory searcher in predictable ways-a mechanism that can explain the systematic deviations from optimal sequential search that have recently been discovered in peoples' behavior. Further, as cognitive load is induced endogenously (within the task), and found to affect choice behavior, this result contributes to the cognitive load literature (in which load is induced exogenously), as well as the cognitive ability literature (in which cognitive ability is measured in a separate task). In addition, while the information overload literature has focused on the detrimental effects of the quantity of information on choice, this result suggests that, holding quantity constant, the order that information is observed in is an essential determinant of choice failure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam Sanjurjo
- Fundamentos del Análisis Económico, Universidad de Alicante, Alicante, Spain
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
493
|
Rustichini A, Padoa-Schioppa C. A neuro-computational model of economic decisions. J Neurophysiol 2015; 114:1382-98. [PMID: 26063776 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00184.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2015] [Accepted: 06/05/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuronal recordings and lesion studies indicate that key aspects of economic decisions take place in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Previous work identified in this area three groups of neurons encoding the offer value, the chosen value, and the identity of the chosen good. An important and open question is whether and how decisions could emerge from a neural circuit formed by these three populations. Here we adapted a biophysically realistic neural network previously proposed for perceptual decisions (Wang XJ. Neuron 36: 955-968, 2002; Wong KF, Wang XJ. J Neurosci 26: 1314-1328, 2006). The domain of economic decisions is significantly broader than that for which the model was originally designed, yet the model performed remarkably well. The input and output nodes of the network were naturally mapped onto two groups of cells in OFC. Surprisingly, the activity of interneurons in the network closely resembled that of the third group of cells, namely, chosen value cells. The model reproduced several phenomena related to the neuronal origins of choice variability. It also generated testable predictions on the excitatory/inhibitory nature of different neuronal populations and on their connectivity. Some aspects of the empirical data were not reproduced, but simple extensions of the model could overcome these limitations. These results render a biologically credible model for the neuronal mechanisms of economic decisions. They demonstrate that choices could emerge from the activity of cells in the OFC, suggesting that chosen value cells directly participate in the decision process. Importantly, Wang's model provides a platform to investigate the implications of neuroscience results for economic theory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aldo Rustichini
- Department of Economics, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota; and
| | - Camillo Padoa-Schioppa
- Departments of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Economics, and Biomedical Engineering, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri
| |
Collapse
|
494
|
|
495
|
Westbrook A, Braver TS. Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. COGNITIVE, AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2015; 15:395-415. [PMID: 25673005 PMCID: PMC4445645 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-015-0334-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 263] [Impact Index Per Article: 29.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive effort has been implicated in numerous theories regarding normal and aberrant behavior and the physiological response to engagement with demanding tasks. Yet, despite broad interest, no unifying, operational definition of cognitive effort itself has been proposed. Here, we argue that the most intuitive and epistemologically valuable treatment is in terms of effort-based decision-making, and advocate a neuroeconomics-focused research strategy. We first outline psychological and neuroscientific theories of cognitive effort. Then we describe the benefits of a neuroeconomic research strategy, highlighting how it affords greater inferential traction than do traditional markers of cognitive effort, including self-reports and physiologic markers of autonomic arousal. Finally, we sketch a future series of studies that can leverage the full potential of the neuroeconomic approach toward understanding the cognitive and neural mechanisms that give rise to phenomenal, subjective cognitive effort.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew Westbrook
- Department of Psychology, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, MO, 63130, USA,
| | | |
Collapse
|
496
|
Reappraising abstract paintings after exposure to background information. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0124159. [PMID: 25945789 PMCID: PMC4422661 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2013] [Accepted: 03/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Can knowledge help viewers when they appreciate an artwork? Experts’ judgments of the aesthetic value of a painting often differ from the estimates of naïve viewers, and this phenomenon is especially pronounced in the aesthetic judgment of abstract paintings. We compared the changes in aesthetic judgments of naïve viewers while they were progressively exposed to five pieces of background information. The participants were asked to report their aesthetic judgments of a given painting after each piece of information was presented. We found that commentaries by the artist and a critic significantly increased the subjective aesthetic ratings. Does knowledge enable experts to attend to the visual features in a painting and to link it to the evaluative conventions, thus potentially causing different aesthetic judgments? To investigate whether a specific pattern of attention is essential for the knowledge-based appreciation, we tracked the eye movements of subjects while viewing a painting with a commentary by the artist and with a commentary by a critic. We observed that critics’ commentaries directed the viewers’ attention to the visual components that were highly relevant to the presented commentary. However, attention to specific features of a painting was not necessary for increasing the subjective aesthetic judgment when the artists’ commentary was presented. Our results suggest that at least two different cognitive mechanisms may be involved in knowledge- guided aesthetic judgments while viewers reappraise a painting.
Collapse
|
497
|
Piantadosi ST, Hayden BY. Utility-free heuristic models of two-option choice can mimic predictions of utility-stage models under many conditions. Front Neurosci 2015; 9:105. [PMID: 25914613 PMCID: PMC4391032 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/24/2014] [Accepted: 03/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Economists often model choices as if decision-makers assign each option a scalar value variable, known as utility, and then select the option with the highest utility. It remains unclear whether as-if utility models describe real mental and neural steps in choice. Although choices alone cannot prove the existence of a utility stage, utility transformations are often taken to provide the most parsimonious or psychologically plausible explanation for choice data. Here, we show that it is possible to mathematically transform a large set of common utility-stage two-option choice models (specifically ones in which dimensions are can be decomposed into additive functions) into a heuristic model (specifically, a dimensional prioritization heuristic) that has no utility computation stage. We then show that under a range of plausible assumptions, both classes of model predict similar neural responses. These results highlight the difficulties in using neuroeconomic data to infer the existence of a value stage in choice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven T Piantadosi
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester Rochester, NY, USA
| | - Benjamin Y Hayden
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester Rochester, NY, USA ; Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester Rochester, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
498
|
Gharib A, Mier D, Adolphs R, Shimojo S. Eyetracking of social preference choices reveals normal but faster processing in autism. Neuropsychologia 2015; 72:70-9. [PMID: 25921868 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2014] [Revised: 04/05/2015] [Accepted: 04/24/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
People with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) have been reported to show atypical attention and evaluative processing, in particular for social stimuli such as faces. The usual measure in these studies is an explicit, subjective judgment, which is the culmination of complex-temporally extended processes that are not typically dissected in detail. Here we addressed a neglected aspect of social decision-making in order to gain further insight into the underlying mechanisms: the temporal evolution of the choice. We investigated this issue by quantifying the alternating patterns of gaze onto faces, as well as nonsocial stimuli, while subjects had to decide which of the two stimuli they preferred. Surprisingly, the temporal profile of fixations relating to choice (the so-called "gaze cascade") was entirely normal in ASD, as were the eventual preference choices. Despite these similarities, we found two key abnormalities: people with ASD made choices more rapidly than did control subjects across the board, and their reaction times for social preference judgments were insensitive to choice difficulty. We suggest that ASD features an altered decision-making process when basing choice on social preferences. One hypothesis motivated by these data is that a choice criterion is reached in ASD regardless of the discriminability of the options.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alma Gharib
- Division of Biology & Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA.
| | - Daniela Mier
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Central Institute of Mental Health, University of Heidelberg/Medical Faculty Mannheim, Germany
| | - Ralph Adolphs
- Division of Biology & Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA; Division of Humanities & Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
| | - Shinsuke Shimojo
- Division of Biology & Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
| |
Collapse
|
499
|
Sakellaridi S, Christova P, Christopoulos VN, Vialard A, Peponis J, Georgopoulos AP. Cognitive mechanisms underlying instructed choice exploration of small city maps. Front Neurosci 2015; 9:60. [PMID: 25852452 PMCID: PMC4367532 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2014] [Accepted: 02/11/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
We investigated the cognitive mechanisms underlying the exploration and decision-making in realistic and novel environments. Twelve human subjects were shown small circular U.S. city maps with two locations highlighted on the circumference, as possible choices for a post office (“targets”). At the beginning of a trial, subjects fixated a spot at the center of the map and ultimately chose one of the two locations. A space syntax analysis of the map paths (from the center to each target) revealed that the chosen location was associated with the less convoluted path, as if subjects navigated mentally the paths in an “ant's way,” i.e., by staying within street boundaries, and ultimately choosing the target that could be reached from the center in the shortest way, and the fewest turns and intersections. The subjects' strategy for map exploration and decision making was investigated by monitoring eye position during the task. This revealed a restricted exploration of the map delimited by the location of the two alternative options and the center of the map. Specifically, subjects explored the areas around the two target options by repeatedly looking at them before deciding which one to choose, presumably implementing an evaluation and decision-making process. The ultimate selection of a specific target was significantly associated with the time spent exploring the area around that target. Finally, an analysis of the sequence of eye fixations revealed that subjects tended to look systematically toward the target ultimately chosen even from the beginning of the trial. This finding indicates an early cognitive selection bias for the ensuing decision process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sofia Sakellaridi
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota Minneapolis, MN, USA ; Brain Sciences Center, Veterans Affairs Medical Center Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Peka Christova
- Brain Sciences Center, Veterans Affairs Medical Center Minneapolis, MN, USA ; Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota Medical School Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Vassilios N Christopoulos
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA
| | - Alice Vialard
- School of Architecture, College of Architecture, Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - John Peponis
- School of Architecture, College of Architecture, Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Apostolos P Georgopoulos
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota Minneapolis, MN, USA ; Brain Sciences Center, Veterans Affairs Medical Center Minneapolis, MN, USA ; Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota Medical School Minneapolis, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
500
|
Orquin JL, Ashby NJS, Clarke ADF. Areas of Interest as a Signal Detection Problem in Behavioral Eye-Tracking Research. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1867] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jacob L. Orquin
- Department of Business Administration/MAPP; Aarhus University; Aarhus Denmark
| | - Nathaniel J. S. Ashby
- Department of Social and Decision Sciences; Carnegie Mellon University; Pittsburgh PA USA
| | | |
Collapse
|