51
|
Gaze-centered spatial updating of reach targets across different memory delays. Vision Res 2011; 51:890-7. [PMID: 21219923 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2010] [Revised: 11/26/2010] [Accepted: 12/22/2010] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has demonstrated that remembered targets for reaching are coded and updated relative to gaze, at least when the reaching movement is made soon after the target has been extinguished. In this study, we want to test whether reach targets are updated relative to gaze following different time delays. Reaching endpoints systematically varied as a function of gaze relative to target irrespective of whether the action was executed immediately or after a delay of 5 s, 8 s or 12 s. The present results suggest that memory traces for reach targets continue to be coded in a gaze-dependent reference frame if no external cues are present.
Collapse
|
52
|
Trojan J, Stolle AM, Carl AM, Kleinböhl D, Tan HZ, Hölzl R. Spatiotemporal integration in somatosensory perception: effects of sensory saltation on pointing at perceived positions on the body surface. Front Psychol 2010; 1:206. [PMID: 21833262 PMCID: PMC3153812 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00206] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2010] [Accepted: 11/01/2010] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In the past, sensory saltation phenomena (Geldard and Sherrick, 1972) have been used repeatedly to analyze the spatiotemporal integration capacity of somatosensory and other sensory mechanisms by means of their psychophysical characteristic. The core phenomenon consists in a systematic mislocalization of one tactile stimulus (the attractee) toward another successive tactile stimulus (the attractant) presented at another location, increasing with shorter intervals. In a series of four experiments, sensory saltation characteristics were studied at the forearm and the abdomen. Participants reported the perceived positions of attractees, attractants, and reference stimuli by pointing. In general, saltation characteristics compared well to those reported in previous studies, but we were able to gain several new insights regarding this phenomenon: (a) the attractee–attractant interval did not exclusively affect the perceived attractee position, but also the perceived attractant position; (b) saltation characteristics were very similar at different body sites and orientations, but did show differences suggesting anisotropy (direction-dependency) in the underlying integration processes; (c) sensory saltation could be elicited with stimulation patterns crossing the body midline on the abdomen. In addition to the saltation-specific results, our experiments demonstrate that pointing reports of perceived positions on the body surface generally show pronounced systematic biases compared to veridical positions, moderate intraindividual consistency, and a high degree of inter-individual variability. Finally, we address methodological and terminological controversies concerning the sensory saltation paradigm and discuss its possible neurophysiological basis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jörg Trojan
- Otto Selz Institute for Applied Psychology, University of Mannheim Mannheim, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
53
|
Optic ataxia and Bálint's syndrome: neuropsychological and neurophysiological prospects. HANDBOOK OF CLINICAL NEUROLOGY 2010. [PMID: 18631703 DOI: 10.1016/s0072-9752(07)88020-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|
54
|
Hesse C, Franz VH. Grasping remembered objects: Exponential decay of the visual memory. Vision Res 2010; 50:2642-50. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2010] [Revised: 07/26/2010] [Accepted: 07/29/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
55
|
van Wermeskerken M, van der Kamp J, Te Velde AF, Valero-Garcia AV, Hoozemans MJM, Savelsbergh GJP. Anticipatory reaching of seven- to eleven-month-old infants in occlusion situations. Infant Behav Dev 2010; 34:45-54. [PMID: 21036402 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2010] [Revised: 09/08/2010] [Accepted: 09/21/2010] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined 7- to 11-month-old infants' anticipatory and reactive reaching for temporarily occluded objects. Infants were presented with laterally approaching objects that moved at different velocities (10, 20, and 40 cm/s) in different occlusion situations (no-, 20 cm-, and 40 cm-occlusion), resulting in occlusion durations ranging between 0 and 4s. Results show that except for object velocity and occlusion distance, occlusion duration was a critical constraint for infants' reaching behaviors. We found that the older infants reached more often, but that an increase in occlusion duration resulted in a decline in reaching frequency that was similar across age groups. Anticipatory reaching declined with increasing occlusion duration, but the adverse effects for longer occlusion durations diminished with age. It is concluded that with increasing age infants are able to retain and use information to guide reaching movements over longer periods of non-visibility, providing support for the graded representation hypothesis (Jonsson & von Hofsten, 2003) and the two-visual systems model (Milner & Goodale, 1995).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margot van Wermeskerken
- Research Institute MOVE, Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
56
|
van Gaal S, Lamme VAF, Ridderinkhof KR. Unconsciously triggered conflict adaptation. PLoS One 2010; 5:e11508. [PMID: 20634898 PMCID: PMC2901348 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011508] [Citation(s) in RCA: 76] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2010] [Accepted: 06/16/2010] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In conflict tasks such as the Stroop, the Eriksen flanker or the Simon task, it is generally observed that the detection of conflict in the current trial reduces the impact of conflicting information in the subsequent trial; a phenomenon termed conflict adaptation. This higher-order cognitive control function has been assumed to be restricted to cases where conflict is experienced consciously. In the present experiment we manipulated the awareness of conflict-inducing stimuli in a metacontrast masking paradigm to directly test this assumption. Conflicting response tendencies were elicited either consciously (through primes that were weakly masked) or unconsciously (strongly masked primes). We demonstrate trial-by-trial conflict adaptation effects after conscious as well as unconscious conflict, which could not be explained by direct stimulus/response repetitions. These findings show that unconscious information can have a longer-lasting influence on our behavior than previously thought and further stretch the functional boundaries of unconscious cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simon van Gaal
- Cognitive Neuroscience Group, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
57
|
Pagano CC, Grutzmacher RP, Jenkins JC. Comparing Verbal and Reaching Responses to Visually Perceived Egocentric Distances. ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2010. [DOI: 10.1207/s15326969eco1303_2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
58
|
Crossmodal interaction between the mental number line and peripersonal haptic space representation in sighted and blind individuals. Atten Percept Psychophys 2010; 72:885-90. [DOI: 10.3758/app.72.4.885] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
59
|
Monaco S, Króliczak G, Quinlan DJ, Fattori P, Galletti C, Goodale MA, Culham JC. Contribution of visual and proprioceptive information to the precision of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 2009; 202:15-32. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-2106-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2009] [Accepted: 11/14/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
60
|
Pisella L, Sergio L, Blangero A, Torchin H, Vighetto A, Rossetti Y. Optic ataxia and the function of the dorsal stream: Contributions to perception and action. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47:3033-44. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 96] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2008] [Revised: 06/06/2009] [Accepted: 06/21/2009] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
61
|
Cohen MX, van Gaal S, Ridderinkhof KR, Lamme VAF. Unconscious errors enhance prefrontal-occipital oscillatory synchrony. Front Hum Neurosci 2009; 3:54. [PMID: 19956401 PMCID: PMC2786300 DOI: 10.3389/neuro.09.054.2009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2009] [Accepted: 11/05/2009] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
The medial prefrontal cortex (MFC) is critical for our ability to learn from previous mistakes. Here we provide evidence that neurophysiological oscillatory long-range synchrony is a mechanism of post-error adaptation that occurs even without conscious awareness of the error. During a visually signaled Go/No-Go task in which half of the No-Go cues were masked and thus not consciously perceived, response errors enhanced tonic (i.e., over 1–2 s) oscillatory synchrony between MFC and occipital cortex (OCC) leading up to and during the subsequent trial. Spectral Granger causality analyses demonstrated that MFC → OCC directional synchrony was enhanced during trials following both conscious and unconscious errors, whereas transient stimulus-induced occipital → MFC directional synchrony was independent of errors in the previous trial. Further, the strength of pre-trial MFC-occipital synchrony predicted individual differences in task performance. Together, these findings suggest that synchronous neurophysiological oscillations are a plausible mechanism of MFC-driven cognitive control that is independent of conscious awareness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael X Cohen
- Amsterdam Center for the Study of Adaptive Control in Brain and Behavior, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
62
|
Abstract
The perception-action model proposes that vision-for-perception and vision-for-action are based on anatomically distinct and functionally independent streams within the visual cortex. This idea can account for diverse experimental findings, and has been hugely influential over the past two decades. The model itself comprises a set of core contrasts between the functional properties of the two visual streams. We critically review the evidence for these contrasts, arguing that each of them has either been refuted or found limited empirical support. We suggest that the perception-action model captures some broad patterns of functional localization, but that the specializations of the two streams are relative, not absolute. The ubiquity and extent of inter-stream interactions suggest that we should reject the idea that the ventral and dorsal streams are functionally independent processing pathways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Schenk
- a Wolfson Research Institute, Durham University , Stockton on Tees , UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
63
|
"Real-time" obstacle avoidance in the absence of primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106:15996-6001. [PMID: 19805240 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0905549106] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
When we reach toward objects, we easily avoid potential obstacles located in the workspace. Previous studies suggest that obstacle avoidance relies on mechanisms in the dorsal visual stream in the posterior parietal cortex. One fundamental question that remains unanswered is where the visual inputs to these dorsal-stream mechanisms are coming from. Here, we provide compelling evidence that these mechanisms can operate in "real-time" without direct input from primary visual cortex (V1). In our first experiment, we used a reaching task to demonstrate that an individual with a dense left visual field hemianopia after damage to V1 remained strikingly sensitive to the position of unseen static obstacles placed in his blind field. Importantly, in a second experiment, we showed that his sensitivity to the same obstacles in his blind field was abolished when a short 2-s delay (without vision) was introduced before reach onset. These findings have far-reaching implications, not only for our understanding of the time constraints under which different visual pathways operate, but also in relation to how these seemingly "primitive" subcortical visual pathways can control complex everyday behavior without recourse to conscious vision.
Collapse
|
64
|
Gaillard R, Dehaene S, Adam C, Clémenceau S, Hasboun D, Baulac M, Cohen L, Naccache L. Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biol 2009; 7:e61. [PMID: 19296722 PMCID: PMC2656551 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 264] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2008] [Accepted: 02/02/2009] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
We compared conscious and nonconscious processing of briefly flashed words using a visual masking procedure while recording intracranial electroencephalogram (iEEG) in ten patients. Nonconscious processing of masked words was observed in multiple cortical areas, mostly within an early time window (<300 ms), accompanied by induced gamma-band activity, but without coherent long-distance neural activity, suggesting a quickly dissipating feedforward wave. In contrast, conscious processing of unmasked words was characterized by the convergence of four distinct neurophysiological markers: sustained voltage changes, particularly in prefrontal cortex, large increases in spectral power in the gamma band, increases in long-distance phase synchrony in the beta range, and increases in long-range Granger causality. We argue that all of those measures provide distinct windows into the same distributed state of conscious processing. These results have a direct impact on current theoretical discussions concerning the neural correlates of conscious access.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raphaël Gaillard
- INSERM, Cognitive Neuro-imaging Unit, Institut Fédératif de Recherche (IFR) 49, Gif sur Yvette, France
- Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire de Santé Mentale et de Thérapeutique, Paris, France
- Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France
| | - Stanislas Dehaene
- INSERM, Cognitive Neuro-imaging Unit, Institut Fédératif de Recherche (IFR) 49, Gif sur Yvette, France
- CEA, I2BM, NeuroSpin center, Gif sur Yvette, France
- Collège de France, Paris, France
| | - Claude Adam
- Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Pôle des Maladies du Système Nerveux, Paris, France
| | - Stéphane Clémenceau
- Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Pôle des Maladies du Système Nerveux, Paris, France
| | - Dominique Hasboun
- Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Pôle des Maladies du Système Nerveux, Paris, France
- Université Pierre et Marie Curie Paris 6, Département de Physiologie, Paris, France
| | - Michel Baulac
- Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Pôle des Maladies du Système Nerveux, Paris, France
- Université Pierre et Marie Curie Paris 6, Département de Physiologie, Paris, France
| | - Laurent Cohen
- INSERM, Cognitive Neuro-imaging Unit, Institut Fédératif de Recherche (IFR) 49, Gif sur Yvette, France
- Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Pôle des Maladies du Système Nerveux, Paris, France
- Université Pierre et Marie Curie Paris 6, Département de Physiologie, Paris, France
| | - Lionel Naccache
- INSERM, Cognitive Neuro-imaging Unit, Institut Fédératif de Recherche (IFR) 49, Gif sur Yvette, France
- Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Pôle des Maladies du Système Nerveux, Paris, France
- Université Pierre et Marie Curie Paris 6, Département de Physiologie, Paris, France
- * To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Hesse C, de Grave DDJ, Franz VH, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. Planning movements well in advance. Cogn Neuropsychol 2009; 25:985-95. [PMID: 18608330 DOI: 10.1080/02643290701862399] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
It has been suggested that the metrics of grasping movements directed to visible objects are controlled in real time and are therefore unaffected by previous experience. We tested whether the properties of a visually presented distractor object influence the kinematics of a subsequent grasping movement performed under full vision. After viewing an elliptical distractor object in one of two different orientations participants grasped a target object, which was either the same object with the same orientation or a circular object without obvious orientation. When grasping the circular target, grip orientation was influenced by the orientation of the distractor. Moreover, as in classical visuomotor priming, grasping movements were initiated faster when distractor and target were identical. Results provide evidence that planning of visually guided grasping movements is influenced by prior perceptual experience, challenging the notion that metric aspects of grasping are controlled exclusively on the basis of real-time information.
Collapse
|
66
|
Hesse C, Franz VH. Memory mechanisms in grasping. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47:1532-45. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2008] [Revised: 06/24/2008] [Accepted: 08/09/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
67
|
Contributions of vision–proprioception interactions to the estimation of time-varying hand and target locations. Exp Brain Res 2009; 195:371-82. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-1798-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2008] [Accepted: 04/02/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
68
|
Shifts of visuospatial attention to invisible (metacontrast-masked) singletons: Clues from reaction times and event-related potential. Adv Cogn Psychol 2009. [DOI: 10.2478/v10053-008-0045-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
69
|
Heath M, Maraj A, Godbolt B, Binsted G. Action without awareness: reaching to an object you do not remember seeing. PLoS One 2008; 3:e3539. [PMID: 18953411 PMCID: PMC2568811 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003539] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2008] [Accepted: 09/30/2008] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Previous work by our group has shown that the scaling of reach trajectories to target size is independent of obligatory awareness of that target property and that “action without awareness” can persist for up to 2000 ms of visual delay. In the present investigation we sought to determine if the ability to scale reaching trajectories to target size following a delay is related to the pre-computing of movement parameters during initial stimulus presentation or the maintenance of a sensory (i.e., visual) representation for on-demand response parameterization. Methodology/Principal Findings Participants completed immediate or delayed (i.e., 2000 ms) perceptual reports and reaching responses to different sized targets under non-masked and masked target conditions. For the reaching task, the limb associated with a trial (i.e., left or right) was not specified until the time of response cuing: a manipulation that prevented participants from pre-computing the effector-related parameters of their response. In terms of the immediate and delayed perceptual tasks, target size was accurately reported during non-masked trials; however, for masked trials only a chance level of accuracy was observed. For the immediate and delayed reaching tasks, movement time as well as other temporal kinematic measures (e.g., times to peak acceleration, velocity and deceleration) increased in relation to decreasing target size across non-masked and masked trials. Conclusions/Significance Our results demonstrate that speed-accuracy relations were observed regardless of whether participants were aware (i.e., non-masked trials) or unaware (i.e., masked trials) of target size. Moreover, the equivalent scaling of immediate and delayed reaches during masked trials indicates that a persistent sensory-based representation supports the unconscious and metrical scaling of memory-guided reaching.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Heath
- School of Kinesiology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
70
|
Dissociating cognitive and motor interference effects on kinesthetic short-term memory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2008; 73:380-9. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-008-0159-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2008] [Accepted: 08/14/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
71
|
|
72
|
Gallace A, Spence C. The cognitive and neural correlates of “tactile consciousness”: A multisensory perspective. Conscious Cogn 2008; 17:370-407. [PMID: 17398116 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2006] [Revised: 12/23/2006] [Accepted: 01/06/2007] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
People's awareness of tactile stimuli has been investigated in far less detail than their awareness of stimuli in other sensory modalities. In an attempt to fill this gap, we provide an overview of studies that are pertinent to the topic of tactile consciousness. We discuss the results of research that has investigated phenomena such as "change blindness", phantom limb sensations, and numerosity judgments in tactile perception, together with the results obtained from the study of patients affected by deficits that can adversely affect tactile perception such as neglect, extinction, and numbsense. The similarities as well as some of the important differences that have emerged when visual and tactile conscious information processing have been compared using similar experimental procedures are highlighted. We suggest that conscious information processing in the tactile modality cannot be separated completely from the more general processing of spatial information in the brain. Finally, the importance of considering tactile consciousness within the larger framework of multisensory information processing is also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alberto Gallace
- Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
73
|
|
74
|
Betker AL, Moussavi Z, Szturm T. Effect of a visual-based sensory motor task on muscle tuning during a dynamic balance task. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF THE IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE 2008; 2008:5077-5080. [PMID: 19163858 DOI: 10.1109/iembs.2008.4650355] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/27/2023]
Abstract
In this research, we explored visually-based sensory motor learning when a transformation was applied to the trajectory used to move and track a visual target in a virtual environment. The virtual task was controlled by the subject's center of foot pressure (COP), where the COP position was mapped to an on-screen cursor. Target balloons appeared randomly on the screen; the subject was instructed to move the COP-controlled cursor to intersect and burst the balloons. A transformation was applied to the movement trajectory, which rotated the on-screen cursor counter-clockwise by 60 degrees . The transformation required the subjects to update their spatial reference coordinates between the physical COP position and the on-screen cursor. To investigate learning during the transformation, electromyogram (EMG) data was recorded from the tibialis anterior and peroneus longus muscles. The muscle activity, calculated as the root mean square (RMS) of the EMG data, was calculated for each muscle as a function of the movement direction during movement initiation. The preferred direction (PD) for each muscle was then determined as the directional sum of the RMS values. The results showed a shift in the preferred direction (PD) of the muscles with learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aimee L Betker
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB R3T 5V6 Canada.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
75
|
Malfait N, Henriques DY, Gribble PL. Shape distortion produced by isolated mismatch between vision and proprioception. J Neurophysiol 2007; 99:231-43. [PMID: 17977930 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00507.2007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To investigate the nature of the visuomotor transformation, previous studies have used pointing tasks and examined how adaptation to a spatially localized mismatch between vision and proprioception generalizes across the workspace. Whereas some studies found extensive spatial generalization of single-point remapping, consistent with the hypothesis of a global realignment of visual and proprioceptive spaces, other studies reported limited transfer associated with variations in initial limb posture. Here, we investigated the effects of spatially localized remapping in the context of a visuomanual tracking task. Subjects tracked a visual target tracing a simple two-dimensional geometrical form without visual feedback except at a single point, where the visual display of the hand was shifted relative to its actual position. After adaptation, hand paths exhibited distortions relative to the visual templates that were inconsistent with the idea of a global realignment of visual and proprioceptive spaces. Results of a visuoproprioceptive matching task showed that these distortions were not limited to active movements but also affected perception of passive limb movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole Malfait
- Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
76
|
Coello Y, Delevoye-Turrell Y. Embodiment, spatial categorisation and action. Conscious Cogn 2007; 16:667-83. [PMID: 17728152 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2007] [Revised: 06/18/2007] [Accepted: 07/01/2007] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Despite the subjective experience of a continuous and coherent external world, we will argue that the perception and categorisation of visual space is constrained by the spatial resolution of the sensory systems but also and above all, by the pre-reflective representations of the body in action. Recent empirical data in cognitive neurosciences will be presented that suggest that multidimensional categorisation of perceptual space depends on body representations at both an experiential and a functional level. Results will also be resumed that show that representations of the body in action are pre-reflective in nature as only some aspects of the pre-reflective states can be consciously experienced. Finally, a neuro-cognitive model based on the integration of afferent and efferent information will be described, which suggests that action simulation and associated predicted sensory consequences may represent the underlying principle that enables pre-reflective representations of the body for space categorisation and selection for action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yann Coello
- Laboratory URECA (EA 1059), University Charles de Gaulle-Lille3, BP 60149, F.59653 Villeneuve d'Ascq cedex, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
77
|
Dijkerman HC, de Haan EHF. Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Behav Brain Sci 2007; 30:189-201; discussion 201-39. [PMID: 17705910 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x07001392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 342] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
AbstractThe functions of the somatosensory system are multiple. We use tactile input to localize and experience the various qualities of touch, and proprioceptive information to determine the position of different parts of the body with respect to each other, which provides fundamental information for action. Further, tactile exploration of the characteristics of external objects can result in conscious perceptual experience and stimulus or object recognition. Neuroanatomical studies suggest parallel processing as well as serial processing within the cerebral somatosensory system that reflect these separate functions, with one processing stream terminating in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and the other terminating in the insula. We suggest that, analogously to the organisation of the visual system, somatosensory processing for the guidance of action can be dissociated from the processing that leads to perception and memory. In addition, we find a second division between tactile information processing about external targets in service of object recognition and tactile information processing related to the body itself. We suggest the posterior parietal cortex subserves both perception and action, whereas the insula principally subserves perceptual recognition and learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Chris Dijkerman
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Research Institute, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
78
|
Ittyerah M, Gaunet F, Rossetti Y. Pointing with the left and right hands in congenitally blind children. Brain Cogn 2007; 64:170-83. [PMID: 17408828 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2006] [Revised: 02/01/2007] [Accepted: 02/07/2007] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Congenitally blind and blindfolded sighted children at ages of 6, 8, 10 and 12 years performed a pointing task with their left and right index fingers at an array of three targets on a touch screen to immediate (0 s) and delayed (4 s) instructions. Accuracy was greater for immediate than delayed pointing and there was an effect of delay for the orientation of the main axis of the pointing distribution in both groups, indicating distinct spatial representations with development such as ego- and allocentric frames of reference, respectively. The pointing responses of the blind covered less surface area indicating better overall accuracy as compared to the sighted blindfolded. The hands differed for four of the six precision and accuracy parameters. The right hand performed better and seemed relatively contextually oriented, whereas the responses of the left hand were closer to the body and egocentrically oriented. The elongation of the scatter of the pointing responses was greater for the boys and more allocentrically oriented, indicating gender differences in spatial representation. The study provides a first evidence of ego- and allocentric spatial frames of reference in congenitally blind children and an ability to point at targets with the left and right hands in the total absence of vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miriam Ittyerah
- Centre For Development Studies, Trivandrum 695001, Kerala, India.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
79
|
Betker AL, Moussavi Z, Szturm T. Visual-based sensory motor learning during dynamic balance tasks viewed in a virtual environment. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF THE IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY SOCIETY. ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE 2007; 2007:6110-6113. [PMID: 18003409 DOI: 10.1109/iembs.2007.4353743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
In this research, we applied a transformation to the normal trajectory used to move and track a visual target in a virtual environment, in order to evaluate adaptation to a visual-based sensory motor transformation. The ability to recalibrate internal to external spatial reference frames is important when changing the relationship between the self and the environment. The virtual task was controlled by the subject's center of foot pressure (COP); the physical COP location is mapped (slaved) to an on-screen cursor (avatar). Target balloons appeared randomly on the screen and the subject was instructed to move the cursor (COP) to intersect the balloon and burst it. When the experimental transformation was applied, the trajectory of the avatar underwent a counter-clockwise rotation of 60 degrees; this required the subjects to update their spatial reference coordinates between the physical COP position and the game avatar. Two parameters were calculated in order to investigate if learning occurred: 1) the displacement angle between the COP trajectory and the direct line path between the starting COP position and target position; and 2) the maximum perpendicular displacement between the COP trajectory and the direct line path to the balloon target. The results showed a decrease in movement error with learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aimee L Betker
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB R3T 5V6 Canada.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
80
|
Brenner E, Smeets JB. Insights About Seeing. Cortex 2007. [DOI: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70482-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
81
|
van Doorn H, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP. Grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion: The contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:1939-47. [PMID: 17240408 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2006] [Revised: 11/15/2006] [Accepted: 11/16/2006] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
The present study examines the contributions of vision for perception processes in action. To this end, the influence of allocentric information on different action components (i.e., the selection of an appropriate mode of action, the pre-planning and online control of movement kinematics) is assessed. Participants (n=10) were presented with a shaft of various lengths (i.e., 13-20 cm) that was embedded in a Müller-Lyer figure. Picking up the shaft would, dependent on its length, either require a one- or a two-handed grasp. In different conditions participants were instructed to give a verbal judgement on the size of the shaft (VSJ); to make a manual estimation of the shaft's length (MLE); to indicate verbally whether they would grasp the shaft with one- or two hands (VAE); to actually grasp the shaft (G). We found that the Müller-Lyer figure affected the choice between using a one- or two-handed grasp, both when the participants actually grasped (G) the object and when they made a verbal estimation (VAE). The illusionary bias was of a similar magnitude as the one found in the verbal (VSJ) and manual perception task (MLE). The illusion had only a minor influence on the movement kinematics, and appears to be restricted to participants in which the grasping condition was immediately preceded by the VSJ-condition. We conclude that vision for perception contributes to the selection of an action mode, and that its contributions beyond that stage are dependent on the particular (experimental) circumstances.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hemke van Doorn
- Institute for Fundamental and Clinical Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
82
|
Hay L, Redon C. Response delay and spatial representation in pointing movements. Neurosci Lett 2006; 408:194-8. [PMID: 17027153 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.08.080] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/22/2006] [Revised: 08/30/2006] [Accepted: 08/31/2006] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Pointing movements decrease in accuracy when target information is removed before movement onset. This time effect was analyzed in relation with the spatial representation of the target location, which can be egocentric (i.e. in relation to the body) or exocentric (i.e. in relation to the external world) depending on the visual environment of the target. The accuracy of pointing movements performed without visual feedback was measured in two delay conditions: 0 and 5-s delay between target removal and movement onset. In each delay condition, targets were presented either in the darkness (egocentric localization) or within a structured visual background (exocentric localization). The results show that pointing was more accurate when targets were presented within a visual background than in the darkness. The time-related decrease in accuracy was observed in the darkness condition, whereas no delay effect was found in the presence of a visual background. Therefore, contextual factors applied to a simple pointing action might induce different spatial representations: a short-lived sensorimotor egocentric representation used in immediate action control, or a long-lived perceptual exocentric representation which drives perception and delayed action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurette Hay
- UMR 6149 CNRS-Université de Provence, Pôle 3 C-Case B, 3 Place Victor Hugo, 13331 Marseille Cedex 03, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
83
|
Gaunet F, Ittyerah M, Rossetti Y. Pointing at targets by children with congenital and transient blindness. Exp Brain Res 2006; 178:167-79. [PMID: 17053909 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-006-0720-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2006] [Accepted: 09/17/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The study investigated pointing at memorized targets in reachable space in congenitally blind (CB) and blindfolded sighted (BS) children (6, 8, 10 and 12 years; ten children in each group). The target locations were presented on a sagittal plane by passive positioning of the left index finger. A go signal for matching the target location with the right index finger was provided 0 or 4 s after demonstration. An age effect was found only for absolute distance errors and the surface area of pointing was smaller for the CB children. Results indicate that early visual experience and age are not predictive factors for pointing in children. The delay was an important factor at all ages and for both groups, indicating distinct spatial representations such as egocentric and allocentric frames of reference, for immediate and delayed pointing, respectively. Therefore, the CB like the BS children are able to use both ego- and allocentric frames of reference.
Collapse
|
84
|
Affiliation(s)
- Lionel Naccache
- Clinical Neurophysiology Department, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, 75013 France.
| |
Collapse
|
85
|
Pisella L, Binkofski F, Lasek K, Toni I, Rossetti Y. No double-dissociation between optic ataxia and visual agnosia: multiple sub-streams for multiple visuo-manual integrations. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44:2734-48. [PMID: 16753188 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 138] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2005] [Revised: 03/10/2006] [Accepted: 03/17/2006] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The current dominant view of the visual system is marked by the functional and anatomical dissociation between a ventral stream specialised for perception and a dorsal stream specialised for action. The "double-dissociation" between visual agnosia (VA), a deficit of visual recognition, and optic ataxia (OA), a deficit of visuo-manual guidance, considered as consecutive to ventral and dorsal damage, respectively, has provided the main argument for this dichotomic view. In the first part of this paper, we show that the currently available empirical data do not suffice to support a double-dissociation between OA and VA. In the second part, we review evidence coming from human neuropsychology and monkey data, which cast further doubts on the validity of a simple double-dissociation between perception and action because they argue for a far more complex organisation with multiple parallel visual-to-motor connections: 1. A dorso-dorsal pathway (involving the most dorsal part of the parietal and pre-motor cortices): for immediate visuo-motor control--with OA as typical disturbance. The latest research about OA is reviewed, showing how these patients exhibit deficits restricted to the most direct and fast visuo-motor transformations. We also propose that mild mirror ataxia, consisting of misreaching errors when the controlesional hand is guided to a visual goal though a mirror, could correspond to OA with an isolated "hand effect". 2. A ventral stream-prefrontal pathway (connections from the ventral visual stream to pre-frontal areas, by-passing the parietal areas): for "mediate" control (involving spatial or temporal transpositions [Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2003). Mediate responses as direct evidence for intention: Neuropsychology of Not to-, Not now- and Not there-tasks. In S. Johnson (Ed.), Cognitive Neuroscience perspectives on the problem of intentional action (pp. 67-105). MIT Press.])--with VA as typical disturbance. Preserved visuo-manual guidance in patients with VA is restricted to immediate goal-directed guidance, they exhibit deficits for delayed or pantomimed actions. 3. A ventro-dorsal pathway (involving the more ventral part of the parietal lobe and the pre-motor and pre-frontal areas): for complex planning and programming relying on high representational levels with a more bilateral organisation or an hemispheric lateralisation--with mirror apraxia, limb apraxia and spatial neglect as representatives. Mirror apraxia is a deficit that affects both hands after unilateral inferior parietal lesion with the patients reaching systematically and repeatedly toward the virtual image in the mirror. Limb apraxia is localized on a more advanced conceptual level of object-related actions and results from deficient integrative, computational and "working memory" capacities of the left inferior parietal lobule. A component of spatial working memory has recently been revealed also in spatial neglect consecutive to lesion involving the network of the right inferior parietal lobule and the right frontal areas. We conclude by pointing to the differential temporal constraints and integrative capabilities of these parallel visuo-motor pathways as keys to interpret the neuropsychological deficits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Pisella
- INSERM UMR-S 534, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, and Université Claude Bernard-Lyon, Espace et Action, Bron, France
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
86
|
Gaunet F, Rossetti Y. Effects of visual deprivation on space representation: immediate and delayed pointing toward memorised proprioceptive targets. Perception 2006; 35:107-24. [PMID: 16491713 DOI: 10.1068/p5333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Congenitally blind, late-blind, and blindfolded-sighted participants performed a pointing task at proximal memorised proprioceptive targets. The locations to be memorised were presented on a sagittal plane by passively positioning the left index finger. A 'go' signal for matching the target location with the right index finger was given 0 or 8 s after left-hand demonstration. Absolute distance errors were smaller in the blind groups, with both delays pooled together; signed distance and direction errors were underestimated with the longer delay, and were overestimated by blind groups, whereas the blindfolded-sighted group underestimated them. Elongation of the scatters was stretched but not affected by delay or group. The surface scatter was greater with the longer delay; and orientation of the main axis of the pointing ellipses shows the use of an egocentric frame of reference by the congenitally blind group for both delays, the use of egocentric (0 s) and exocentric (8 s) frame of reference by the blindfolded-sighted group, with the late-blind group using an intermediate frame of reference for both delays. Therefore, early and late visual-deprivation effects are distinguished from transient visual-deprivation effects as long-term deprivation leads to increased capabilities (absolute distance estimations), unaltered organisation (for surface and elongation), and altered organisation (amplitude and direction estimations, orientation of pointing distribution) of the spatial representation with proprioception. Besides providing an extensive exploration of pointing ability and mechanisms in the visually deprived population, the results show that cross-modal plasticity applies not only to neural bases but extends to spatial behaviour.
Collapse
|
87
|
Di Nocera F, Couyoumdjian A, Ferlazzo F. Crossing the pillars of Hercules: the role of spatial frames of reference in error making. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2006; 59:204-21. [PMID: 16556568 DOI: 10.1080/17470210500151451] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
This paper introduces a perspective accounting for errors that may occur as a result of the human interaction with the three-dimensional (3D) space. Particularly, we are interested in errors that are caused by cognitively crossing the boundary between peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Several behavioural, neurophysiological, and neuropsychological perspectives are reviewed in order to develop the theoretical framework on which our ideas are founded. We argue that cognitively crossing the boundary implies switching from one frame of reference to another, which, as a consequence, implies specific costs. The results of five experiments (overall involving 76 participants) support our hypothesis, suggesting the existence of different action-oriented spatial frames of reference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Di Nocera
- Cognitive Ergonomics Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Rome, "La Sapienza", Via dei Marsi, 78-00185 Rome, Italy.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Coello Y, Iwanow O. Effect of structuring the workspace on cognitive and sensorimotor distance estimation: No dissociation between perception and action. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2006; 68:278-89. [PMID: 16773899 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193675] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Independent processing of visual information for perception and action is supported by studies about visual illusions, which showed that context information influences overtjudgment but not reaching attempts. The objection was raised, however, that these two types of performance are notdirectly comparable, since they generally focus on different properties of the visual input. The goal of the present study was to quantify the influence of context information (in the form of a textured background) on the cognitive and sensorimotor processing of egocentric distance. We found that the subjective area comprising reachable objects (probed with a cognitive task) decreased, whereas the amplitude of reaching movement (probed with a sensorimotor task) increased in the presence of the textured background with both binocular and monocular viewing. Directional motor performance was not affected by the experimental conditions, but there was a tendency for the kinematic parameters to mimic trajectory variations. The similar but opposite effects of the textured background in the cognitive and sensorimotor tasks suggested that in both tasks the visual targets were perceived as closer when they were presented in a sparse environment. A common explanation for the opposite effects was confirmed by the percentage of background influence, which was highly correlated in the two tasks. We conclude that visual processing for perception and action cannot be dissociated from context influence, since it does not differ when the tasks entail the processing of similar spatial characteristics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yann Coello
- Laboratoire URECA, Université Charles de Gaulle, Lille, Villeneuve d'Ascq, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Carrozzo M, Koch G, Turriziani P, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA, Lacquaniti F. Integration of cognitive allocentric information in visuospatial short-term memory through the hippocampus. Hippocampus 2006; 15:1072-84. [PMID: 16161036 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.20126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Visuospatial short-term memory relies on a widely distributed neocortical network: some areas support the encoding process of the visually acquired spatial information, whereas other ares are more involved in the active maintenance of the encoded information. Recently, in a pointing to remembered targets task, it has been shown in healthy subjects that, for memory delays of 5 s, spatial errors are affected also by cognitive allocentric information, i.e., covert spatial information derived from a pure mental representation. We tested the effect of a lesion of the hippocampus on the accuracy of pointing movements toward remembered targets, with memory delays falling in the 0.5-30 s range. The spatial distributions of the two target sets we used (line and left-right) allowed the exploitation of cognitive allocentric spatial information: both sets were in the frontal plane, the line one being composed by eleven points distributed uniformly along a virtual line tilted 45 degrees away from the vertical, whereas the left-right set was composed by two workspaces symmetrically distributed at the extremes of a horizontal virtual line. We have found a significant difference between the performance of three hippocampal amnesic subjects and a group of normal controls for delays equal to or longer than 15 s, the difference being along the allocentric axis, i.e., the direction of the virtual line defined by the target set. On this basis we suggest that the hippocampal formation may enhance the spatial information processed within short-term memory with cognitive allocentric information. The association that may be operated through the neocortical-hippocampal loop of the newly acquired spatial information with well established spatial cognitive items could affect the precision of the short-term memory storage for memory delays exceeding about 15 s and might be the result of a modulation of the span of the spatial memory buffer along context-specific directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Carrozzo
- C.N.R., Institute of Neuroscience, Laboratory of Psychobiology and Psychopharmacology, Rome, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Ansorge U, Neumann O. Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cuing task. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2006; 31:762-77. [PMID: 16131248 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.762] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In 5 experiments, the authors tested whether the processing of nonconscious spatial stimulus information depends on a prior intention. This test was conducted with the metacontrast dissociation paradigm. Experiment 1 demonstrated that masked primes that could not be discriminated above chance level affected responses to the visible stimuli that masked them. Experiments 2 and 3 showed that this effect was abolished when the task instruction was changed in such a way that the primes ceased to be task relevant. Experiments 4 and 5 demonstrated that a prime's effect depended on whether it was associated with the same response as the target or with an opposite response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ulrich Ansorge
- Department of PsychologyBielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
91
|
Ansorge U. Die Rolle von Absichten bei der automatischen Verarbeitung visuell-räumlicher Reizinformation. PSYCHOLOGISCHE RUNDSCHAU 2006. [DOI: 10.1026/0033-3042.57.1.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Wird visuell-räumliche Reizinformation automatisch verarbeitet? Psychologische Experimente scheinen das zu belegen. Reize, die plötzlich am Rand des Blickfelds erscheinen, ziehen die Aufmerksamkeit unwillkürlich auf den Ort ihres Erscheinens, selbst dann, wenn wir einen anderen Ort beachten wollen. Und Reizpositionen aktivieren Reaktionen, obwohl die Positionen “irrelevant“ sind. In einer Reihe von Arbeiten konnte allerdings nachgewiesen werden, dass diese Effekte in erheblichem Maß auf Absichten beruhen: Plötzlich erscheinende Reize ziehen die Aufmerksamkeit vor allem bei hoher Passung zwischen Suchabsichten und irrelevanten Reizen (oder Reizmerkmalen) an, und Reizpositionen aktivieren Reaktionen, wenn die Probanden die entsprechende räumliche Information benutzen, um zwischen Reaktionsalternativen zu unterscheiden. Was als automatische Verarbeitung erscheint, könnte daher auf einem allgemeineren Verarbeitungsprinzip beruhen: einer Passung der Merkmale vermeintlich irrelevanter Reize zu den willentlich ausgewählten Kriterien für die Verarbeitung relevanter Merkmale.
Collapse
|
92
|
Abstract
Do relative binocular disparities guide our movements in depth? In order to find out we asked subjects to move a 'cursor' to a target within a simulated horizontal plane at eye height. They did so by moving a computer mouse. We determined how quickly subjects responded to the target jumping in depth. We found that it took subjects about 200 ms to respond to changes in binocular disparity. Subjects responded just as quickly if the cursor was temporarily only visible to one eye near the time that the target jumped in depth, and less vigorously, though just as quickly, if the cursor jumped rather than the target, so the fastest binocular responses cannot be based directly on the relative retinal disparity between the target and the cursor. Subjects reacted faster to changes in the target's height in the visual field than to changes in binocular disparity, but did not react faster to changes in image size. These results suggest that binocular vision mainly improves people's everyday movements by giving them a better sense of the distances of relevant objects, rather than by relative retinal disparities being used to directly guide the movement. We propose that relative disparities only guide parts of very slow movements that require extreme precision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eli Brenner
- Department of Neuroscience, ErasmusMC, P. O. Box 1738, 3000 DR, Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
93
|
van Doorn H, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP. Catching and matching bars with different orientations. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2005; 120:288-306. [PMID: 16036210 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2005] [Revised: 05/10/2005] [Accepted: 05/22/2005] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The hypothesis that perception enslaves action is examined by assessing whether systematic distortions in perceptual judgments are reflected by inaccuracies in catching. In the first experiment, participants had to align manually the orientation of a reference bar placed at different distances in the frontoparallel plane. In the second experiment participants had to catch differently orientated moving bars, which became invisible at different distances from the interception point. In the matching experiment, systematic errors in the alignment of orientation were found in particular for oblique orientations, the magnitude of which increased with increasing distance of the reference bar. The inaccuracies in the final hand orientation during the catching task, however, did not mirror this pattern of deviations. The findings are interpreted to be more consistent with recent views that vision for perception (i.e., matching) and vision for action (i.e., catching) are dissociated than with the view that perception enslaves action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hemke van Doorn
- Institute for Fundamental and Clinical Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
94
|
Mosier KM, Scheidt RA, Acosta S, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. Remapping hand movements in a novel geometrical environment. J Neurophysiol 2005; 94:4362-72. [PMID: 16148276 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00380.2005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 86] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The issue of how the Euclidean properties of space are represented in the nervous system is a main focus in the study of visual perception, but is equally relevant to motor learning. The goal of our experiments was to investigate how the properties of space guide the remapping of motor coordination. Subjects wore an instrumented data glove that recorded the finger motions. Signals generated by the glove operated a remotely controlled endpoint: a cursor on a computer monitor. The subjects were instructed to execute movements of this endpoint with controlled motions of the fingers. This required inverting a highly redundant map from fingers to cursor motions. We found that 1) after training with visual feedback of the final error (but not of the ongoing cursor motion), subjects learned to map cursor locations into configurations of the fingers; 2) extended practice of movement led to more rectilinear cursor movement, a trend facilitated by training under continuous visual feedback of cursor motions; 3) with practice, subjects reduced motion in the degrees of freedom that did not contribute to the movements of the cursor; 4) with practice, subjects reduced variability of both cursor and hand movements; and 5) the reduction of errors and the increase in linearity generalized beyond the set of movements used for training. These findings suggest that subjects not only learned to produce novel coordinated movement to control the placement of the cursor, but they also developed a representation of the Euclidean space on which hand movements were remapped.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristine M Mosier
- Department of Radiology, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
95
|
|
96
|
Obhi SS, Goodale MA. The effects of landmarks on the performance of delayed and real-time pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 2005; 167:335-44. [PMID: 16041512 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-005-0055-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2004] [Accepted: 03/28/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Converging lines of evidence suggest that the presence of non-target landmarks affects the performance of delayed target-directed movements (e.g., Diedrichsen et al. 2004; Sheth and Shimojo 2004). In the present experiment, we examined the effects of non-target landmarks on the accuracy and precision of delayed and immediate target-directed pointing movements. In our experiment, the landmarks were present just prior to and during the presentation of the target; they were never present during the execution of the movement. Absolute errors were significantly reduced when the landmarks were available during target presentation for both delayed and immediate action conditions. In contrast, the presence of landmarks improved the precision of delayed but not immediate movements (as indexed by the variable error). The locus of this "landmark benefit" appears to be in the encoding of target position since landmarks were never available after target offset. We suggest that, when available, information provided by landmarks is used to improve the accuracy of the estimation of target location. Since the targets were presented for only 100 ms, it is apparent that the spatial information available from landmarks can be quite rapidly used to estimate target position. Further, with respect to the precision of movements, we suggest that the presence of landmarks serves to improve the stability of the estimation of target position. This particular reliance on landmark information becomes more critical as the movement is delayed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sukhvinder S Obhi
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, N2L 3C5, Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
97
|
Voisin J, Michaud G, Chapman CE. Haptic shape discrimination in humans: insight into haptic frames of reference. Exp Brain Res 2005; 164:347-56. [PMID: 15959722 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-005-2256-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2004] [Accepted: 12/06/2004] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
This study investigates how a change in the physical relation between objects (two-dimensional, 2-D, angles) and a subject, as well as scanning conditions, modify the ability to discriminate small changes in 2-D shape. Subjects scanned pairs of angles (90 masculine standard; 91 masculine-103 masculine comparison angles) with the right index finger of the out-stretched arm, identifying the larger of each pair. When joint rotation was restricted to the shoulder, the discrimination threshold significantly increased when the angles were explored with the shoulder in a more eccentric position rather than closer to the midline (60 masculine versus 30 masculine to the right). This result was attributed to changes in proprioceptive sensitivity, since explorations restricted to distal joints (wrist/second metacarpophalangeal joint) showed no change with shoulder position. The results showed, moreover, that discrimination threshold was similar for distal and proximal joints when the delay between scanning the pairs of angles was long (15 s). This observation suggests that regional variations in proprioceptive acuity (proximal>distal) may reflect an adaptation to generate an invariant central representation of haptic shape. Using a shorter interscan delay (5 s), a position-dependent increase in discrimination threshold was revealed for distal explorations, an effect that disappeared when the head was turned in the direction of the unseen angle (vision occluded). We suggest that these results can be explained by the existence of two competing egocentric frames of reference with different time courses, one of short duration that is centred on the arm/hand, and a second of longer duration centred on the head. At the short delay, the reference frames interacted to distort the haptic representation when they were misaligned. This distortion was resolved at the long delay, possibly through suppression of the arm/hand-centred reference frame.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julien Voisin
- Centre de Recherche en Sciences Neurologiques, Département de Physiologie, Université de Montréal, 6128, Succursale Centre Ville, Montréal, QC, Canada, H3C 3J7
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
98
|
Brenner E, Meijer WJ, Cornelissen FW. Judging relative positions across saccades. Vision Res 2005; 45:1587-602. [PMID: 15781075 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.11.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2004] [Revised: 11/17/2004] [Accepted: 11/24/2004] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
When components of a shape are presented asynchronously during smooth pursuit, the retinal image determines the perceived shape, as if the parts belong to the moving object that the eyes are pursuing. Saccades normally shift our gaze between structures of interest, so there is no reason to expect anything to have moved with the eyes. We therefore decided to examine how people judge the separation between a target flashed before and another flashed after a saccade. Subjects tracked a jumping dot with their eyes. Targets were flashed at predetermined retinal positions, with a 67-242 ms interval between the flashes. After each trial subjects indicated where they had seen the targets. We selected the trials on which subjects made a complete saccade between the presentations of the two targets. For short inter-target intervals, subjects' judgements depended almost exclusively on the retinal separation, even when there were conspicuous visual references nearby. Even for the longest intervals, only part of the change in eye orientation was taken into consideration. These findings cannot simply be accounted for on the basis of the mislocalisation of individual targets or a compression of space near saccades. We conclude that the retinal separation determines the perceived separation between targets presented with a short interval between them, irrespective of any intervening eye movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Brenner
- Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus Medical Centre, P.O. Box 1738, Dr. Molenwaterplein 50, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Israël I, Lecoq C, Capelli A, Golomer E. Vestibular memory-contingent whole-body return: brave exocentered dancers. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2005; 1039:306-13. [PMID: 15826984 DOI: 10.1196/annals.1325.029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
A whole-body, self-driven return from passive rotation (90 degrees to 270 degrees ) test was executed by male and female subjects, and by professional female ballet dancers. To accomplish the return task, subjects were free to use the egocentered reference frame (inversion) or the exocentered one (completion). The results show that with inversion all kinetic parameters were reproduced, whereas the completion performance was highly variable. Although inversion was the default strategy, female subjects used more completions than male subjects, and female dancers still more, although not more accurately. The high variability noted in completion shows a deficiency in integrating vestibular signals for updating the egocentric representation of an external target during passive body rotations, even in dancers. Furthermore, with completions after 180 degrees stimuli, the results suggest that both ego- and exocentered reference frames cannot be used simultaneously.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I Israël
- LPBD-EPHE, 41 rue Gay-Lussac, 75005 Paris, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
100
|
Keen RE, Berthier NE. Continuities and discontinuities in infants' representation of objects and events. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2005; 32:243-79. [PMID: 15641465 DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2407(04)80009-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel E Keen
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|