51
|
Schwartz PH, Perkins SM, Schmidt KK, Muriello PF, Althouse S, Rawl SM. Providing Quantitative Information and a Nudge to Undergo Stool Testing in a Colorectal Cancer Screening Decision Aid: A Randomized Clinical Trial. Med Decis Making 2017; 37:688-702. [DOI: 10.1177/0272989x17698678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Peter H. Schwartz
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, KKS, PFM)
- Indiana University Center for Bioethics, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, PFM)
- Philosophy Department, Indiana University School of Liberal Arts, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS)
- Indiana University Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, SMP, SMR)
- Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (SMP, SA)
| | - Susan M. Perkins
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, KKS, PFM)
- Indiana University Center for Bioethics, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, PFM)
- Philosophy Department, Indiana University School of Liberal Arts, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS)
- Indiana University Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, SMP, SMR)
- Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (SMP, SA)
| | - Karen K. Schmidt
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, KKS, PFM)
- Indiana University Center for Bioethics, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, PFM)
- Philosophy Department, Indiana University School of Liberal Arts, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS)
- Indiana University Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, SMP, SMR)
- Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (SMP, SA)
| | - Paul F. Muriello
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, KKS, PFM)
- Indiana University Center for Bioethics, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, PFM)
- Philosophy Department, Indiana University School of Liberal Arts, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS)
- Indiana University Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, SMP, SMR)
- Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (SMP, SA)
| | - Sandra Althouse
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, KKS, PFM)
- Indiana University Center for Bioethics, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, PFM)
- Philosophy Department, Indiana University School of Liberal Arts, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS)
- Indiana University Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, SMP, SMR)
- Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (SMP, SA)
| | - Susan M. Rawl
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, KKS, PFM)
- Indiana University Center for Bioethics, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, PFM)
- Philosophy Department, Indiana University School of Liberal Arts, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS)
- Indiana University Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, IN, USA (PHS, SMP, SMR)
- Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA (SMP, SA)
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Chavarria V, Vian J, Pereira C, Data-Franco J, Fernandes BS, Berk M, Dodd S. The Placebo and Nocebo Phenomena: Their Clinical Management and Impact on Treatment Outcomes. Clin Ther 2017; 39:477-486. [PMID: 28237673 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2017.01.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2016] [Revised: 01/25/2017] [Accepted: 01/30/2017] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE This overview focuses on placebo and nocebo effects in clinical trials and routine care. Our goal was to propose strategies to improve outcomes in clinical practice, maximizing placebo effects and reducing nocebo effects, as well as managing these phenomena in clinical trials. METHODS A narrative literature search of PubMed was conducted (January 1980-September 2016). Systematic reviews, randomized controlled trials, observational studies, and case series that had an emphasis on placebo or nocebo effects in clinical practice were included in the qualitative synthesis. Search terms included: placebo, nocebo, clinical, clinical trial, clinical setting, placebo effect, nocebo effect, adverse effects, and treatment outcomes. This search was augmented by a manual search of the references of the key articles and the related literature. FINDINGS Placebo and nocebo effects are psychobiological events imputable to the therapeutic context. Placebo is defined as an inert substance that provokes perceived benefits, whereas the term nocebo is used when an inert substance causes perceived harm. Their major mechanisms are expectancy and classical conditioning. Placebo is used in several fields of medicine, as a diagnostic tool or to reduce drug dosage. Placebo/nocebo effects are difficult to disentangle from the natural course of illness or the actual effects of a new drug in a clinical trial. There are known strategies to enhance clinical results by manipulating expectations and conditioning. IMPLICATIONS Placebo and nocebo effects occur frequently and are clinically significant but are underrecognized in clinical practice. Physicians should be able to recognize these phenomena and master tactics on how to manage these effects to enhance the quality of clinical practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victor Chavarria
- Institut de Neuropsiquiatria i Adiccions (INAD), Parc de salut Mar (PSM), Barcelona, Spain
| | - João Vian
- Psychiatry and Mental Health Department, Centro Hospitalar Lisboa Norte, Lisbon, Portugal; Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Círia Pereira
- Psychiatry and Mental Health Department, Centro Hospitalar Lisboa Norte, Lisbon, Portugal; Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - João Data-Franco
- Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal; Departamento de Psiquiatria e Saúde Mental, Hospital Beatriz Ângelo, Lisboa, Portugal
| | - Brisa S Fernandes
- IMPACT Strategic Research Centre, School of Medicine, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia; Laboratory of Calcium Binding Proteins in the Central Nervous System, Department of Biochemistry, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
| | - Michael Berk
- IMPACT Strategic Research Centre, School of Medicine, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia; University Hospital Geelong, Barwon Health, Geelong, VIC Australia; Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia; Centre for Youth Mental Health, Parkville, VIC, Australia; Florey Institute, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Seetal Dodd
- IMPACT Strategic Research Centre, School of Medicine, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia; University Hospital Geelong, Barwon Health, Geelong, VIC Australia; Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia; Centre for Youth Mental Health, Parkville, VIC, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Manuel DG, Abdulaziz KE, Perez R, Beach S, Bennett C. Personalized risk communication for personalized risk assessment: Real world assessment of knowledge and motivation for six mortality risk measures from an online life expectancy calculator. Inform Health Soc Care 2017; 43:42-55. [PMID: 28068156 DOI: 10.1080/17538157.2016.1255632] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
In the clinical setting, previous studies have shown personalized risk assessment and communication improves risk perception and motivation. We evaluated an online health calculator that estimated and presented six different measures of life expectancy/mortality based on a person's sociodemographic and health behavior profile. Immediately after receiving calculator results, participants were invited to complete an online survey that asked how informative and motivating they found each risk measure, whether they would share their results and whether the calculator provided information they need to make lifestyle changes. Over 80% of the 317 survey respondents found at least one of six healthy living measures highly informative and motivating, but there was moderate heterogeneity regarding which measures respondents found most informative and motivating. Overall, health age was most informative and life expectancy most motivating. Approximately 40% of respondents would share the results with their clinician (44%) or social networks (38%), although the information they would share was often different from what they found informative or motivational. Online personalized risk assessment allows for a more personalized communication compared to historic paper-based risk assessment to maximize knowledge and motivation, and people should be provided a range of risk communication measures that reflect different risk perspectives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Douglas G Manuel
- a Clinical Epidemiology Program, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute , Ottawa , ON , Canada.,b Departments of Family Medicine , University of Ottawa , Ottawa , ON , Canada.,c Bruyère Research Institute , Ottawa , ON , Canada.,d School of Public and Population Health, University of Ottawa , Ottawa , ON , Canada.,e Institute for Clinical Evaluative Sciences , Ottawa and Toronto , ON , Canada
| | - Kasim E Abdulaziz
- a Clinical Epidemiology Program, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute , Ottawa , ON , Canada
| | - Richard Perez
- a Clinical Epidemiology Program, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute , Ottawa , ON , Canada.,e Institute for Clinical Evaluative Sciences , Ottawa and Toronto , ON , Canada
| | - Sarah Beach
- a Clinical Epidemiology Program, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute , Ottawa , ON , Canada
| | - Carol Bennett
- a Clinical Epidemiology Program, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute , Ottawa , ON , Canada.,e Institute for Clinical Evaluative Sciences , Ottawa and Toronto , ON , Canada
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Simon ST, Higginson IJ, Booth S, Harding R, Weingärtner V, Bausewein C. Benzodiazepines for the relief of breathlessness in advanced malignant and non-malignant diseases in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2016; 10:CD007354. [PMID: 27764523 PMCID: PMC6464146 DOI: 10.1002/14651858.cd007354.pub3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND This is an updated version of the original Cochrane review published in Issue 1, 2010, on 'Benzodiazepines for the relief of breathlessness in advanced malignant and non-malignant diseases in adults'. Breathlessness is one of the most common symptoms experienced in the advanced stages of malignant and non-malignant disease. Benzodiazepines are widely used for the relief of breathlessness in advanced diseases and are regularly recommended in the literature. At the time of the previously published Cochrane review, there was no evidence for a beneficial effect of benzodiazepines for the relief of breathlessness in people with advanced cancer and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD). OBJECTIVES The primary objective of this review was to determine the efficacy of benzodiazepines for the relief of breathlessness in people with advanced disease. Secondary objectives were to determine the efficacy of different benzodiazepines, different doses of benzodiazepines, different routes of application, adverse effects of benzodiazepines, and the efficacy in different disease groups. SEARCH METHODS This is an update of a review published in 2010. We searched 14 electronic databases up to September 2009 for the original review. We checked the reference lists of all relevant studies, key textbooks, reviews, and websites. For the update, we searched CENTRAL, MEDLINE, and EMBASE and registers of clinical trials for further ongoing or unpublished studies, up to August 2016. We contacted study investigators and experts in the field of palliative care asking for further studies, unpublished data, or study details when necessary. SELECTION CRITERIA We included randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and controlled clinical trials (CCTs) assessing the effect of benzodiazepines compared with placebo or active control in relieving breathlessness in people with advanced stages of cancer, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), chronic heart failure (CHF), motor neurone disease (MND), and idiopathic pulmonary fibrosis (IPF). DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS Two review authors independently assessed identified titles and abstracts. Three review authors independently performed assessment of all potentially relevant studies (full text), data extraction, and assessment of methodological quality. We carried out meta-analysis where appropriate. MAIN RESULTS Overall, we identified eight studies for inclusion: seven in the previous review and an additional study for this update. We also identified two studies awaiting classification in this update. The studies were small (a maximum number of 101 participants) and comprised data from a total of 214 participants with advanced cancer or COPD, which we analysed. There was only one study of low risk of bias. Most of the studies had an unclear risk of bias due to lack of information on random sequence generation, concealment, and attrition. Analysis of all studies did not show a beneficial effect of benzodiazepines for the relief of breathlessness (the primary outcome) in people with advanced cancer and COPD (8 studies, 214 participants) compared to placebo, midazolam, morphine, or promethazine. Furthermore, we observed no statistically significant effect in the prevention of episodic breathlessness (breakthrough dyspnoea) in people with cancer (after 48 hours: risk ratio of 0.76 (95% CI 0.53 to 1.09; 2 studies, 108 participants)) compared to morphine. Sensitivity analyses demonstrated no statistically significant differences regarding type of benzodiazepine, dose, route and frequency of delivery, duration of treatment, or type of control. Benzodiazepines caused statistically significantly more adverse events, particularly drowsiness and somnolence, when compared to placebo (risk difference 0.74 (95% CI 0.37, 1.11); 3 studies, 38 participants). In contrast, two studies reported that morphine caused more adverse events than midazolam (RD -0.18 (95% CI -0.31, -0.04); 194 participants). AUTHORS' CONCLUSIONS Since the last version of this review, we have identified one new study for inclusion, but the conclusions remain unchanged. There is no evidence for or against benzodiazepines for the relief of breathlessness in people with advanced cancer and COPD. Benzodiazepines caused more drowsiness as an adverse effect compared to placebo, but less compared to morphine. Benzodiazepines may be considered as a second- or third-line treatment, when opioids and non-pharmacological measures have failed to control breathlessness. There is a need for well-conducted and adequately powered studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steffen T Simon
- Institute of Palliative Care (ipac)Jägerstr. 64‐66OldenburgGermany26121
- University Hospital of CologneDepartment of Palliative MedicineCologneGermany
- King's College LondonDepartment of Palliative Care, Policy and Rehabilitation, Cicely Saunders InstituteLondonUK
| | - Irene J Higginson
- King's College LondonDepartment of Palliative Care, Policy and Rehabilitation, Cicely Saunders InstituteLondonUK
| | - Sara Booth
- Cambridge University HospitalsDepartment of Palliative CareCambridgeUK
| | - Richard Harding
- King's College LondonDepartment of Palliative Care, Policy and Rehabilitation, Cicely Saunders InstituteLondonUK
| | - Vera Weingärtner
- University Hospital of CologneDepartment of Palliative MedicineCologneGermany
| | - Claudia Bausewein
- LMU MunichDepartment of Palliative Medicine, Munich University HospitalMarchioninistr. 15MunichGermany81377
| | | |
Collapse
|
55
|
Brown B, Balatsoukas P, Williams R, Sperrin M, Buchan I. Interface design recommendations for computerised clinical audit and feedback: Hybrid usability evidence from a research-led system. Int J Med Inform 2016; 94:191-206. [PMID: 27573327 PMCID: PMC5015594 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2016.07.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2016] [Revised: 07/09/2016] [Accepted: 07/14/2016] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Audit and Feedback (A&F) is a widely used quality improvement technique that measures clinicians' clinical performance and reports it back to them. Computerised A&F (e-A&F) system interfaces may consist of four key components: (1) Summaries of clinical performance; (2) Patient lists; (3) Patient-level data; (4) Recommended actions. There is a lack of evidence regarding how to best design e-A&F interfaces; establishing such evidence is key to maximising usability, and in turn improving patient safety. AIM To evaluate the usability of a novel theoretically-informed and research-led e-A&F system for primary care (the Performance Improvement plaN GeneratoR: PINGR). OBJECTIVES (1) Describe PINGR's design, rationale and theoretical basis; (2) Identify usability issues with PINGR; (3) Understand how these issues may interfere with the cognitive goals of end-users; (4) Translate the issues into recommendations for the user-centred design of e-A&F systems. METHODS Eight experienced health system evaluators performed a usability inspection using an innovative hybrid approach consisting of five stages: (1) Development of representative user tasks, Goals, and Actions; (2) Combining Heuristic Evaluation and Cognitive Walkthrough methods into a single protocol to identify usability issues; (3) Consolidation of issues; (4) Severity rating of consolidated issues; (5) Analysis of issues according to usability heuristics, interface components, and Goal-Action structure. RESULTS A final list of 47 issues were categorised into 8 heuristic themes. The most error-prone heuristics were 'Consistency and standards' (13 usability issues; 28% of the total) and 'Match between system and real world' (n=10, 21%). The recommended actions component of the PINGR interface had the most usability issues (n=21, 45%), followed by patient-level data (n=5, 11%), patient lists (n=4, 9%), and summaries of clinical performance (n=4, 9%). The most error-prone Actions across all user Goals were: (1) Patient selection from a list; (2) Data identification from a figure (both population-level and patient-level); (3) Disagreement with a system recommendation. CONCLUSIONS By contextualising our findings within the wider literature on health information system usability, we provide recommendations for the design of e-A&F system interfaces relating to their four key components, in addition to how they may be integrated within a system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Brown
- Health eResearch Centre, Farr Institute of Health Informatics Research, Centre for Health Informatics, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.
| | - Panos Balatsoukas
- Health eResearch Centre, Farr Institute of Health Informatics Research, Centre for Health Informatics, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Richard Williams
- Health eResearch Centre, Farr Institute of Health Informatics Research, Centre for Health Informatics, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Matthew Sperrin
- Health eResearch Centre, Farr Institute of Health Informatics Research, Centre for Health Informatics, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| | - Iain Buchan
- Health eResearch Centre, Farr Institute of Health Informatics Research, Centre for Health Informatics, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Gurmankin AD, Helweg-Larsen M, Armstrong K, Kimmel SE, Volpp KGM. Comparing the Standard Rating Scale and the Magnifier Scale for Assessing Risk Perceptions. Med Decis Making 2016; 25:560-70. [PMID: 16160211 DOI: 10.1177/0272989x05280560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Objective. A new risk perception rating scale (“magnifier scale”) was recently developed to reduce elevated perceptions of low-probability health events, but little is known about its performance. The authors tested whether the magnifier scale lowers risk perceptions for low-probability (in 0%–1% magnifying glass section of scale) but not high-probability (>1%) events compared to a standard rating scale (SRS). Method. In studies 1 (n = 463) and 2 (n = 105), undergraduates completed a survey assessing risk perceptions of high- and low-probability events in a randomized 2X 2 design: in study 1 using the magnifier scale or SRS, numeric risk information provided or not, and in study 2 using the magnifier scale or SRS, high- or low-probability event. In study 3, hypertension patients at the Philadelphia Veterans Affairs hospital completed a similar survey (n = 222) assessing risk perceptions of 2 self-relevant high-probability events—heart attack and stroke—with the magnifier scale or the SRS. Results. In study 1, when no risk information was provided, risk perceptions for both high- and low-probability events were significantly lower (P < 0.0001) when using the magnifier scale compared to the SRS, but risk perceptions were no different by scale when risk information was provided (interaction term: P = 0.003). In studies 2 and 3, risk perceptions for the high-probability events were significantly lower using the magnifier scale than the SRS (P = 0.015 and P = 0.014, respectively). Conclusions. The magnifier scale lowered risk perceptions but did so for low- and high-probability events, suggesting that the magnifier scale should not be used for assessments of risk perceptions for high-probability events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea D Gurmankin
- Department of Society, Human Development and Health, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
57
|
What do patients really want to know? The effects of age and of different approaches to presenting risk on patient preferences for information on medication side effects. Eur Geriatr Med 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.eurger.2016.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
58
|
Underhill K, Morrow KM, Colleran C, Calabrese SK, Operario D, Salovey P, Mayer KH. Explaining the Efficacy of Pre-exposure Prophylaxis (PrEP) for HIV Prevention: A Qualitative Study of Message Framing and Messaging Preferences Among US Men Who have Sex with Men. AIDS Behav 2016; 20:1514-26. [PMID: 25963772 PMCID: PMC4643421 DOI: 10.1007/s10461-015-1088-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
We investigated message comprehension and message framing preferences for communicating about PrEP efficacy with US MSM. We conducted eight focus groups (n = 38) and n = 56 individual interviews with MSM in Providence, RI. Facilitators probed comprehension, credibility, and acceptability of efficacy messages, including percentages, non-numerical paraphrases, efficacy ranges versus point estimates, and success- versus failure-framed messages. Our findings indicated a range of comprehension and operational understandings of efficacy messages. Participants tended to prefer percentage-based and success-framed messages, although preferences varied for communicating about efficacy using a single percentage versus a range. Participants reported uncertainty about how to interpret numerical estimates, and many questioned whether trial results would predict personal effectiveness. These results suggest that providers and researchers implementing PrEP may face challenges in communicating with users about efficacy. Efforts to educate MSM about PrEP should incorporate percentage-based information, and message framing decisions may influence message credibility and overall PrEP acceptability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristen Underhill
- Yale Center for Interdisciplinary Research on AIDS, Yale University, 135 College St, Suite 200, New Haven, CT, 06510, USA.
- Yale Law School, Yale University, P.O. Box 208215, New Haven, CT, 06520, USA.
| | - Kathleen M Morrow
- Centers for Behavioral and Preventive Medicine, The Miriam Hospital, Providence, RI, USA
| | - Christopher Colleran
- Centers for Behavioral and Preventive Medicine, The Miriam Hospital, Providence, RI, USA
| | - Sarah K Calabrese
- Yale Center for Interdisciplinary Research on AIDS, Yale University, 135 College St, Suite 200, New Haven, CT, 06510, USA
- Yale School of Public Health, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Don Operario
- Program in Public Health, Brown University, Providence, RI, USA
| | - Peter Salovey
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
59
|
Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Milcke E, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics. Psychol Sci Public Interest 2016; 8:53-96. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 718] [Impact Index Per Article: 79.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
Abstract
Many doctors, patients, journalists, and politicians alike do not understand what health statistics mean or draw wrong conclusions without noticing. Collective statistical illiteracy refers to the widespread inability to understand the meaning of numbers. For instance, many citizens are unaware that higher survival rates with cancer screening do not imply longer life, or that the statement that mammography screening reduces the risk of dying from breast cancer by 25% in fact means that 1 less woman out of 1,000 will die of the disease. We provide evidence that statistical illiteracy (a) is common to patients, journalists, and physicians; (b) is created by nontransparent framing of information that is sometimes an unintentional result of lack of understanding but can also be a result of intentional efforts to manipulate or persuade people; and (c) can have serious consequences for health. The causes of statistical illiteracy should not be attributed to cognitive biases alone, but to the emotional nature of the doctor–patient relationship and conflicts of interest in the healthcare system. The classic doctor–patient relation is based on (the physician's) paternalism and (the patient's) trust in authority, which make statistical literacy seem unnecessary; so does the traditional combination of determinism (physicians who seek causes, not chances) and the illusion of certainty (patients who seek certainty when there is none). We show that information pamphlets, Web sites, leaflets distributed to doctors by the pharmaceutical industry, and even medical journals often report evidence in nontransparent forms that suggest big benefits of featured interventions and small harms. Without understanding the numbers involved, the public is susceptible to political and commercial manipulation of their anxieties and hopes, which undermines the goals of informed consent and shared decision making. What can be done? We discuss the importance of teaching statistical thinking and transparent representations in primary and secondary education as well as in medical school. Yet this requires familiarizing children early on with the concept of probability and teaching statistical literacy as the art of solving real-world problems rather than applying formulas to toy problems about coins and dice. A major precondition for statistical literacy is transparent risk communication. We recommend using frequency statements instead of single-event probabilities, absolute risks instead of relative risks, mortality rates instead of survival rates, and natural frequencies instead of conditional probabilities. Psychological research on transparent visual and numerical forms of risk communication, as well as training of physicians in their use, is called for. Statistical literacy is a necessary precondition for an educated citizenship in a technological democracy. Understanding risks and asking critical questions can also shape the emotional climate in a society so that hopes and anxieties are no longer as easily manipulated from outside and citizens can develop a better-informed and more relaxed attitude toward their health.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gerd Gigerenzer
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin
- Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Berlin
| | - Wolfgang Gaissmaier
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin
- Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Berlin
| | - Elke Kurz-Milcke
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin
- Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Berlin
| | - Lisa M. Schwartz
- The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Practice's Center for Medicine and the Media, Dartmouth Medical School
| | - Steven Woloshin
- The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Practice's Center for Medicine and the Media, Dartmouth Medical School
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Spruit SL, van de Poel I, Doorn N. Informed Consent in Asymmetrical Relationships: an Investigation into Relational Factors that Influence Room for Reflection. NANOETHICS 2016; 10:123-138. [PMID: 27478516 PMCID: PMC4949294 DOI: 10.1007/s11569-016-0262-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2015] [Accepted: 04/21/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
In recent years, informed consent has been suggested as a way to deal with risks posed by engineered nanomaterials. We argue that while we can learn from experiences with informed consent in treatment and research contexts, we should be aware that informed consent traditionally pertains to certain features of the relationships between doctors and patients and researchers and research participants, rather than those between producers and consumers and employers and employees, which are more prominent in the case of engineered nanomaterials. To better understand these differences, we identify three major relational factors that influence whether valid informed consent is obtainable, namely dependency, personal proximity, and existence of shared interests. We show that each type of relationship offers different opportunities for reflection and therefore poses distinct challenges for obtaining valid informed consent. Our analysis offers a systematic understanding of the possibilities for attaining informed consent in the context of nanomaterial risks and makes clear that measures or regulations to improve the obtainment of informed consent should be attuned to the specific interpersonal relations to which it is supposed to apply.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shannon Lydia Spruit
- Department of Values, Technology and Innovation, Delft University of Technology, Delft, The Netherlands
| | - Ibo van de Poel
- Department of Values, Technology and Innovation, Delft University of Technology, Delft, The Netherlands
| | - Neelke Doorn
- Department of Values, Technology and Innovation, Delft University of Technology, Delft, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
Veldwijk J, Essers BAB, Lambooij MS, Dirksen CD, Smit HA, de Wit GA. Survival or Mortality: Does Risk Attribute Framing Influence Decision-Making Behavior in a Discrete Choice Experiment? VALUE IN HEALTH : THE JOURNAL OF THE INTERNATIONAL SOCIETY FOR PHARMACOECONOMICS AND OUTCOMES RESEARCH 2016; 19:202-9. [PMID: 27021754 DOI: 10.1016/j.jval.2015.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2014] [Revised: 08/31/2015] [Accepted: 11/04/2015] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To test how attribute framing in a discrete choice experiment (DCE) affects respondents' decision-making behavior and their preferences. METHODS Two versions of a DCE questionnaire containing nine choice tasks were distributed among a representative sample of the Dutch population aged 55 to 65 years. The DCE consisted of four attributes related to the decision regarding participation in genetic screening for colorectal cancer (CRC). The risk attribute included was framed positively as the probability of surviving CRC and negatively as the probability of dying from CRC. Panel mixed-logit models were used to estimate the relative importance of the attributes. The data of the positively and negatively framed DCE were compared on the basis of direct attribute ranking, dominant decision-making behavior, preferences, and importance scores. RESULTS The majority (56%) of the respondents ranked survival as the most important attribute in the positively framed DCE, whereas only a minority (8%) of the respondents ranked mortality as the most important attribute in the negatively framed DCE. Respondents made dominant choices based on survival significantly more often than based on mortality. The framing of the risk attribute significantly influenced all attribute-level estimates and resulted in different preference structures among respondents in the positively and negatively framed data set. CONCLUSIONS Risk framing affects how respondents value the presented risk. Positive risk framing led to increased dominant decision-making behavior, whereas negative risk framing led to risk-seeking behavior. Attribute framing should have a prominent part in the expert and focus group interviews, and different types of framing should be used in the pilot version of DCEs as well as in actual DCEs to estimate the magnitude of the effect of choosing different types of framing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jorien Veldwijk
- Centre for Prevention and Health Services Research, National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, Bilthoven, The Netherlands; Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Brigitte A B Essers
- Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Medical Technology Assessment, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands; CAPHRI School of Public Health and Primary Care, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Mattijs S Lambooij
- Centre for Prevention and Health Services Research, National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, Bilthoven, The Netherlands
| | - Carmen D Dirksen
- Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Medical Technology Assessment, Maastricht University Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlands; CAPHRI School of Public Health and Primary Care, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Henriette A Smit
- Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - G Ardine de Wit
- Centre for Prevention and Health Services Research, National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, Bilthoven, The Netherlands; Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Maule AJ, Maule S. Aiding Lay Decision Making Using a Cognitive Competencies Approach. Front Psychol 2016; 6:1884. [PMID: 26779052 PMCID: PMC4703786 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01884] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2015] [Accepted: 11/20/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Two prescriptive approaches have evolved to aid human decision making: just in time interventions that provide support as a decision is being made; and just in case interventions that educate people about future events that they may encounter so that they are better prepared to make an informed decision when these events occur. We review research on these two approaches developed in the context of supporting everyday decisions such as choosing an apartment, a financial product or a medical procedure. We argue that the lack of an underlying prescriptive theory has limited the development and evaluation of these interventions. We draw on recent descriptive research on the cognitive competencies that underpin human decision making to suggest new ways of interpreting how and why existing decision aids may be effective and suggest a different way of evaluating their effectiveness. We also briefly outline how our approach has the potential to develop new interventions to support everyday decision making and highlight the benefits of drawing on descriptive research when developing and evaluating interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A. J. Maule
- Leeds University Business School, University of LeedsLeeds, UK
| | - Simon Maule
- Leeds University Business School, University of LeedsLeeds, UK
- Linstock CommunicationsLondon, UK
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Bui TC, Krieger HA, Blumenthal-Barby JS. Framing Effects on Physicians' Judgment and Decision Making. Psychol Rep 2015; 117:508-22. [DOI: 10.2466/13.pr0.117c20z0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
This study aimed to assess physicians' susceptibility to framing effects in clinical judgment and decision making. A survey was administered online to 159 general internists in the United States. Participants were randomized into two groups, in which clinical scenarios varied in their framings: frequency vs percentage, with cost information vs without, female patient vs male patient, and mortality vs survival. Results showed that physicians' recommendations for patients in hypothetical scenarios were significantly different when the predicted probability of the outcomes was presented in frequency versus percentage form and when it was presented in mortality rate vs survival rate of the same magnitude. Physicians' recommendations were not different for other framing effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thanh C. Bui
- Department of Behavioral Science, The University of Texas M. D. Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, Texas
| | - Heather A. Krieger
- Center for Medical Ethics and Health Policy, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
| | | |
Collapse
|
64
|
Abstract
This study examined the role of crisis advice appearing in US news coverage of the 2001 anthrax attacks. Coverage of any crisis can spark public outrage, including fear, speculation, and contradictory or confusing evidence, especially when the stories do not contain practical advice. Five coders analyzed 833 news stories from 272 major US newspapers, the Associated Press, National Public Radio, and 4 major US television networks. Practical advice appeared in only a quarter of the stories, even though practical advice for self-protection was mentioned 3 times more often than the vague advice that simply advised people not to panic. Public health officials provided the most practical advice, while scientists provided the least practical advice. Stories containing practical advice also provided more elucidating information, explaining why the threat was low, reducible, treatable, and detectable. Over the 3 phases of the anthrax crisis, an inverse relationship appeared between the amount of news coverage containing practical advice compared to "outrage rhetoric." Stories mentioned practical advice more often during the post-impact phase than earlier in the crisis. Elucidating, explanatory advice emphasized actions, risk comparisons, and tradeoffs. The findings indicate that when journalists use credible sources to provide practical advice and avoid speculation, their coverage can prevent the spread of misinformation and confusion during a bioterror attack. Also, journalists should provide context and sourcing when discussing advice during the outbreak and impact phases of the crisis, because these explanations could counteract outrage and threat distortion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristen Alley Swain
- Kristen Alley Swain, PhD, is Associate Professor, Meek School of Journalism and New Media, University of Mississippi , University, MS
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Communicating risk using absolute risk reduction or prolongation of life formats: cluster-randomised trial in general practice. Br J Gen Pract 2015; 64:e199-207. [PMID: 24686884 DOI: 10.3399/bjgp14x677824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND It is important that patients are well-informed about risks and benefits of therapies to help them decide whether to accept medical therapy. Different numerical formats can be used in risk communication but It remains unclear how the different formats affect decisions made by real-life patients. AIM To compare the impact of using Prolongation Of Life (POL) and Absolute Risk Reduction (ARR) information formats to express effectiveness of cholesterol-lowering therapy on patients' redemptions of statin prescriptions, and on patients' confidence in their decision and satisfaction with the risk communication. DESIGN AND SETTING Cluster-randomised clinical trial in general practices. Thirty-four Danish GPs from 23 practices participated in a primary care-based clinical trial concerning use of quantitative effectiveness formats for risk communication in health prevention consultations. METHOD GPs were cluster-randomised (treating practices as clusters) to inform patients about cardiovascular mortality risk and the effectiveness of statin treatment using either POL or ARR formats. Patients' redemptions of statin prescriptions were obtained from a regional prescription database. The COMRADE questionnaire was used to measure patients' confidence in their decision and satisfaction with the risk communication. RESULTS Of the 240 patients included for analyses, 112 were allocated to POL information and 128 to ARR. Patients redeeming a statin prescription totalled six (5.4%) when informed using POL, and 32 (25.0%) when using ARR. The level of confidence in decision and satisfaction with risk communication did not differ between the risk formats. CONCLUSION Patients redeemed statin prescriptions less often when their GP communicated treatment effectiveness using POL compared with ARR.
Collapse
|
66
|
Kunneman M, Stiggelbout AM, Marijnen CAM, Pieterse AH. Probabilities of benefit and harms of preoperative radiotherapy for rectal cancer: What do radiation oncologists tell and what do patients understand? PATIENT EDUCATION AND COUNSELING 2015; 98:1092-1098. [PMID: 26025810 DOI: 10.1016/j.pec.2015.05.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2015] [Revised: 05/01/2015] [Accepted: 05/14/2015] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Probabilities of benefits and harms of treatment may help patients when making a treatment decision. This study aimed to examine (1) whether and how radiation oncologists convey probabilities to rectal cancer patients, and (2) patients' estimates of probabilities of major outcomes of rectal cancer treatment. METHODS First consultations of oncologists and patients eligible for preoperative radiotherapy (PRT) (N=90) were audio taped. Tapes were transcribed verbatim and coded to identify probabilistic information presented. Patients (N=56) filled in a post-consultation questionnaire on their estimates of probabilities. RESULTS Probabilities were mentioned in 99% (local recurrence), 75% (incontinence), 72% and 40% (sexual dysfunction in males and females, respectively) of cases. Most patients (89%) correctly estimated that PRT decreases the probability of local recurrence, and 10% and 38%/54% that it increases the probability of incontinence and sexual dysfunction in males/females, respectively. Patients tended to underestimate the probabilities of harms of treatment. CONCLUSION Our results show that oncologists almost always mention probabilities of benefit of PRT. In contrast, probabilities of harms often go unmentioned. The effect of PRT on adverse events is often underestimated. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS Oncologists should stay alert to patients' possible misunderstanding of probabilistic information and should check patients' perceptions of probabilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marleen Kunneman
- Leiden University Medical Centre, Department of Medical Decision Making, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Anne M Stiggelbout
- Leiden University Medical Centre, Department of Medical Decision Making, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Corrie A M Marijnen
- Leiden University Medical Centre, Department of Clinical Oncology, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Arwen H Pieterse
- Leiden University Medical Centre, Department of Medical Decision Making, Leiden, the Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Doust J, Trevena L, Bastian H, Burgess J, Edwards AGK. Interventions for improving understanding and minimising the psychological impact of screening. Hippokratia 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.cd001212.pub3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jenny Doust
- Bond University; Centre for Research in Evidence Based Practice; Gold Coast Queensland Australia 4229
| | - Lyndal Trevena
- The University of Sydney; Room 322 Edward Ford Building (A27) Sydney NSW Australia 2006
| | - Hilda Bastian
- National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health; National Center for Biotechnology Information; 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda Maryland USA 20894
| | - Jacquii Burgess
- Bond University; Faculty of Health Sciences and Medicine; Gold Coast Queensland Australia 4229
| | - Adrian GK Edwards
- Cardiff University; Cochrane Institute of Primary Care and Public Health, School of Medicine; 2nd Floor, Neuadd Meirionnydd Heath Park Cardiff Wales UK CF14 4YS
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Seeking consent from those who cannot answer: new light on emergency research conducted under the exception from informed consent. Crit Care Med 2015; 43:710-1. [PMID: 25700060 DOI: 10.1097/ccm.0000000000000801] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
69
|
Deignan B, Hoffman-Goetz L. Emotional tone of ontario newspaper articles on the health effects of industrial wind turbines before and after policy change. JOURNAL OF HEALTH COMMUNICATION 2015; 20:531-538. [PMID: 25806896 DOI: 10.1080/10810730.2014.999894] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Newspapers are often a primary source of health information for the public about emerging technologies. Information in newspapers can amplify or attenuate readers' perceptions of health risk depending on how it is presented. Five geographically distinct wind energy installations in Ontario, Canada were identified, and newspapers published in their surrounding communities were systematically searched for articles on health effects from industrial wind turbines from May 2007 to April 2011. The authors retrieved 421 articles from 13 community, 2 provincial, and 2 national newspapers. To measure the emotional tone of the articles, the authors used a list of negative and positive words, informed from previous studies as well as from a random sample of newspaper articles included in this study. The majority of newspaper articles (64.6%, n = 272) emphasized negative rather than positive/neutral tone, with community newspapers publishing a higher proportion of negative articles than provincial or national newspapers, χ(2)(2) = 15.1, p < .001. Articles were more likely to be negative when published 2 years after compared with 2 years before provincial legislation to reduce dependence on fossil fuels (the Green Energy Act), χ(2)(3) = 9.7, p < .05. Repeated public exposure to negative newspaper content may heighten readers' health risk perceptions about wind energy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Deignan
- a School of Public Health and Health Systems , University of Waterloo , Waterloo , Ontario , Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
70
|
Hilton NZ, Scurich N, Helmus LM. Communicating the risk of violent and offending behavior: review and introduction to this special issue. BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2015; 33:1-18. [PMID: 25693950 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2160] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
How to communicate risk of recidivism in correctional and forensic contexts has been a subject of scholarly discussion for two decades. This emerging literature, however, is sparse compared with studies on the assessment of risk for violent and offending behavior. In this special issue of Behavioral Sciences and the Law, we have gathered together empirical and review papers exemplifying promising directions and methodologies. We begin with a review of the state of the field, and lessons that can be drawn from research into medical risk assessment and risk communication, finding that many of the same principles apply to the forensic context. How risks are framed, and how numerate assessors are, affects how risk information is understood and applied. We discuss the existing research bearing on these issues, as well as the conceptual, practical, empirical, and legal implications of communicating risk using numerical or categorical risk terms. Along with the seven articles in this volume, we suggest directions for future research on measuring and communicating change, understanding and managing the statistical literacy of those who use and communicate risk assessments, and developing a theoretical framework for forensic risk communication research. We hope this volume will help integrate and invigorate research into forensic risk communication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N Zoe Hilton
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, and Waypoint Research Institute, Penetanguishene, ON, Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
71
|
Effects of presenting risk information in different formats to cardiologists. A Latin American survey. ARCHIVOS DE CARDIOLOGIA DE MEXICO 2015; 85:3-8. [PMID: 25450431 DOI: 10.1016/j.acmx.2014.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2013] [Revised: 07/10/2014] [Accepted: 09/02/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Previous publications demonstrated that the presentation of treatment benefits in terms of relative risk reduction (RRR) rather than in terms of absolute risk reduction (ARR) or number of patients to treat (NNT) might favor the perception of outcome effectiveness. The objective was to perform a cognitive evaluation to assess how the manner in which risks and benefits of screening methods and treatments are presented can affect medical care decision-taking in a sample of cardiologists. METHODS Four-hundred and six Latin American cardiologists answered a questionnaire reporting the results of clinical trials presented as RRR, ARR or NNT, associated or not to biased graphs. RESULTS Cardiologists' decision-taking was different when comparing treatment benefits presented as RRR (62.2%) vs. ARR (40.4%) (p=0.000000), and as RRR vs. NNT (44.4%) (p=0.000000). However, their decision-taking was similar when information was presented as NNT or ARR (p=0.073). The inclusion of biased graphs was misinterpreted as an actual data difference (RRR: 61.6% vs. ARR: 14.0%, p=0.000000). CONCLUSIONS This study demonstrated that Latin American cardiologists could misinterpret statistical data when information of clinical trials is presented in terms of RRR. We emphasize the need to enhance cardiologists' training in quantitative techniques, to improve medical care decision-making.
Collapse
|
72
|
Godino JG, van Sluijs EMF, Sutton S, Griffin SJ. Understanding perceived risk of type 2 diabetes in healthy middle-aged adults: a cross-sectional study of associations with modelled risk, clinical risk factors, and psychological factors. Diabetes Res Clin Pract 2014; 106:412-9. [PMID: 25467619 PMCID: PMC4337811 DOI: 10.1016/j.diabres.2014.10.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2014] [Revised: 09/23/2014] [Accepted: 10/17/2014] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
AIMS To determine the perceived risk of type 2 diabetes in a sample of healthy middle-aged adults and examine the association between perceived risk and modelled risk, clinical risk factors, and psychological factors theorised to be antecedents of behaviour change. METHODS An exploratory, cross-sectional analysis of perceived risk of type 2 diabetes (framed according to time and in comparison with peers) was conducted using baseline data collected from 569 participants of the Diabetes Risk Communication Trial (Cambridgeshire, UK). Type 2 diabetes risk factors were measured during a health assessment and the Framingham Offspring Diabetes Risk Score was used to model risk. Questionnaires assessed psychological factors including anxiety, diabetes-related worry, behavioural intentions, and other theory-based antecedents of behaviour change. Multivariable regression analyses were used to examine associations between perceived risk and potential correlates. RESULTS Participants with a high perceived risk were at higher risk according to the Framingham Offspring Diabetes Risk Score (p<0.001). Higher perceived risk was observed in those with a higher body fat percentage, lower self-rated health, higher diabetes-related worry, and lower self-efficacy for adhering to governmental recommendations for physical activity (all p<0.001). The framing of perceived risk according to time and in comparison with peers did not influence these results. CONCLUSIONS High perceived risk of type 2 diabetes is associated with higher risk of developing the disease, and a decreased likelihood of engagement in risk-reducing health behaviours. Risk communication interventions should target high-risk individuals with messages about the effectiveness of prevention strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Job G Godino
- MRC Epidemiology Unit, Institute of Metabolic Science, University of Cambridge, Box 285, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, United Kingdom
| | - Esther M F van Sluijs
- MRC Epidemiology Unit, Institute of Metabolic Science, University of Cambridge, Box 285, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, United Kingdom
| | - Stephen Sutton
- Behavioural Science Group, Institute of Public Health, University of Cambridge, Forvie Site, Robinson Way, Cambridge CB2 0SR, United Kingdom
| | - Simon J Griffin
- MRC Epidemiology Unit, Institute of Metabolic Science, University of Cambridge, Box 285, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Edmonds BT, McKenzie F, Panoch J, Frankel RM. Comparing neonatal morbidity and mortality estimates across specialty in periviable counseling. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2014; 28:2145-9. [PMID: 25354284 PMCID: PMC4431952 DOI: 10.3109/14767058.2014.981807] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To describe and compare estimates of neonatal morbidity and mortality communicated by neonatologists and obstetricians in simulated periviable counseling encounters. METHODS A simulation-based study of 16 obstetricians (OBs) and 15 neonatologists counseling standardized patients portraying pregnant women with ruptured membranes at 23 weeks gestation. Two investigators tabulated all instances of numerically-described risk estimates across individuals and by specialty. RESULTS Overall, 12/15 (80%) neonatologists utilized numeric estimates of survival; 6/16 (38%) OBs did. OBs frequently deferred the discussion of "exact numbers" to neonatologists. The 12 neonatologists provided 13 unique numeric estimates, ranging from 3% to 50% survival. Half of those neonatologists provided two to three different estimates in a single encounter. By comparison, six OBs provided four unique survival estimates ("50%", "30-40%", "1/3-1/2", "<10%"). Only 2/15 (13%) neonatologists provided numeric estimates of survival without impairment. None of the neonatologists used the term "intact" survival, while five OBs did. Three neonatologists gave numeric estimates of long-term disability and one OB did. CONCLUSION We found substantial variation in estimates and noteworthy omissions of discussions related to long-term morbidity. Across specialties, we noted inconsistencies in the use and meaning of terms like "intact survival." More tools and training are needed to improve the quality and consistency of periviable risk-communication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Fatima McKenzie
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Indiana University School of Medicine
| | - Janet Panoch
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Indiana University School of Medicine
| | - Richard M. Frankel
- Mary Margaret Walther Center for Research and Education in Palliative Care, IU Simon Cancer Center
- Roudebush Veterans Affairs Medical Center
- Department of Medicine, Indiana University School of Medicine
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Rush KL, Kjorven M, Hole R. Older Adults’ Risk Practices From Hospital to Home: A Discourse Analysis. THE GERONTOLOGIST 2014; 56:494-503. [DOI: 10.1093/geront/gnu092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2014] [Accepted: 08/30/2014] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
|
75
|
Stell LK. Volume-outcome disparities and informed consent: What should surgeons disclose? J Surg Oncol 2014; 110:636-42. [DOI: 10.1002/jso.23718] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2014] [Accepted: 06/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Lance K. Stell
- Medical Humanities Program; Davidson College; Davidson North Carolina
- UNC Chapel Hill School of Medicine, Department of Internal Medicine; Carolinas Medical Center; Charlotte North Carolina
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Büchter RB, Fechtelpeter D, Knelangen M, Ehrlich M, Waltering A. Words or numbers? Communicating risk of adverse effects in written consumer health information: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 2014; 14:76. [PMID: 25155972 PMCID: PMC4153005 DOI: 10.1186/1472-6947-14-76] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2013] [Accepted: 08/20/2014] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Various types of framing can influence risk perceptions, which may have an impact on treatment decisions and adherence. One way of framing is the use of verbal terms in communicating the probabilities of treatment effects. We systematically reviewed the comparative effects of words versus numbers in communicating the probability of adverse effects to consumers in written health information. METHODS Nine electronic databases were searched up to November 2012. Teams of two reviewers independently assessed studies. INCLUSION CRITERIA randomised controlled trials; verbal versus numerical presentation; context: written consumer health information. RESULTS Ten trials were included. Participants perceived probabilities presented in verbal terms as higher than in numeric terms: commonly used verbal descriptors systematically led to an overestimation of the absolute risk of adverse effects (Range of means: 3% - 54%). Numbers also led to an overestimation of probabilities, but the overestimation was smaller (2% - 20%). The difference in means ranged from 3.8% to 45.9%, with all but one comparison showing significant results. Use of numbers increased satisfaction with the information (MD: 0.48 [CI: 0.32 to 0.63], p < 0.00001, I2 = 0%) and likelihood of medication use (MD for very common side effects: 1.45 [CI: 0.78 to 2.11], p = 0.0001, I2 = 68%; MD for common side effects: 0.90 [CI: 0.61 to 1.19], p < 0.00001, I2 = 1%; MD for rare side effects: 0.39 [0.02 to 0.76], p = 0.04, I2 = not applicable). Outcomes were measured on a 6-point Likert scale, suggesting small to moderate effects. CONCLUSIONS Verbal descriptors including "common", "uncommon" and "rare" lead to an overestimation of the probability of adverse effects compared to numerical information, if used as previously suggested by the European Commission. Numbers result in more accurate estimates and increase satisfaction and likelihood of medication use. Our review suggests that providers of consumer health information should quantify treatment effects numerically. Future research should focus on the impact of personal and contextual factors, use representative samples or be conducted in real life settings, measure behavioral outcomes and address whether benefit information can be described verbally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roland Brian Büchter
- Department of Health Information, Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care (IQWiG), Im Mediapark 8, 50670 Cologne, Germany
| | - Dennis Fechtelpeter
- Department of Health Information, Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care (IQWiG), Im Mediapark 8, 50670 Cologne, Germany
| | - Marco Knelangen
- Department of Health Information, Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care (IQWiG), Im Mediapark 8, 50670 Cologne, Germany
| | - Martina Ehrlich
- Department of Health Information, Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care (IQWiG), Im Mediapark 8, 50670 Cologne, Germany
| | - Andreas Waltering
- Department of Health Information, Institute for Quality and Efficiency in Health Care (IQWiG), Im Mediapark 8, 50670 Cologne, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Patel S, Ngunjiri A, Hee SW, Yang Y, Brown S, Friede T, Griffiths F, Lord J, Sandhu H, Thistlethwaite J, Tysall C, Underwood M. Primum non nocere: shared informed decision making in low back pain--a pilot cluster randomised trial. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2014; 15:282. [PMID: 25146587 PMCID: PMC4247192 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2474-15-282] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2014] [Accepted: 07/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Low back pain is a common and disabling condition leading to large health service and societal costs. Although there are several treatment options for back pain little is known about how to improve patient choice in treatment selection. The purpose of this study was to pilot a decision support package to help people choose between low back pain treatments. Methods This was a single-centred pilot cluster randomised controlled trial conducted in a community physiotherapy service. We included adults with non-specific low back pain referred for physiotherapy. Intervention participants were sent an information booklet prior to their first consultation. Intervention physiotherapists were trained to enhance their skills in shared informed decision making. Those in the control arm received care as usual. The primary outcome was satisfaction with the treatment received at four months using a five-point Likert Scale dichotomised into “satisfaction” (very satisfied or somewhat satisfied) and “non-satisfaction” (neither satisfied nor dissatisfied, somewhat dissatisfied or very dissatisfied). Results We recruited 148 participants. In the control arm 67% of participants were satisfied with their treatment and in the intervention arm 53%. The adjusted relative risk of being satisfied was 1.28 (95% confidence interval 0.79 to 2.09). For most secondary outcomes the trend was towards worse outcomes in the intervention group. For one measure; the Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire, this difference was clinically important (2.27, 95% confidence interval 0.08 to 4.47). Mean healthcare costs were slightly lower (£38 saving per patient) within the intervention arm but health outcomes were also less favourable (0.02 fewer QALYs); the estimated probability that the intervention would be cost-effective at an incremental threshold of £20,000 per QALY was 16%. Conclusion We did not find that this decision support package improved satisfaction with treatment; it may have had a substantial negative effect on clinical outcome, and is very unlikely to prove cost-effective. That a decision support package might have a clinically important detrimental effect is of concern. To our knowledge this has not been observed previously. Decision support packages should be formally tested for clinical and cost-effectiveness, and safety before implementation. Trial registration Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN46035546 registered on 11/02/10. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/1471-2474-15-282) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shilpa Patel
- Division of Health Sciences, Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, CV4 7AL Coventry, UK.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
78
|
Burton M, Collins K, Caldon LJM, Wyld L, Reed MWR. Information Needs of Older Women Faced with a Choice of Primary Endocrine Therapy or Surgery for Early-Stage Breast Cancer: A Literature Review. CURRENT BREAST CANCER REPORTS 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s12609-014-0159-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
79
|
Leach CJ, Hodgson L, Defever E, Ives R. Communicating risk and shared decision-making in osteopathic practice: A pilot study using focus groups to test a patient information leaflet. Eur J Integr Med 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.eujim.2014.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
|
80
|
Tait AR, Teig MK, Voepel-Lewis T. Informed consent for anesthesia: a review of practice and strategies for optimizing the consent process. Can J Anaesth 2014; 61:832-42. [PMID: 24898765 DOI: 10.1007/s12630-014-0188-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2014] [Accepted: 05/21/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE Patients must receive information in a manner that promotes understanding so they can make informed decisions about anesthesia and other medical interventions. Unfortunately, history is replete with examples of the negative consequences of inadequate disclosure of information and lack of patient understanding. While obtaining consent for anesthesia poses unique challenges, the ability of the anesthesiologist to engage the patient in meaningful discussion is critical as a means to ensure that the patient is truly informed. This narrative review aims to: 1) discuss the process of informed consent as it applies to anesthesia practice; 2) describe the salient issues related to patient capacity, disclosure, understanding, decision-making, and documentation of the informed consent process; and 3) discuss current strategies to improve the presentation and understanding of consent information. SOURCE Review of the extant literature, including the authors' own research. PRINCIPAL FINDINGS Despite the ethical imperative of informed consent, many decision-makers have limited understanding of medical information. The reasons for this are multifactorial but often result from incomplete disclosure and presentation of generic information that does not take into account differences in information needs, values, and preferences of individual patients. Several simple strategies are available, however, that can enhance decision-makers' understanding of both written and verbal information. CONCLUSIONS Despite the unique challenges of obtaining consent for anesthesia on the day of surgery, attention to the manner in which information for anesthesia care is provided and adoption of simple strategies to enhance understanding can go a long way to ensure that decision-makers are appropriately informed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alan R Tait
- Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan Health System, 1500 E. Medical Center Drive, Ann Arbor, MI, 48109, USA,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
81
|
Bodemer N, Meder B, Gigerenzer G. Communicating Relative Risk Changes with Baseline Risk: Presentation Format and Numeracy Matter. Med Decis Making 2014; 34:615-26. [PMID: 24803429 DOI: 10.1177/0272989x14526305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/25/2012] [Accepted: 02/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Treatment benefits and harms are often communicated as relative risk reductions and increases, which are frequently misunderstood by doctors and patients. One suggestion for improving understanding of such risk information is to also communicate the baseline risk. We investigated 1) whether the presentation format of the baseline risk influences understanding of relative risk changes and 2) the mediating role of people's numeracy skills. METHOD We presented laypeople (N = 1234) with a hypothetical scenario about a treatment that decreased (Experiments 1a, 2a) or increased (Experiments 1b, 2b) the risk of heart disease. Baseline risk was provided as a percentage or a frequency. In a forced-choice paradigm, the participants' task was to judge the risk in the treatment group given the relative risk reduction (or increase) and the baseline risk. Numeracy was assessed using the Lipkus 11-item scale. RESULTS Communicating baseline risk in a frequency format facilitated correct understanding of a treatment's benefits and harms, whereas a percentage format often impeded understanding. For example, many participants misinterpreted a relative risk reduction as referring to an absolute risk reduction. Participants with higher numeracy generally performed better than those with lower numeracy, but all participants benefitted from a frequency format. Limitations are that we used a hypothetical medical scenario and a nonrepresentative sample. CONCLUSIONS Presenting baseline risk in a frequency format improves understanding of relative risk information, whereas a percentage format is likely to lead to misunderstandings. People's numeracy skills play an important role in correctly understanding medical information. Overall, communicating treatment benefits and harms in the form of relative risk changes remains problematic, even when the baseline risk is explicitly provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolai Bodemer
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Berlin, Germany (NB, GG),Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Center for Adaptive Behavior and Cognition (ABC), Berlin, Germany (NB, BM, GG)
| | - Björn Meder
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Center for Adaptive Behavior and Cognition (ABC), Berlin, Germany (NB, BM, GG)
| | - Gerd Gigerenzer
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Berlin, Germany (NB, GG),Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Center for Adaptive Behavior and Cognition (ABC), Berlin, Germany (NB, BM, GG)
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Dicke JM, Van Duyne L, Bradshaw R. The utilization and choices of aneuploidy screening in a midwestern population. J Genet Couns 2014; 23:874-80. [PMID: 24777550 PMCID: PMC4156785 DOI: 10.1007/s10897-014-9711-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2012] [Accepted: 03/07/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The types, interpretation, and use of first- and second-trimester aneuploidy screening are often unclear for many women. This impairs appropriate decision making and understanding of the implications of prenatal genetic testing options. The purpose of this study was to examine the utilization of Stepwise Sequential screening in our Midwestern population, demographic factors associated with choice of screening and method of risk reporting and it's potential impact on women's choices. First trimester screening was performed for 2,634 women during the study period. Results were not reported or "framed" as "positive" or "negative". Rather, the specific age-risk and screen-risk for T21 were relayed, along with options for follow-up Stepwise Sequential screening and invasive testing. Nearly 80 % of women declined Stepwise Sequential screening. Minorities and women of lower education were least likely to pursue further screening. Less than 4 % of the study population elected invasive testing. First trimester screening was associated with a 53 % reduction in amniocenteses and 20 % fewer CVS's compared to pre-first trimester screening availability. Reporting age-and screen-risks for T21, rather than classifying results as "positive" or "negative" based on a pre-determined threshold, was associated with a low uptake of further testing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey M Dicke
- Department Obstetrics and Gynecology, Washington University in St. Louis, School of Medicine, 660 S. Euclid Avenue, Campus Box 8064, St. Louis, MO, 63110-1094, USA,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
83
|
Kellar I, Mason D. Social patterning in knowledge following an informed choice invitation for type 2 diabetes screening. Diabet Med 2014; 31:504-8. [PMID: 24117707 DOI: 10.1111/dme.12334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2013] [Revised: 07/26/2013] [Accepted: 09/19/2013] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
AIMS To describe prevalence of knowledge of items specified by the U.K. General Medical Council as required to make an informed choice following an invitation for screening for type 2 diabetes and investigate whether knowledge was socio-economically patterned. METHODS A 9-item knowledge questionnaire was employed immediately following an informed choice invitation to type 2 diabetes screening that was piloted with 278 people between 40 and 69 years in the U.K. between February and April 2006. RESULTS With the exception of post-diagnosis treatment and the effectiveness of early treatment in preventing long-term problems, information was typically understood correctly. Social patterning was observed: individuals who left full-time education before 19 years of age were less likely to understand the most likely test result, the effectiveness of early treatment in preventing long-term problems or the possible harms of screening. CONCLUSIONS Even risk communication materials developed for ease of readability can result in inequity, limiting autonomy in healthcare decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I Kellar
- Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds
| | | |
Collapse
|
84
|
Applying a knowledge translation model to the uptake of the Baby Friendly Health Initiative in the Australian health care system. Women Birth 2014; 27:79-85. [PMID: 24686049 DOI: 10.1016/j.wombi.2014.03.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2013] [Revised: 02/21/2014] [Accepted: 03/01/2014] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The Baby Friendly Hospital Initiative is a global, evidence-based, public health initiative. The evidence underpinning the Initiative supports practices promoting the initiation and maintenance of breastfeeding and encourages women's informed infant feeding decisions. In Australia, where the Initiative is known as the Baby Friendly Health Initiative (BFHI) the translation of evidence into practice has not been uniform, as demonstrated by a varying number of maternity facilities in each State and Territory currently accredited as 'baby friendly'. This variance has persisted regardless of BFHI implementation in Australia gaining 'in principle' support at a national and governmental level as well as inclusion in health policy in several states. There are many stakeholders that exert an influence on policy development and health care practices. AIM Identify a theory and model to examine where and how barriers occur in the gap between evidence and practice in the uptake of the BFHI in Australia. RESULTS Knowledge translation theory and the research to practice pipeline model are used to examine the identified barriers to BFHI implementation and accreditation in Australia. CONCLUSION Australian and international studies have identified similar issues that have either enabled implementation of the BFHI or acted as a barrier. Knowledge translation theory and the research to practice pipeline model is of practical value to examine barriers. Recommendations in the form of specific targeted strategies to facilitate knowledge transfer and supportive practices into the Australian health care system and current midwifery practice are included.
Collapse
|
85
|
Lin PJ, Concannon TW, Greenberg D, Cohen JT, Rossi G, Hille J, Auerbach HR, Fang CH, Nadler ES, Neumann PJ. Does framing of cancer survival affect perceived value of care? A willingness-to-pay survey of US residents. Expert Rev Pharmacoecon Outcomes Res 2014; 13:513-22. [PMID: 23977977 DOI: 10.1586/14737167.2013.814948] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
AIMS To investigate the relationship between the framing of survival gains and the perceived value of cancer care. METHODS Through a population-based survey of 2040 US adults, respondents were randomized to one of the two sets of hypothetical scenarios, each of which described the survival benefit for a new treatment as either an increase in median survival time (median survival), or an increase in the probability of survival for a given length of time (landmark survival). Each respondent was presented with two randomly selected scenarios with different prognosis and survival improvements, and asked about their willingness to pay (WTP) for the new treatments. RESULTS Predicted WTP increased with survival benefits and respondents' income, regardless of how survival benefits were described. Framing therapeutic benefits as improvements in landmark rather than median time survival increased the proportion of the population willing to pay for that gain by 11-35%, and the mean WTP amount by 42-72% in the scenarios we compared. CONCLUSION How survival benefits are described may influence the value people place on cancer care.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pei-Jung Lin
- Center for the Evaluation of Value and Risk in Health, Institute for Clinical Research and Health Policy Studies, Tufts Medical Center, Boston, MA, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
86
|
Holton A, Lee N, Coleman R. Commenting on health: a framing analysis of user comments in response to health articles online. JOURNAL OF HEALTH COMMUNICATION 2014; 19:825-837. [PMID: 24446785 DOI: 10.1080/10810730.2013.837554] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Public health officials have continually urged journalists and other members of the news media to ease off health frames that focus on individuals and to instead promote broader societal frames. Although some scholarly research has reinforced these pleas, none has examined the interplay between frames of health news coverage and resulting public comments. The current online environment invites such an analysis, allowing news organizations to post articles online and the public to comment on those articles. Using a content analysis, this study reveals thematic frames in online health stories may drive down gain-oriented responses, while episodic frames may prompt the public to share more personal comments. Furthermore, the findings examine other textual factors--gain and loss frames and mobilizing information--that may be driving the volume and frames of user comments to health stories online.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Avery Holton
- a Department of Communication , University of Utah , Salt Lake City , Utah , USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
87
|
Loon MSKV, van Dijk-de Vries A, van der Weijden T, Elwyn G, Widdershoven GAM. Ethical issues in cardiovascular risk management. Nurs Ethics 2013; 21:540-53. [DOI: 10.1177/0969733013505313] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Involving patients in decisions on primary prevention can be questioned from an ethical perspective, due to a tension between health promotion activities and patient autonomy. A nurse-led intervention for prevention of cardiovascular diseases, including counselling (risk communication, and elements of shared decision-making and motivational interviewing) and supportive tools such as a decision aid, was implemented in primary care. The aim of this study was to evaluate the nurse-led intervention from an ethical perspective by exploring in detail the experiences of patients with the intervention, and their views on the role of both the nurse and patient. The study had a qualitative design. 18 patients who had received the intervention participated. Data were gathered by in-depth interviews. The interviews were analysed using directed content analysis. The findings revealed that patients perceived the consultations not as an infringement on their autonomy, but as supportive to risk reduction efforts they tried but found hard to realise. They specifically emphasised the role of the nurse, and appreciated the nurse's realistic advice, encouragement, and help in understanding. Patients' views on and experiences with risk management are in line with notions of relational autonomy, caring cooperation and communicative action found in the literature. We conclude that patients define the relationship with the nurse as shared work in the process of developing a healthier lifestyle.
Collapse
|
88
|
Carson-Stevens A, Davies MM, Jones R, Chik ADP, Robbé IJ, Fiander AN. Framing patient consent for student involvement in pelvic examination: a dual model of autonomy. JOURNAL OF MEDICAL ETHICS 2013; 39:676-680. [PMID: 23322682 DOI: 10.1136/medethics-2012-100809] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
Patient consent has been formulated in terms of radical individualism rather than shared benefits. Medical education relies on the provision of patient consent to provide medical students with the training and experience to become competent doctors. Pelvic examination represents an extreme case in which patients may legitimately seek to avoid contact with inexperienced medical students particularly where these are male. However, using this extreme case, this paper will examine practices of framing and obtaining consent as perceived by medical students. This paper reports findings of an exploratory qualitative study of medical students and junior doctors. Participants described a number of barriers to obtaining informed consent. These related to misunderstandings concerning student roles and experiences and insufficient information on the nature of the examination. Participants reported perceptions of the negative framing of decisions on consent by nursing staff where the student was male. Potentially coercive practices of framing of the decision by senior doctors were also reported. Participants outlined strategies they adopted to circumvent patients' reasons for refusal. Practices of framing the information used by students, nurses and senior doctors to enable patients to decide about consent are discussed in the context of good ethical practice. In the absence of a clear ethical model, coercion appears likely. We argue for an expanded model of autonomy in which the potential tension between respecting patients' autonomy and ensuring the societal benefit of well-trained doctors is recognised. Practical recommendations are made concerning information provision and clear delineations of student and patient roles and expectations.
Collapse
|
89
|
Wiedemann PM, Schuetz H, Boerner F, Clauberg M, Croft R, Shukla R, Kikkawa T, Kemp R, Gutteling JM, de Villiers B, da Silva Medeiros FN, Barnett J. When precaution creates misunderstandings: the unintended effects of precautionary information on perceived risks, the EMF case. RISK ANALYSIS : AN OFFICIAL PUBLICATION OF THE SOCIETY FOR RISK ANALYSIS 2013; 33:1788-1801. [PMID: 23551041 DOI: 10.1111/risa.12034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
In the past decade, growing public concern about novel technologies with uncertain potential long-term impacts on the environment and human health has moved risk policies toward a more precautionary approach. Focusing on mobile telephony, the effects of precautionary information on risk perception were analyzed. A pooled multinational experimental study based on a 5 × 2 × 2 factorial design was conducted in nine countries. The first factor refers to whether or not information on different types of precautionary measures was present, the second factor to the framing of the precautionary information, and the third factor to the order in which cell phones and base stations were rated by the study participants. The data analysis on the country level indicates different effects. The main hypothesis that informing about precautionary measures results in increased risk perceptions found only partial support in the data. The effects are weaker, both in terms of the effect size and the frequency of significant effects, across the various precautionary information formats used in the experiment. Nevertheless, our findings do not support the assumption that informing people about implemented precautionary measures will decrease public concerns.
Collapse
|
90
|
Abstract
STUDY DESIGN Blinded parallel-group randomized controlled trial. OBJECTIVE Establish the frequency and severity of adverse effects from short-term usual chiropractic treatment of the spine when compared with a sham treatment group. SUMMARY OF BACKGROUND DATA Previous studies have demonstrated that adverse events occur during chiropractic treatment. However, as a result of design limitations in previous studies, particularly the lack of sham-controlled randomized trials, understanding of these adverse events and their relation with chiropractic treatment is suboptimal. METHODS We conducted a trial to examine the occurrence of adverse events resulting from chiropractic treatment. It was conducted across 12 chiropractic clinics in Perth, Western Australia. The participants comprised 183 adults, aged 20 to 85 years, with spinal pain. Ninety-two participants received individualized care consistent with the chiropractors' usual treatment approach; 91 participants received a sham intervention. Each participant received 2 treatments. RESULTS Completed adverse questionnaires were returned by 94.5% of the participants after appointment 1 and 91.3% after appointment 2. Thirty-three percent of the sham group and 42% of the usual care group reported at least 1 adverse event. Common adverse events were increased pain (sham 29%; usual care 36%), muscle stiffness (sham 29%; usual care 37%), and headache (sham 17%; usual care 9%). The relative risk (RR) was not significant for adverse event occurrence (RR = 1.24; 95% CI: 0.85-1.81), occurrence of severe adverse events (RR = 1.9; 95% CI: 0.98-3.99), adverse event onset (RR = 0.16; 95% CI: 0.02-1.34), or adverse event duration (RR = 1.13; 95% CI: 0.59-2.18). No serious adverse events were reported. CONCLUSION A substantial proportion of adverse events after chiropractic treatment may result from natural history variation and nonspecific effects. LEVEL OF EVIDENCE 2.
Collapse
|
91
|
Garcia-Retamero R, Cokely ET. Simple but powerful health messages for increasing condom use in young adults. JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 2013; 52:30-42. [PMID: 24007406 DOI: 10.1080/00224499.2013.806647] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
In a large longitudinal study involving young adults, we conducted an eight-hour STD educational intervention and examined the impact of the intervention on the efficacy of a message for promoting condom use. The message was framed in positive or negative terms and was presented visually or in numbers (percentages or frequencies). Results indicated that the numerical positive-framed message increased condom use among young adults who did not receive the intervention, whereas the numerical negative-framed message did not. Attitudes toward condom use along with changes in intentions to use condoms mediated this framing effect. In contrast, the positive-framed and negative-framed messages were equally and highly effective for promoting condom use when the messages were presented visually or when young adults received the STD educational intervention before reading the message, suggesting that the simple brochures featuring visual aids were as effective in changing attitudes and behavioral intentions as the extensive intervention. These findings add to a growing body of evidence detailing the mechanisms that allow well-constructed visual aids to be among the most effective, transparent, memorable, and ethically desirable means of risk communication. Clinical and public health implications are discussed.
Collapse
|
92
|
Schwarze ML, Kehler JM, Campbell TC. Navigating high risk procedures with more than just a street map. J Palliat Med 2013; 16:1169-71. [PMID: 24003993 DOI: 10.1089/jpm.2013.0221] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Margaret L Schwarze
- 1 Department of Surgery, Division of Vascular Surgery, University of Wisconsin , Madison, Wisconsin
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
93
|
Scherer LD, Ubel PA, McClure J, Greene SM, Alford SH, Holtzman L, Exe N, Fagerlin A. Belief in numbers: When and why women disbelieve tailored breast cancer risk statistics. PATIENT EDUCATION AND COUNSELING 2013; 92:253-9. [PMID: 23623330 PMCID: PMC4208303 DOI: 10.1016/j.pec.2013.03.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/11/2012] [Revised: 03/11/2013] [Accepted: 03/22/2013] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To examine when and why women disbelieve tailored information about their risk of developing breast cancer. METHODS 690 women participated in an online program to learn about medications that can reduce the risk of breast cancer. The program presented tailored information about each woman's personal breast cancer risk. Half of women were told how their risk numbers were calculated, whereas the rest were not. Later, they were asked whether they believed that the program was personalized, and whether they believed their risk numbers. If a woman did not believe her risk numbers, she was asked to explain why. RESULTS Beliefs that the program was personalized were enhanced by explaining the risk calculation methods in more detail. Nonetheless, nearly 20% of women did not believe their personalized risk numbers. The most common reason for rejecting the risk estimate was a belief that it did not fully account for personal and family history. CONCLUSIONS The benefits of tailored risk statistics may be attenuated by a tendency for people to be skeptical that these risk estimates apply to them personally. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS Decision aids may provide risk information that is not accepted by patients, but addressing the patients' personal circumstances may lead to greater acceptance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura D Scherer
- VA Ann Arbor Center for Clinical Management Research, Ann Arbor, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
94
|
Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review. Ann Behav Med 2013; 43:101-16. [PMID: 21993844 DOI: 10.1007/s12160-011-9308-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 586] [Impact Index Per Article: 48.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Message framing has been an important focus in health communication research, yet prior meta-analyses found limited support for using framing to increase persuasiveness of health messages. PURPOSE This meta-analysis distinguished the outcomes used to assess the persuasive impact of framed messages (attitudes, intentions, or behavior). METHODS One hundred eighty-nine effect sizes were identified from 94 peer-reviewed, published studies which compared the persuasive impact of gain- and loss-framed messages. RESULTS Gain-framed messages were more likely than loss-framed messages to encourage prevention behaviors (r = 0.083, p = 0.002), particularly for skin cancer prevention, smoking cessation, and physical activity. No effect of framing was found when persuasion was assessed by attitudes/intentions or among studies encouraging detection. CONCLUSIONS Gain-framed messages appear to be more effective than loss-framed messages in promoting prevention behaviors. Research should examine the contexts in which loss-framed messages are most effective, and the processes that mediate the effects of framing on behavior.
Collapse
|
95
|
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This paper aims to provide an overview on the nocebo effect, focusing on recognition - its phenomenology, at-risk demographic profiles, clinical situations and personality factors, as well as discriminating somatic symptoms in the general population from treatment-related adverse effects. Lastly, the paper addresses available evidence-based strategies for management and minimisation of the nocebo effect. METHOD Data for this paper were identified by searching PubMed using the search terms "nocebo" and "nocebo effect", augmented by a manual search of the references of the key papers and the related literature. RESULTS The nocebo effect refers to non-pharmacodynamic, harmful or undesirable effects occurring after inactive treatment, a phenomenon that also occurs in the context of active therapy. Known drivers include classical conditioning and negative expectations concerning treatment. Recent meta-analyses have reported a considerable prevalence, ranging from 18% in the symptomatic treatment of migraine, to more than 74% in multiple sclerosis. Recognition of the nocebo-driven adverse effects presents a challenge, especially because of its non-specific nature and the similarity to the active medication's expected profile. Traits such as neuroticism, pessimism and type A personalities may predispose individuals to this phenomenon. Clinical management of the nocebo effect includes awareness and recognition, changing the manner of disclosure of potential drug-related adverse effects, shaping patients' expectations and enhancing the treatment alliance. CONCLUSION The nocebo effect is a common, clinically significant, yet covert driver of clinical outcomes. Increased awareness of its features, as well as knowledge of strategies on how to manage it, are fundamental so that clinicians can mitigate its impact on clinical practice.
Collapse
|
96
|
Adekanmbi VT, Uthman OA, Mudasiru OM. Exploring variations in childhood stunting in Nigeria using league table, control chart and spatial analysis. BMC Public Health 2013; 13:361. [PMID: 23597167 PMCID: PMC3640947 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-361] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2012] [Accepted: 04/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Stunting, linear growth retardation is the best measure of child health inequalities as it captures multiple dimensions of children's health, development and environment where they live. The developmental priorities and socially acceptable health norms and practices in various regions and states within Nigeria remains disaggregated and with this, comes the challenge of being able to ascertain which of the regions and states identifies with either high or low childhood stunting to further investigate the risk factors and make recommendations for action oriented policy decisions. METHODS We used data from the birth histories included in the 2008 Nigeria Demographic and Health Survey (DHS) to estimate childhood stunting. Stunting was defined as height for age below minus two standard deviations from the median height for age of the standard World Health Organization reference population. We plotted control charts of the proportion of childhood stunting for the 37 states (including federal capital, Abuja) in Nigeria. The Local Indicators of Spatial Association (LISA) were used as a measure of the overall clustering and is assessed by a test of a null hypothesis. RESULTS Childhood stunting is high in Nigeria with an average of about 39%. The percentage of children with stunting ranged from 11.5% in Anambra state to as high as 60% in Kebbi State. Ranking of states with respect to childhood stunting is as follows: Anambra and Lagos states had the least numbers with 11.5% and 16.8% respectively while Yobe, Zamfara, Katsina, Plateau and Kebbi had the highest (with more than 50% of their under-fives having stunted growth). CONCLUSIONS Childhood stunting is high in Nigeria and varied significantly across the states. The northern states have a higher proportion than the southern states. There is an urgent need for studies to explore factors that may be responsible for these special cause variations in childhood stunting in Nigeria.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victor T Adekanmbi
- Institute of Public Health, Obafemi Awolowo University, Ile-Ife, Nigeria.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
97
|
Harmsen CG, Jarbøl DE, Nexøe J, Støvring H, Gyrd-Hansen D, Nielsen JB, Edwards A, Kristiansen IS. Impact of effectiveness information format on patient choice of therapy and satisfaction with decisions about chronic disease medication: the "Influence of intervention Methodologies on Patient Choice of Therapy (IMPACT)" cluster-randomised trial in general practice. BMC Health Serv Res 2013; 13:76. [PMID: 23442351 PMCID: PMC3599428 DOI: 10.1186/1472-6963-13-76] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/09/2013] [Accepted: 02/14/2013] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Risk communication is an integral part of shared decision-making in health care. In the context of interventions for chronic diseases it represents a particular challenge for all health practitioners. By using two different quantitative formats to communicate risk level and effectiveness of a cholesterol-lowering drug, we posed the research question: how does the format of risk information influence patients’ decisions concerning therapy, patients’ satisfaction with the communication as well as confidence in the decision. We hypothesise that patients are less prone to accept therapy when the benefits of long-term intervention are presented in terms of prolongation of life (POL) in months compared to the absolute risk reduction (ARR). We hypothesise that patients presented with POL will be more satisfied with the communication and confident in their decision, suggesting understanding of the time-related term. Methods/Design In 2009 a sample of 328 general practitioners (GPs) in the Region of Southern Denmark was invited to participate in a primary care-based clinical trial among patients making real-life clinical decisions together with their GP. Interested GPs were cluster-randomised to inform patients about cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk and the effectiveness of statin therapy using either POL or ARR. The GPs attended a training session before informing their patients. Before training and after the trial period they received a questionnaire about their attitudes to risk communication and the use of numerical information. Patients’ redemptions of statin prescriptions will be registered in a regional prescription database to evaluate a possible association between redemption rates and effectiveness format. The Combined Outcome Measure for Risk Communication And Treatment Decision Making Effectiveness (COMRADE) questionnaire will be used to measure patients’ confidence and satisfaction with the risk communication immediately after the conversation with their GPs. Discussion This randomised clinical trial compares the impact of two effectiveness formats on real-life risk communication between patients and GPs, including affective patient outcomes and actual choices about acceptance of therapy. Though we found difficulties in recruiting GPs, according to the study protocol we have succeeded in engaging sufficient GPs for the trial, enabling us to perform the planned analyses. Trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov Protocol Registration System
http://ww.clinicaltrials.gov/NCT01414751
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlotte Gry Harmsen
- Research Unit of General Practice, University of Southern Denmark, Southern Denmark, Denmark.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
98
|
Raymond CM, Spoehr J. The acceptability of climate change in agricultural communities: comparing responses across variability and change. JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL MANAGEMENT 2013; 115:69-77. [PMID: 23246767 DOI: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2012.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2012] [Revised: 08/26/2012] [Accepted: 11/01/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
This study examined how the terms used to describe climate change influence landholder acceptability judgements and attitudes toward climate change at the local scale. Telephone surveys were conducted with landholders from viticultural (n = 97) or cereal growing (n = 195) backgrounds in rural South Australia. A variety of descriptive and inferential statistics were used to examine the influence of human-induced climate change and winter/spring drying trend terms on adaptation responses and uncertainties surrounding climate change science. We found that the terms used to describe climate change leads to significant differences in adaptation response and levels of scepticism surrounding climate change in rural populations. For example, those respondents who accepted human induced climate change as a reality were significantly more likely to invest in technologies to sow crops earlier or increase the amount of water stored or harvested on their properties than respondents who accepted the winter/spring drying trend as a reality. The results have implications for the targeting of climate change science messages to both rural landholders and communities of practice involved in climate change adaptation planning and implementation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher M Raymond
- Institute for Land, Water and Society, Charles Sturt University, Elizabeth Mitchell Drive, Albury, NSW 2640, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Karmali KN, Lloyd-Jones DM. Adding a life-course perspective to cardiovascular-risk communication. Nat Rev Cardiol 2013; 10:111-5. [DOI: 10.1038/nrcardio.2012.185] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
|
100
|
Knox SA, Viney RC, Gu Y, Hole AR, Fiebig DG, Street DJ, Haas MR, Weisberg E, Bateson D. The effect of adverse information and positive promotion on women's preferences for prescribed contraceptive products. Soc Sci Med 2013; 83:70-80. [PMID: 23351426 DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.12.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2011] [Revised: 10/07/2012] [Accepted: 12/20/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Recent rapid growth in the range of contraceptive products has given women more choice, but also adds complexity to the resultant decision of which product to choose. This paper uses a discrete choice experiment (DCE) to investigate the effect of adverse information and positive promotion on women's stated preferences for prescribed contraceptive products. In November 2007, 527 Australian women aged 18-49 years were recruited from an online panel. Each was randomly allocated to one of three information conditions. The control group only received basic information on contraceptive products. One treatment group also received adverse information on the risks of the combined oral pill. The other group received basic information and promotional material on the vaginal ring, newly introduced into Australia and on the transdermal patch, which is unavailable in Australia. Respondents completed 32 choice sets with 3 product options where each option was described by a product label: either combined pill, minipill, injection, implant, hormonal IUD, hormonal vaginal ring, hormonal transdermal patch or copper IUD; and by the attributes: effect on acne, effect on weight, frequency of administration, contraceptive effectiveness, doctor's recommendation, effect on periods and cost. Women's choices were analysed using a generalized multinomial logit model (G-MNL) and model estimates were used to predict product shares for each information condition. The predictions indicated that adverse information did not affect women's preferences for products relative to only receiving basic information. The promotional material increased women's preferences for the transdermal patch. Women in all groups had a low preference for the vaginal ring which was not improved by promotion. The findings highlight the need for researchers to pay attention to setting the context when conducting DCEs as this can significantly affect results.
Collapse
|