101
|
Harvey DY, Traut HJ, Middleton EL. Semantic interference in speech error production in a randomized continuous naming task: Evidence from aphasia. LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE 2018; 34:69-86. [PMID: 30619906 PMCID: PMC6319938 DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1501500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2017] [Accepted: 07/09/2018] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
Naming pictures from the same semantic category hinders subsequent naming from that category (i.e., semantic interference), irrespective of the number of intervening different-category exemplars named. Persistent semantic interference has been well documented in chronometric studies, and has been attributed to experience-driven adjustments in the strength of connections between semantic and lexical representations. However, whether parallel effects exist in speech error data remains unclear. In the current study, people with aphasia, a speaker population prone to naming errors, provided naming responses to a large picture corpus presented in random order that comprised multiple exemplars drawn from several different categories. We found persistent semantic interference in the task in semantic error rates specifically, and that semantic similarity between consecutive related exemplars modulated the effect. The results provide further evidence for the presumed lexical-semantic locus and mechanism(s) underlying semantic interference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Denise Y. Harvey
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, Pennsylvania, USA
- Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Hilary J. Traut
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, Pennsylvania, USA
- Now at the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
102
|
Breining BL, Nozari N, Rapp B. Learning in complex, multi-component cognitive systems: Different learning challenges within the same system. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2018; 45:1093-1106. [PMID: 30035563 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000630] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Using word learning as an example of a complex system, we investigated how differences in the structure of the subcomponents in which learning occurs can have significant consequences for the challenge of integrating new information within such systems. Learning a new word involves integrating information into the two key stages/subcomponents of processing within the word production system. In the first stage, multiple semantic features are mapped onto a single word. Conversely, in the second stage, a single word is mapped onto multiple segmental features. We tested whether the unitary goal of word learning leads to different local outcomes in these two stages because of their reversed mapping patterns. Neurotypical individuals (N = 17) learned names and semantic features for pictures of unfamiliar objects presented in semantically related, segmentally related and unrelated blocks. Both similarity types interfered with word learning. However, feature learning was differentially affected within the two subcomponents of word production. Semantic similarity facilitated learning distinctive semantic features (i.e., features unique to each item), whereas segmental similarity facilitated learning shared segmental features (i.e., features common to several items in a block). These results are compatible with an incremental learning model in which learning not only strengthens certain associations but also weakens others according to the local goals of each subcomponent. More generally, they demonstrate that the same overall learning goal can lead to opposite learning outcomes in the subcomponents of a complex system. The general principles uncovered may extend beyond word learning to other complex systems with multiple subcomponents. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
103
|
Schuchard J, Middleton EL. The Roles of Retrieval Practice Versus Errorless Learning in Strengthening Lexical Access in Aphasia. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2018; 61:1700-1717. [PMID: 29946697 PMCID: PMC6195057 DOI: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0352] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2017] [Revised: 01/31/2018] [Accepted: 03/15/2018] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The purpose of this study was to determine how 2 methods known to improve naming impairment in aphasia (i.e., retrieval practice and errorless learning) affect lexical access. We hypothesized that instances of naming during retrieval practice use and strengthen item-specific connections in each of 2 stages of lexical access: Stage 1, meaning-to-word connections, and Stage 2, word-to-phonology connections. In contrast, errorless learning prioritizes opportunities for repeating words, which we expect to primarily strengthen item-specific connections in Stage 2 because repetition circumvents the need for semantically driven word retrieval. Method We tested the outcomes of retrieval practice versus errorless learning training for items that were selected because the naming errors they elicited suggested weakened connections at Stage 1 or at Stage 2 of lexical access for each of 10 individuals with chronic aphasia. Each participant's Stage 1 items and Stage 2 items were divided evenly between the 2 training conditions. Naming tests were administered 1 day and 1 week after training to assess retention of training gains. We also examined whether the participants' pretraining naming error profiles were associated with the relative efficacy of retrieval practice versus errorless learning. Results The posttraining naming tests showed an advantage of retrieval practice over errorless learning for Stage 1 items and an advantage of errorless learning over retrieval practice for Stage 2 items. In addition, greater percentages of phonological error naming responses prior to training were associated with greater posttraining accuracy in the errorless learning condition relative to the retrieval practice condition. Conclusions The findings suggest that the advantage of retrieval practice for naming impairment in aphasia largely results from greater strengthening of practiced semantic-lexical connections compared with errorless learning, which prioritizes repetition and, therefore, mainly confers strengthening of practiced lexical-phonological connections. Understanding how specific training conditions improve naming can help predict the relative efficacy of each method for individuals with aphasia.
Collapse
|
104
|
Betts HN, Gilbert RA, Cai ZG, Okedara ZB, Rodd JM. Retuning of lexical-semantic representations: Repetition and spacing effects in word-meaning priming. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2018; 44:1130-1150. [PMID: 29283607 PMCID: PMC6012009 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000507] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2017] [Revised: 09/27/2017] [Accepted: 09/27/2017] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Current models of word-meaning access typically assume that lexical-semantic representations of ambiguous words (e.g., 'bark of the dog/tree') reach a relatively stable state in adulthood, with only the relative frequencies of meanings and immediate sentence context determining meaning preference. However, recent experience also affects interpretation: recently encountered word-meanings become more readily available (Rodd et al., 2016, 2013). Here, 3 experiments investigated how multiple encounters with word-meanings influence the subsequent interpretation of these ambiguous words. Participants heard ambiguous words contextually-disambiguated towards a particular meaning and, after a 20- to 30-min delay, interpretations of the words were tested in isolation. We replicate the finding that 1 encounter with an ambiguous word biased the later interpretation of this word towards the primed meaning for both subordinate (Experiments 1, 2, 3) and dominant meanings (Experiment 1). In addition, for the first time, we show cumulative effects of multiple repetitions of both the same and different meanings. The effect of a single subordinate exposure persisted after a subsequent encounter with the dominant meaning, compared to a dominant exposure alone (Experiment 1). Furthermore, 3 subordinate word-meaning repetitions provided an additional boost to priming compared to 1, although only when their presentation was spaced (Experiments 2, 3); massed repetitions provided no such boost (Experiments 1, 3). These findings indicate that comprehension is guided by the collective effect of multiple recently activated meanings and that the spacing of these activations is key to producing lasting updates to the lexical-semantic network. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah N Betts
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
| | | | - Zhenguang G Cai
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
| | - Zainab B Okedara
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
| | - Jennifer M Rodd
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
| |
Collapse
|
105
|
Schuchard J, Middleton EL. Word repetition and retrieval practice effects in aphasia: Evidence for use-dependent learning in lexical access. Cogn Neuropsychol 2018; 35:271-287. [PMID: 29724159 PMCID: PMC6089641 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1461615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
This study tested the hypothesis that a use-dependent learning mechanism operates at each of two stages of lexical access: retrieval of a word from semantics ("Stage 1"), followed by retrieval of the word's constituent phonemes ("Stage 2"). Two participants with aphasia were selected due to their contrasting types of naming impairment (Stage 1 versus Stage 2 difficulty). For each participant, items were assigned to naming training that involved retrieval practice (retrieval of the name from semantics) or repetition practice (hear the name and orally repeat it). Naming tests were administered one day and one week after training. The results supported the predicted training effects: (a) Because successful naming via retrieval practice requires both Stage 1 and Stage 2, this technique uses and strengthens item-specific connections in both stages. (b) Because word repetition circumvents semantically driven retrieval, this technique primarily uses and strengthens item-specific connections in Stage 2.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Schuchard
- a Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute , Elkins Park , PA , USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
106
|
Macizo P, Álvarez A. Do we access meaning when we name Arabic digits? Electrophysiological evidence. Br J Psychol 2018; 109:879-896. [PMID: 29893048 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
In this study, we evaluated whether the naming of Arabic digits required access to semantic information. Participants named pictures and Arabic digits blocked by category or intermixed with exemplars of other categories while behavioural and electrophysiological measures were gathered. Pictures were named slower and Arabic digits faster in the blocked context relative to the mixed context. Around 350-450 ms after the presentation of pictures and Arabic digits, brain waves were more positive in anterior regions and more negative in posterior regions when the blocked context was compared with the mixed context. The pattern of electrophysiological results suggests that pictures and Arabic digits are both processed semantically and they are subject to repetition effects during the naming task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pedro Macizo
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC, Spain), University of Granada, Spain
| | - Alejandro Álvarez
- Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC, Spain), University of Granada, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
107
|
Nozari N, Hepner CR. To select or to wait? The importance of criterion setting in debates of competitive lexical selection. Cogn Neuropsychol 2018; 36:193-207. [PMID: 29869940 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1476335] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
Competitive accounts of lexical selection propose that the activation of competitors slows down the selection of the target. Non-competitive accounts, on the other hand, posit that target response latencies are independent of the activation of competing items. In this paper, we propose a signal detection framework for lexical selection and show how a flexible selection criterion affects claims of competitive selection. Specifically, we review evidence from neurotypical and brain-damaged speakers and demonstrate that task goals and the state of the production system determine whether a competitive or a non-competitive selection profile arises. We end by arguing that there is conclusive evidence for a flexible criterion in lexical selection, and that integrating criterion shifts into models of language production is critical for evaluating theoretical claims regarding (non-)competitive selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nazbanou Nozari
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA.,Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
108
|
Oppenheim G, Wu YJ, Thierry G. Found in Translation: Late Bilinguals Do Automatically Activate Their Native Language When They Are Not Using It. Cogn Sci 2018; 42:1700-1713. [PMID: 29802646 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12618] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2017] [Revised: 03/12/2018] [Accepted: 03/14/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In their paper "Do Bilinguals Automatically Activate Their Native Language When They Are Not Using it?", Costa, Pannunzi, Deco, and Pickering (Cognitive Science, 2017) proposed a reinterpretation of Thierry and Wu's (2004, 2007) finding of native language-based (Chinese, L1) ERP effects when they tested Chinese-English late bilinguals exclusively in their second language (English, L2). Using simulations in a six-node Hebbian learning model (three L1 nodes, three L2 nodes), Costa et al. suggested that form overlaps in L1 between otherwise unrelated words create a persistent relationship between their L2 translations. In this scenario, words in the nascent L2 lexicon overlapping in their L1 translations would become linked during learning because of the form overlap in L1; once the L2 words are learned, the direct link between them would be sufficient to generate robust, apparently "L1-mediated" priming without requiring any activation of L1 translations. Costa et al. contend that links between L2 words remain beyond the learning phase, even after links to L1 representations have been severed, and thus that their model affords an alternative account to (but not a rebuttal of) Thierry and Wu's claim of language non-selective activation-or automatic activation of translation equivalents-in late bilinguals. In this response, we build on Costa et al.'s original simulation code, showing that it can only reproduce L1-independent priming when implementing the L1 disconnection in their particular way. By contrast, when severing inter-language connections bidirectionally, their model fails to retain any sizeable influence of L1 form overlap on L2 activations. The model is not the theory, however, and we discuss several issues that would need to be addressed in further attempts to model language non-selective activation in late bilinguals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gary Oppenheim
- School of Psychology, Bangor University
- Department of Psychology, Rice University
| | - Yan Jing Wu
- Faculty of Foreign Languages, Ningbo University
| | - Guillaume Thierry
- School of Psychology, Bangor University
- Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Bangor University
| |
Collapse
|
109
|
Python G, Fargier R, Laganaro M. When Wine and Apple Both Help the Production of Grapes: ERP Evidence for Post-lexical Semantic Facilitation in Picture Naming. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:136. [PMID: 29692716 PMCID: PMC5902702 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2017] [Accepted: 03/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Producing a word in referential naming requires to select the right word in our mental lexicon among co-activated semantically related words. The mechanisms underlying semantic context effects during speech planning are still controversial, particularly for semantic facilitation which investigation remains under-represented in contrast to the plethora of studies dealing with interference. Our aim is to study the time-course of semantic facilitation in picture naming, using a picture-word "interference" paradigm and event-related potentials (ERPs). Methods: We compared two different types of semantic relationships, associative and categorical, in a single word priming and a double word priming paradigm. The primes were presented visually with a long negative Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA), which is expected to cause facilitation. Results: Shorter naming latencies were observed after both associative and categorical primes, as compared to unrelated primes, and even shorter latencies after two primes. Electrophysiological results showed relatively late modulations of waveform amplitudes for both types of primes (beginning ~330 ms post picture onset with a single prime and ~275 ms post picture onset with two primes), corresponding to a shift in latency of similar topographic maps across conditions. Conclusion: The present results are in favor of a post-lexical locus of semantic facilitation for associative and categorical priming in picture naming and confirm that semantic facilitation is as relevant as semantic interference to inform on word production. The post-lexical locus argued here might be related to self-monitoting or/and to modulations at the level of word-form planning, without excluding the participation of strategic processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Grégoire Python
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Raphaël Fargier
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Marina Laganaro
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
110
|
Kleinman D, Gollan TH. Inhibition accumulates over time at multiple processing levels in bilingual language control. Cognition 2018; 173:115-132. [PMID: 29405945 PMCID: PMC5812452 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2017] [Revised: 12/24/2017] [Accepted: 01/18/2018] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
It is commonly assumed that bilinguals enable production in their nondominant language by inhibiting their dominant language temporarily, fully lifting inhibition to switch back. In a re-analysis of data from 416 Spanish-English bilinguals who repeatedly named a small set of pictures while switching languages in response to cues, we separated trials into different types that revealed three cumulative effects. Bilinguals named each picture (a) faster for every time they had previously named that same picture in the same language, an asymmetric repetition priming effect that was greater in their nondominant language, and (b) more slowly for every time they had previously named that same picture in the other language, an effect that was equivalent across languages and implies symmetric lateral inhibition between translation equivalents. Additionally, (c) bilinguals named pictures in the dominant language more slowly for every time they had previously named unrelated pictures in the nondominant language, exhibiting asymmetric language-wide global inhibition. These mechanisms dynamically alter the balances of activation between languages and between lemmas, providing evidence for an oft-assumed but seldom demonstrated key mechanism of bilingual control (competition between translations), resolving the mystery of why reversed language dominance sometimes emerges (the combined forces of asymmetrical effects emerge over time in mixed-language blocks), and also explaining other longer-lasting effects (block order). Key signatures of bilingual control can depend on seemingly trivial methodological details (e.g., the number of trials in a block) because inhibition is applied cumulatively at both local and global levels, persisting long after each individual act of selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Kleinman
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA.
| | - Tamar H Gollan
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, USA
| |
Collapse
|
111
|
Freund M, Nozari N. Is adaptive control in language production mediated by learning? Cognition 2018; 176:107-130. [PMID: 29550688 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2017] [Revised: 03/06/2018] [Accepted: 03/07/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Recent work using the Picture Word Interference (PWI) paradigm has revealed that language production, similar to non-verbal tasks, shows a robust Congruency Sequence Effect (CSE), defined as a decreased congruency effect following incongruent trials. Although CSE is considered an index of adaptive control, its mechanism is debated. In two experiments, we tested the predictions of a learning model of adaptive control in production, using a task-switching paradigm fully balanced to evaluate CSE on a PWI trial as a function of the congruency of a 2-back PWI trial (within-task CSE), as well as a 1-back trial belonging to a different task (cross-task CSE). The second task was a visuospatial task with congruent and incongruent trials in Experiment 1, and a self-paced reading task with ambiguous and unambiguous sentences in Experiment 2 that imposed a gap between the two PWI trials twice as long of that in Experiment 1. A learning model posits that CSE is the result of changes to the connection weights between task-specific representations and a control center, which leads to two predictions in our paradigm: (a) a robust within-task CSE unaffected by the intervening trial and the gap duration, and (b) an absent or reversed cross-task CSE. These predictions were contrasted with two versions of an activation model of CSE. In accord with the predictions of the learning model, we found robust within-task CSE in PWI in both Experiments with a comparable effect size. Similarly, evidence of within-task CSE was also found in the visuospatial and sentence reading tasks. On the other hand, examination of cross-task CSE from PWI to the other tasks and vice versa revealed either absent or reversed CSE. Collectively, these results support a learning model of adaptive control in language production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Freund
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University, 1629 Thames Street, Suite 350, Baltimore, MD 21231, USA
| | - Nazbanou Nozari
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University, 1629 Thames Street, Suite 350, Baltimore, MD 21231, USA; Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, 1629 Thames Street, Suite 350, Baltimore, MD 21231, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
112
|
Roelofs A. A unified computational account of cumulative semantic, semantic blocking, and semantic distractor effects in picture naming. Cognition 2018; 172:59-72. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2017] [Revised: 11/29/2017] [Accepted: 12/04/2017] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
113
|
Hubbard NA, Weaver TP, Turner MP, Rypma B. Re-examination of "release-from-PI" phenomena: recall accuracy does not recover after a semantic switch. Memory 2018; 26:1191-1205. [PMID: 29376767 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1428349] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Recall accuracy decreases over successive memory trials using similar memoranda. This effect reflects proactive interference (PI) - the tendency for previously studied information to reduce recall of new information. However, recall improves if memoranda for a subsequent trial are semantically dissimilar from the previous trials. This improvement is thought to reflect a release from PI. We tested whether PI is reduced or released from the semantic category for which it had been induced by employing paradigms which featured inducement, semantic switch, and then return-to-original category epochs. Two experiments confirmed that PI was not released after various semantic switch trials (effects from d = -0.93 to -1.6). Combined analyses from both studies demonstrated that the number of intervening new category trials did not reduce or release PI. In fact, in all conditions recall accuracy decreased, demonstrating that PI is maintained and can increase after the new category trials. The release-from-PI account cannot accommodate these broader dynamics of PI. This account is also incongruent with evidence and theory from cognitive psychology, linguistics, and neuroscience. We propose a reintroduction-of-PI account which explains these broader PI dynamics and is consistent with the wider psychological and neurosciences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas A Hubbard
- a McGovern Institute for Brain Research , Massachusetts Institute of Technology , Cambridge , MA , USA
| | - Travis P Weaver
- b Department of Psychology , Vanderbilt University , Nashville , TN , USA
| | - Monroe P Turner
- c School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences , University of Texas at Dallas , Richardson , TX , USA
| | - Bart Rypma
- c School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences , University of Texas at Dallas , Richardson , TX , USA.,d Department of Psychiatry , University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center , Dallas , TX , USA
| |
Collapse
|
114
|
Canini M, Della Rosa PA, Catricalà E, Strijkers K, Branzi FM, Costa A, Abutalebi J. Semantic interference and its control: A functional neuroimaging and connectivity study. Hum Brain Mapp 2018; 37:4179-4196. [PMID: 27355179 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23304] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2015] [Revised: 06/20/2016] [Accepted: 06/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
During picture naming, the ease with which humans generate words is dependent upon the context in which they are named. For instances, naming previously presented items results in facilitation. Instead, naming a picture semantically related to previous items displays persistent interference effects (i.e., cumulative semantic interference, CSI). The neural correlates of CSI are still unclear and it is a matter of debate whether semantic control, or cognitive control more in general, is necessary for the resolution of CSI. We carried out an event-related fMRI experiment to assess the neural underpinnings of the CSI effect and the involvement and nature of semantic control. Both left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and the left caudate nucleus (LCN) showed a linear increase of BOLD response positively associated with the consecutive number of presentations of semantically related pictures independently of task-load. The generalized psychophysiological interaction analysis showed that LIFG demonstrated a quantitative neural connectivity difference with the left supramarginal and angular gyri for increases of task-load and with the fusiform gyri for linear CSI increases. Furthermore, seed-to-voxel functional connectivity showed that LIFG activity coupled with different regions involved in cognitive control and lexicosemantic processing when semantic interference was elicited to a minimum or maximum degree. Our results are consistent with the lexical-competitive nature of the CSI effect, and we provide novel evidence that semantic control lies upon a more general cognitive control network (i.e., LIFG and LCN) responsible for resolving interference between competing semantically related items through connectivity with different brain areas in order to guarantee the correct response. Hum Brain Mapp 37:4179-4196, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matteo Canini
- Faculty of Psychology, San Raffaele University & San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milano, Italy
| | | | | | | | - Francesca Martina Branzi
- Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom
| | - Albert Costa
- Universitat De Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona & ICREA, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Jubin Abutalebi
- Faculty of Psychology, San Raffaele University & San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milano, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
115
|
Abstract
Through computational modelling of language behaviour in both healthy and brain-damaged individuals, Anders, Riès, van Maanen and Alario ( in press ) propose that to facilitate word selection when alternatives compete for production, the left lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) changes the threshold for word selection. Because selecting a word during multiword speech involves resolving competition from target-related competitors as well as words produced in the past and future, a word-selection deficit caused by damage to the left PFC may result in difficulties in producing multiword speech. I review evidence in support of this hypothesis and discuss alternative explanations. Future work should extend Anders and colleagues' conclusions to understand whether a deficit in word selection when producing single words accounts for failures to communicate via multiword speech.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tatiana T Schnur
- a Department of Neurosurgery , Baylor College of Medicine , Houston , USA
| |
Collapse
|
116
|
de Zubicaray GI, McLean M, Oppermann F, Hegarty A, McMahon K, Jescheniak JD. The shape of things to come in speech production: Visual form interference during lexical access. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2018; 71:1921-1938. [PMID: 28805133 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1367018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Naming a picture is slower in categorically related compared with unrelated contexts, an effect termed semantic interference. This effect has informed the development of all contemporary models of lexical access in speech production. However, category members typically share visual features, so semantic interference might in part reflect this confound. Surprisingly, little work has addressed this issue, and the relative absence of evidence for visual form interference has been proposed to be problematic for production models implementing competitive lexical selection mechanisms. In a series of five experiments using two different naming paradigms, we demonstrate a reliable visual form interference effect in the absence of a category relation and show the effect is more likely to originate during lexical or later response selection than during perceptual/conceptual processing. We conclude visual form interference in naming is a significant complicating factor for studies of semantic interference effects and discuss the implications for current accounts of lexical access in spoken word production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Greig I de Zubicaray
- 1 Faculty of Health, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Mia McLean
- 2 School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Frank Oppermann
- 3 Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Aidan Hegarty
- 2 School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Katie McMahon
- 4 Centre for Advanced Imaging, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
117
|
Nozari N. How Special Is Language Production? Perspectives From Monitoring and Control. PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/bs.plm.2018.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
|
118
|
Incremental learning of perceptual and conceptual representations and the puzzle of neural repetition suppression. Psychon Bull Rev 2017; 23:1055-71. [PMID: 27294423 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0855-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Incremental learning models of long-term perceptual and conceptual knowledge hold that neural representations are gradually acquired over many individual experiences via Hebbian-like activity-dependent synaptic plasticity across cortical connections of the brain. In such models, variation in task relevance of information, anatomic constraints, and the statistics of sensory inputs and motor outputs lead to qualitative alterations in the nature of representations that are acquired. Here, the proposal that behavioral repetition priming and neural repetition suppression effects are empirical markers of incremental learning in the cortex is discussed, and research results that both support and challenge this position are reviewed. Discussion is focused on a recent fMRI-adaptation study from our laboratory that shows decoupling of experience-dependent changes in neural tuning, priming, and repetition suppression, with representational changes that appear to work counter to the explicit task demands. Finally, critical experiments that may help to clarify and resolve current challenges are outlined.
Collapse
|
119
|
Kuperberg GR, Delaney-Busch N, Fanucci K, Blackford T. Priming production: Neural evidence for enhanced automatic semantic activity preceding language production in schizophrenia. NEUROIMAGE-CLINICAL 2017; 18:74-85. [PMID: 29387525 PMCID: PMC5789757 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2017] [Revised: 11/27/2017] [Accepted: 12/19/2017] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Introduction Lexico-semantic disturbances are considered central to schizophrenia. Clinically, their clearest manifestation is in language production. However, most studies probing their underlying mechanisms have used comprehension or categorization tasks. Here, we probed automatic semantic activity prior to language production in schizophrenia using event-related potentials (ERPs). Methods 19 people with schizophrenia and 16 demographically-matched healthy controls named target pictures that were very quickly preceded by masked prime words. To probe automatic semantic activity prior to production, we measured the N400 ERP component evoked by these targets. To determine the origin of any automatic semantic abnormalities, we manipulated the type of relationship between prime and target such that they overlapped in (a) their semantic features (semantically related, e.g. "cake" preceding a < picture of a pie >, (b) their initial phonemes (phonemically related, e.g. "stomach" preceding a < picture of a starfish >), or (c) both their semantic features and their orthographic/phonological word form (identity related, e.g. "socks" preceding a < picture of socks >). For each of these three types of relationship, the same targets were paired with unrelated prime words (counterbalanced across lists). We contrasted ERPs and naming times to each type of related target with its corresponding unrelated target. Results People with schizophrenia showed abnormal N400 modulation prior to naming identity related (versus unrelated) targets: whereas healthy control participants produced a smaller amplitude N400 to identity related than unrelated targets, patients showed the opposite pattern, producing a larger N400 to identity related than unrelated targets. This abnormality was specific to the identity related targets. Just like healthy control participants, people with schizophrenia produced a smaller N400 to semantically related than to unrelated targets, and showed no difference in the N400 evoked by phonemically related and unrelated targets. There were no differences between the two groups in the pattern of naming times across conditions. Conclusion People with schizophrenia can show abnormal neural activity associated with automatic semantic processing prior to language production. The specificity of this abnormality to the identity related targets suggests that that, rather than arising from abnormalities of either semantic features or lexical form alone, it may stem from disruptions of mappings (connections) between the meaning of words and their form.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gina R Kuperberg
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, United States; Department of Psychiatry and the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, United States.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
120
|
tDCS effects on word production: Limited by design? Comment on Westwood et al. (2017). Cortex 2017; 96:137-142. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.06.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2017] [Accepted: 06/14/2017] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
121
|
Python G, Fargier R, Laganaro M. ERP evidence of distinct processes underlying semantic facilitation and interference in word production. Cortex 2017; 99:1-12. [PMID: 29121484 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2016] [Revised: 06/13/2017] [Accepted: 09/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
In everyday conversations, we take advantage of lexical-semantic contexts to facilitate speech production, but at the same time, we also have to reduce interference and inhibit semantic competitors. The blocked cyclic naming paradigm (BCNP) has been used to investigate such context effects. Typical results on production latencies showed semantic facilitation (or no effect) during the first presentation cycle, and interference emerging in subsequent cycles. Even if semantic contexts might be just as facilitative as interfering, previous BCNP studies focused on interference, which was interpreted as reflecting lemma selection and self-monitoring processes. Facilitation in the first cycle was rarely considered/analysed, although it potentially informs on word production to the same extent as interference. Here we contrasted the event-related potential (ERP) signatures of both semantic facilitation and interference in a BCNP. ERPs differed between homogeneous and heterogeneous blocks from about 365 msec post picture onset in the first cycle (facilitation) and in an earlier time-window (270 msec post picture onset) in the third cycle (interference). Three different analyses of the ERPs converge towards distinct processes underlying semantic facilitation and interference (post-lexical vs lexical respectively). The loci of semantic facilitation and interference are interpreted in the context of different theoretical frameworks of language production: the post-lexical locus of semantic facilitation involves interactive phonological-semantic processes and/or self-monitoring, whereas the lexical locus of semantic interference is in line with selection through increased lexical competition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Grégoire Python
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Switzerland; Neurorehabilitation Unit, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, CHUV, Lausanne, Switzerland.
| | - Raphaël Fargier
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Marina Laganaro
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
122
|
Having a task partner affects lexical retrieval: Spoken word production in shared task settings. Cognition 2017; 166:94-106. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2016] [Revised: 05/12/2017] [Accepted: 05/16/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
123
|
Harmon Z, Kapatsinski V. Putting old tools to novel uses: The role of form accessibility in semantic extension. Cogn Psychol 2017; 98:22-44. [PMID: 28830015 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2017] [Revised: 08/06/2017] [Accepted: 08/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
An increase in frequency of a form has been argued to result in semantic extension (Bybee, 2003; Zipf, 1949). Yet, research on the acquisition of lexical semantics suggests that a form that frequently co-occurs with a meaning gets restricted to that meaning (Xu & Tenenbaum, 2007). The current work reconciles these positions by showing that - through its effect on form accessibility - frequency causes semantic extension in production, while at the same time causing entrenchment in comprehension. Repeatedly experiencing a form paired with a specific meaning makes one more likely to re-use the form to express related meanings, while also increasing one's confidence that the form is never used to express those meanings. Recurrent pathways of semantic change are argued to result from a tug of war between the production-side pressure to reuse easily accessible forms and the comprehension-side confidence that one has seen all possible uses of a frequent form.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zara Harmon
- Department of Linguistics, University of Oregon, United States.
| | | |
Collapse
|
124
|
Janciauskas M, Chang F. Input and Age-Dependent Variation in Second Language Learning: A Connectionist Account. Cogn Sci 2017; 42 Suppl 2:519-554. [PMID: 28744901 PMCID: PMC6001481 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12519] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2016] [Revised: 05/17/2017] [Accepted: 05/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Language learning requires linguistic input, but several studies have found that knowledge of second language (L2) rules does not seem to improve with more language exposure (e.g., Johnson & Newport, 1989). One reason for this is that previous studies did not factor out variation due to the different rules tested. To examine this issue, we reanalyzed grammaticality judgment scores in Flege, Yeni-Komshian, and Liu's (1999) study of L2 learners using rule-related predictors and found that, in addition to the overall drop in performance due to a sensitive period, L2 knowledge increased with years of input. Knowledge of different grammar rules was negatively associated with input frequency of those rules. To better understand these effects, we modeled the results using a connectionist model that was trained using Korean as a first language (L1) and then English as an L2. To explain the sensitive period in L2 learning, the model's learning rate was reduced in an age-related manner. By assigning different learning rates for syntax and lexical learning, we were able to model the difference between early and late L2 learners in input sensitivity. The model's learning mechanism allowed transfer between the L1 and L2, and this helped to explain the differences between different rules in the grammaticality judgment task. This work demonstrates that an L1 model of learning and processing can be adapted to provide an explicit account of how the input and the sensitive period interact in L2 learning.
Collapse
|
125
|
Affiliation(s)
- Gary M. Oppenheim
- Bangor University, Bangor, Gwynedd, UK
- Rice University, Houston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
126
|
Belke E. Effects of lesions to the left lateral prefrontal cortex on task-specific top-down biases and response strategies in blocked-cyclic naming. Cogn Neuropsychol 2017; 34:26-32. [DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1329200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Eva Belke
- Sprachwissenschaftliches Institut, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
127
|
Fink A, Oppenheim GM, Goldrick M. Interactions between Lexical Access and Articulation. LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE 2017; 33:12-24. [PMID: 29399594 PMCID: PMC5793891 DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2017.1348529] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2016] [Accepted: 06/23/2017] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
This study investigates the interaction of lexical access and articulation in spoken word production, examining two dimensions along which theories vary. First, does articulatory variation reflect a fixed plan, or do lexical access-articulatory interactions continue after response initiation? Second, to what extent are interactive mechanisms hard-wired properties of the production system, as opposed to flexible? In two picture-naming experiments, we used semantic neighbor manipulations to induce lexical and conceptual co-activation. Our results provide evidence for multiple sources of interaction, both before and after response initiation. While interactive effects can vary across participants, we do not find strong evidence of variation of effects within individuals, suggesting that these interactions are relatively fixed features of each individual's production system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angela Fink
- Northwestern University, Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University, 2016 Sheridan Rd., Evanston, IL 60626
| | - Gary M Oppenheim
- Bangor University, School of Psychology, Adeilad Brigantia, Penrallt Road, Bangor, Gwynedd LL57 2AS, UK
- Rice University, Department of Psychology, Houston, TX 77251
- University of California San Diego, Center for Research in Language, 9500 Gilman Dr, La Jolla, CA 92037
| | - Matthew Goldrick
- Northwestern University, Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University, 2016 Sheridan Rd., Evanston, IL 60626
| |
Collapse
|
128
|
Spatiotemporal dynamics of word retrieval in speech production revealed by cortical high-frequency band activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2017; 114:E4530-E4538. [PMID: 28533406 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1620669114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Word retrieval is core to language production and relies on complementary processes: the rapid activation of lexical and conceptual representations and word selection, which chooses the correct word among semantically related competitors. Lexical and conceptual activation is measured by semantic priming. In contrast, word selection is indexed by semantic interference and is hampered in semantically homogeneous (HOM) contexts. We examined the spatiotemporal dynamics of these complementary processes in a picture naming task with blocks of semantically heterogeneous (HET) or HOM stimuli. We used electrocorticography data obtained from frontal and temporal cortices, permitting detailed spatiotemporal analysis of word retrieval processes. A semantic interference effect was observed with naming latencies longer in HOM versus HET blocks. Cortical response strength as indexed by high-frequency band (HFB) activity (70-150 Hz) amplitude revealed effects linked to lexical-semantic activation and word selection observed in widespread regions of the cortical mantle. Depending on the subsecond timing and cortical region, HFB indexed semantic interference (i.e., more activity in HOM than HET blocks) or semantic priming effects (i.e., more activity in HET than HOM blocks). These effects overlapped in time and space in the left posterior inferior temporal gyrus and the left prefrontal cortex. The data do not support a modular view of word retrieval in speech production but rather support substantial overlap of lexical-semantic activation and word selection mechanisms in the brain.
Collapse
|
129
|
Facilitation and interference in naming: A consequence of the same learning process? Cognition 2017; 165:61-72. [PMID: 28501548 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2016] [Revised: 04/25/2017] [Accepted: 04/30/2017] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Our success with naming depends on what we have named previously, a phenomenon thought to reflect learning processes. Repeatedly producing the same name facilitates language production (i.e., repetition priming), whereas producing semantically related names hinders subsequent performance (i.e., semantic interference). Semantic interference is found whether naming categorically related items once (continuous naming) or multiple times (blocked cyclic naming). A computational model suggests that the same learning mechanism responsible for facilitation in repetition creates semantic interference in categorical naming (Oppenheim, Dell, & Schwartz, 2010). Accordingly, we tested the predictions that variability in semantic interference is correlated across categorical naming tasks and is caused by learning, as measured by two repetition priming tasks (picture-picture repetition priming, Exp. 1; definition-picture repetition priming, Exp. 2, e.g., Wheeldon & Monsell, 1992). In Experiment 1 (77 subjects) semantic interference and repetition priming effects were robust, but the results revealed no relationship between semantic interference effects across contexts. Critically, learning (picture-picture repetition priming) did not predict semantic interference effects in either task. We replicated these results in Experiment 2 (81 subjects), finding no relationship between semantic interference effects across tasks or between semantic interference effects and learning (definition-picture repetition priming). We conclude that the changes underlying facilitatory and interfering effects inherent to lexical access are the result of distinct learning processes where multiple mechanisms contribute to semantic interference in naming.
Collapse
|
130
|
Anders R, Riès S, Van Maanen L, Alario FX. Lesions to the left lateral prefrontal cortex impair decision threshold adjustment for lexical selection. Cogn Neuropsychol 2017; 34:1-20. [PMID: 28632042 PMCID: PMC8191230 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1282447] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Patients with lesions in the left prefrontal cortex (PFC) have been shown to be impaired in lexical selection, especially when interference between semantically related alternatives is increased. To more deeply investigate which computational mechanisms may be impaired following left PFC damage due to stroke, a psychometric modelling approach is employed in which we assess the cognitive parameters of the patients from an evidence accumulation (sequential information sampling) modelling of their response data. We also compare the results to healthy speakers. Analysis of the cognitive parameters indicates an impairment of the PFC patients to appropriately adjust their decision threshold, in order to handle the increased item difficulty that is introduced by semantic interference. Also, the modelling contributes to other topics in psycholinguistic theory, in which specific effects are observed on the cognitive parameters according to item familiarization, and the opposing effects of priming (lower threshold) and semantic interference (lower drift) which are found to depend on repetition. These results are developed for the blocked-cyclic picture naming paradigm, in which pictures are presented within semantically homogeneous (HOM) or heterogeneous (HET) blocks, and are repeated several times per block. Overall, the results are in agreement with a role of the left PFC in adjusting the decision threshold for lexical selection in language production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Royce Anders
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LPC, Marseille, France
| | - Stéphanie Riès
- Department of Psychology and Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA
- School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
131
|
Interference from related actions in spoken word production: Behavioural and fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia 2017; 96:78-88. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2016] [Revised: 01/09/2017] [Accepted: 01/09/2017] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
132
|
Belke E. The Role of Task-Specific Response Strategies in Blocked-Cyclic Naming. Front Psychol 2017; 7:1955. [PMID: 28119637 PMCID: PMC5221667 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01955] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2016] [Accepted: 11/29/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
In word retrieval, speakers need to select a lexical entry among several co-activated candidates for lexicalization. How a target entry is selected is a matter of ongoing debate. Semantic context effects on naming times, as seen in the blocked-cyclic naming paradigm, are of specific interest to this debate. In the standard version of this paradigm, participants name lists of objects compiled from several repetitions (cycles) of a small set of semantically related objects (homogeneous context) or unrelated objects (heterogeneous context). In the first cycle, participants typically show either no context effect or semantic facilitation. From cycle two onward, they display a stable semantic interference effect that does not increase over cycles. In this review, I demonstrate that the early semantic facilitation effect is only observed consistently in studies that present homogeneous and heterogeneous lists in a blocked fashion. With this design, participants can easily pick up on the categorical relatedness of the items in semantically related contexts and apply this knowledge strategically. In principle, such response strategies can be easily tied in with existing models of lexical selection, but they are incompatible with accounts of semantic context effects that take the semantic facilitation effect in cycle 1 to be a consequence of processes inherent to the lexicalization process. Users of the blocked-cyclic naming paradigm should review their experimental designs carefully regarding potential response strategies. Once these are taken into account, the paradigm can be used to study lexical-semantic encoding in different populations of healthy and also impaired speakers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva Belke
- Sprachwissenschaftliches Institut, Ruhr-Universität Bochum Bochum, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
133
|
Boelens H, La Heij W. The development of semantic blocking in children. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017; 35:310-315. [PMID: 28090654 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2016] [Revised: 11/15/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Pictures are named more slowly in the context of semantically related pictures than in the context of unrelated pictures. This semantic blocking effect has been studied extensively in adult participants, and one study has revealed its presence in 6-year-old children. However, little is known about the development of the effect with age. In this study, a blocked cyclic naming procedure was arranged for 5- to 7-year-old and 10- to 12-year-old children. The semantic blocking effect obtained did not differ in size between the two age groups. This finding is tentatively interpreted as evidence that the semantic blocking effect does not have the same underlying cause as interference effects typically observed in naming tasks involving a distractor stimulus, like the Stroop task. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? The semantic blocking effect has been demonstrated in adults, but little is known about its development in childhood. Age-related changes in performance in children have been used to distinguish various types of inhibitory control. What does this study add? A semantic blocking effect was obtained in 5- to 7-year-old children and - for the first time - in 10- to 12-year-old children. In the two age groups, the effect was equal in size and did not show up in the first cycles of the experiment. The findings are argued to be in line with the distinction unintentional vs. intentional inhibitory control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Harrie Boelens
- Institute of Psychology and Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, The Netherlands
| | - Wido La Heij
- Institute of Psychology and Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
134
|
Hoedemaker RS, Ernst J, Meyer AS, Belke E. Language production in a shared task: Cumulative Semantic Interference from self- and other-produced context words. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2017; 172:55-63. [PMID: 27907879 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.11.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2016] [Revised: 10/17/2016] [Accepted: 11/15/2016] [Indexed: 10/20/2022] Open
Abstract
This study assessed the effects of semantic context in the form of self-produced and other-produced words on subsequent language production. Pairs of participants performed a joint picture naming task, taking turns while naming a continuous series of pictures. In the single-speaker version of this paradigm, naming latencies have been found to increase for successive presentations of exemplars from the same category, a phenomenon known as Cumulative Semantic Interference (CSI). As expected, the joint-naming task showed a within-speaker CSI effect, such that naming latencies increased as a function of the number of category exemplars named previously by the participant (self-produced items). Crucially, we also observed an across-speaker CSI effect, such that naming latencies slowed as a function of the number of category members named by the participant's task partner (other-produced items). The magnitude of the across-speaker CSI effect did not vary as a function of whether or not the listening participant could see the pictures their partner was naming. The observation of across-speaker CSI suggests that the effect originates at the conceptual level of the language system, as proposed by Belke's (2013) Conceptual Accumulation account. Whereas self-produced and other-produced words both resulted in a CSI effect on naming latencies, post-experiment free recall rates were higher for self-produced than other-produced items. Together, these results suggest that both speaking and listening result in implicit learning at the conceptual level of the language system but that these effects are independent of explicit learning as indicated by item recall.
Collapse
|
135
|
Westwood SJ, Olson A, Miall RC, Nappo R, Romani C. Limits to tDCS effects in language: Failures to modulate word production in healthy participants with frontal or temporal tDCS. Cortex 2017; 86:64-82. [PMID: 27912107 PMCID: PMC5264390 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2016] [Revised: 10/19/2016] [Accepted: 10/25/2016] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a method of non-invasive brain stimulation widely used to modulate cognitive functions. Recent studies, however, suggests that effects are unreliable, small and often non-significant at least when stimulation is applied in a single session to healthy individuals. We examined the effects of frontal and temporal lobe anodal tDCS on naming and reading tasks and considered possible interactions with linguistic activation and selection mechanisms as well as possible interactions with item difficulty and participant individual variability. Across four separate experiments (N, Exp 1A = 18; 1B = 20; 1C = 18; 2 = 17), we failed to find any difference between real and sham stimulation. Moreover, we found no evidence of significant effects limited to particular conditions (i.e., those requiring suppression of semantic interference), to a subset of participants or to longer RTs. Our findings sound a cautionary note on using tDCS as a means to modulate cognitive performance. Consistent effects of tDCS may be difficult to demonstrate in healthy participants in reading and naming tasks, and be limited to cases of pathological neurophysiology and/or to the use of learning paradigms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Andrew Olson
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - R Chris Miall
- Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
| | - Raffaele Nappo
- Aston University, Life & Health Sciences, Birmingham, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
136
|
Does segmental overlap help or hurt? Evidence from blocked cyclic naming in spoken and written production. Psychon Bull Rev 2016; 23:500-6. [PMID: 26179140 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0900-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Past research has demonstrated interference effects when words are named in the context of multiple items that share a meaning. This interference has been explained within various incremental learning accounts of word production, which propose that each attempt at mapping semantic features to lexical items induces slight but persistent changes that result in cumulative interference. We examined whether similar interference-generating mechanisms operate during the mapping of lexical items to segments by examining the production of words in the context of others that share segments. Previous research has shown that initial-segment overlap amongst a set of target words produces facilitation, not interference. However, this initial-segment facilitation is likely due to strategic preparation, an external factor that may mask underlying interference. In the present study, we applied a novel manipulation in which the segmental overlap across target items was distributed unpredictably across word positions, in order to reduce strategic response preparation. This manipulation led to interference in both spoken (Exp. 1) and written (Exp. 2) production. We suggest that these findings are consistent with a competitive learning mechanism that applies across stages and modalities of word production.
Collapse
|
137
|
Matushanskaya A, Mädebach A, Müller MM, Jescheniak JD. When Sufficiently Processed, Semantically Related Distractor Pictures Hamper Picture Naming. Exp Psychol 2016; 63:307-317. [DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Abstract. Prominent speech production models view lexical access as a competitive process. According to these models, a semantically related distractor picture should interfere with target picture naming more strongly than an unrelated one. However, several studies failed to obtain such an effect. Here, we demonstrate that semantic interference is obtained, when the distractor picture is sufficiently processed. Participants named one of two pictures presented in close temporal succession, with color cueing the target. Experiment 1 induced the prediction that the target appears first. When this prediction was violated (distractor first), semantic interference was observed. Experiment 2 ruled out that the time available for distractor processing was the driving force. These results show that semantically related distractor pictures interfere with the naming response when they are sufficiently processed. The data thus provide further support for models viewing lexical access as a competitive process.
Collapse
|
138
|
Ruiz A, Togato G, García-Gámez AB, Macizo P. Individual differences in banknote processing: the role of altruism. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1237954] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
139
|
Middleton EL, Schwartz MF, Rawson KA, Traut H, Verkuilen J. Towards a Theory of Learning for Naming Rehabilitation: Retrieval Practice and Spacing Effects. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2016; 59:1111-1122. [PMID: 27716858 PMCID: PMC5345556 DOI: 10.1044/2016_jslhr-l-15-0303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2015] [Accepted: 02/23/2016] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The purpose of this article was to examine how different types of learning experiences affect naming impairment in aphasia. METHODS In 4 people with aphasia with naming impairment, we compared the benefits of naming treatment that emphasized retrieval practice (practice retrieving target names from long-term memory) with errorless learning (repetition training, which preempts retrieval practice) according to different schedules of learning. The design was within subjects. Items were administered for multiple training trials for retrieval practice or repetition in a spaced schedule (an item's trials were separated by multiple unrelated trials) or massed schedule (1 trial intervened between an item's trials). In the spaced condition, we studied 3 magnitudes of spacing to evaluate the impact of effortful retrieval during training on the ultimate benefits conferred by retrieval practice naming treatment. The primary outcome was performance on a retention test of naming after 1 day, with a follow-up test after 1 week. RESULTS Group analyses revealed that retrieval practice outperformed errorless learning, and spaced learning outperformed massed learning at retention test and at follow-up. Increases in spacing in the retrieval practice condition yielded more robust learning of retrieved information. CONCLUSION This study delineates the importance of retrieval practice and spacing for treating naming impairment in aphasia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Hilary Traut
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, PA
| | | |
Collapse
|
140
|
Benitez VL, Yurovsky D, Smith LB. Competition between multiple words for a referent in cross-situational word learning. JOURNAL OF MEMORY AND LANGUAGE 2016; 90:31-48. [PMID: 27087742 PMCID: PMC4831079 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
Three experiments investigated competition between word-object pairings in a cross-situational word-learning paradigm. Adults were presented with One-Word pairings, where a single word labeled a single object, and Two-Word pairings, where two words labeled a single object. In addition to measuring learning of these two pairing types, we measured competition between words that refer to the same object. When the word-object co-occurrences were presented intermixed in training (Experiment 1), we found evidence for direct competition between words that label the same referent. Separating the two words for an object in time eliminated any evidence for this competition (Experiment 2). Experiment 3 demonstrated that adding a linguistic cue to the second label for a referent led to different competition effects between adults who self-reported different language learning histories, suggesting both distinctiveness and language learning history affect competition. Finally, in all experiments, competition effects were unrelated to participants' explicit judgments of learning, suggesting that competition reflects the operating characteristics of implicit learning processes. Together, these results demonstrate that the role of competition between overlapping associations in statistical word-referent learning depends on time, the distinctiveness of word-object pairings, and language learning history.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viridiana L. Benitez
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 E. 10 St., Bloomington, IN, 47405, USA
| | - Daniel Yurovsky
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 E. 10 St., Bloomington, IN, 47405, USA
| | - Linda B. Smith
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 E. 10 St., Bloomington, IN, 47405, USA
| |
Collapse
|
141
|
Llorens A, Dubarry AS, Trébuchon A, Chauvel P, Alario FX, Liégeois-Chauvel C. Contextual modulation of hippocampal activity during picture naming. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2016; 159:92-101. [PMID: 27380274 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.05.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2015] [Revised: 05/17/2016] [Accepted: 05/24/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Picture naming is a standard task used to probe language processes in healthy and impaired speakers. It recruits a broad neural network of language related areas, among which the hippocampus is rarely included. However, the hippocampus could play a role during picture naming, subtending, for example, implicit learning of the links between pictured objects and their names. To test this hypothesis, we recorded hippocampal activity during plain picture naming, without memorization requirement; we further assessed whether this activity was modulated by contextual factors such as repetition priming and semantic interference. Local field potentials recorded from intracerebral electrodes implanted in the healthy hippocampi of epileptic patients revealed a specific and reliable pattern of activity, markedly modulated by repetition priming and semantic context. These results indicate that the hippocampus is recruited during picture naming, presumably in relation to implicit learning, with contextual factors promoting differential hippocampal processes, possibly subtended by different sub-circuitries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Llorens
- Aix Marseille Univ, Inserm, Institut des Neurosciences des Systemes, Marseille, France; Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, UMR7290, LPC, Marseille, France
| | - A-S Dubarry
- Aix Marseille Univ, Inserm, Institut des Neurosciences des Systemes, Marseille, France; Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, UMR7290, LPC, Marseille, France
| | - A Trébuchon
- Aix Marseille Univ, Inserm, Institut des Neurosciences des Systemes, Marseille, France; AP-HM, Neurophysiologie Clinique, Marseille, France
| | - P Chauvel
- Aix Marseille Univ, Inserm, Institut des Neurosciences des Systemes, Marseille, France; AP-HM, Neurophysiologie Clinique, Marseille, France
| | - F-X Alario
- Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, UMR7290, LPC, Marseille, France
| | - C Liégeois-Chauvel
- Aix Marseille Univ, Inserm, Institut des Neurosciences des Systemes, Marseille, France.
| |
Collapse
|
142
|
Cumulative semantic interference for associative relations in language production. Cognition 2016; 152:20-31. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2015] [Revised: 03/09/2016] [Accepted: 03/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
143
|
Britt AE, Ferrara C, Mirman D. Distinct Effects of Lexical and Semantic Competition during Picture Naming in Younger Adults, Older Adults, and People with Aphasia. Front Psychol 2016; 7:813. [PMID: 27458393 PMCID: PMC4937813 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00813] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2016] [Accepted: 05/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Producing a word requires selecting among a set of similar alternatives. When many semantically related items become activated, the difficulty of the selection process is increased. Experiment 1 tested naming of items with either multiple synonymous labels ("Alternate Names," e.g., gift/present) or closely semantically related but non-equivalent responses ("Near Semantic Neighbors," e.g., jam/jelly). Picture naming was fastest and most accurate for pictures with only one label ("High Name Agreement"), slower and less accurate in the Alternate Names condition, and slowest and least accurate in the Near Semantic Neighbors condition. These results suggest that selection mechanisms in picture naming operate at two distinct levels of processing: selecting between similar but non-equivalent names requires two selection processes (semantic and lexical), whereas selecting among equivalent names only requires one selection at the lexical level. Experiment 2 examined how these selection mechanisms are affected by normal aging and found that older adults had significantly more difficulty in the Near Semantic Neighbors condition, but not in the Alternate Names condition. This suggests that aging affects semantic processing and selection more strongly than it affects lexical selection. Experiment 3 examined the role of the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) in these selection processes by testing individuals with aphasia secondary to stroke lesions that either affected the LIFG or spared it. Surprisingly, there was no interaction between condition and lesion group: the presence of LIFG damage was not associated with substantively worse naming performance for pictures with multiple acceptable labels. These results are not consistent with a simple view of LIFG as the locus of lexical selection and suggest a more nuanced view of the neural basis of lexical and semantic selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Allison E. Britt
- Department of Psychology, Drexel UniversityPhiladelphia, PA, USA
| | - Casey Ferrara
- Moss Rehabilitation Research InstituteElkins Park, PA, USA
| | - Daniel Mirman
- Department of Psychology, Drexel UniversityPhiladelphia, PA, USA
- Moss Rehabilitation Research InstituteElkins Park, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
144
|
Meinzer M, Yetim Ö, McMahon K, de Zubicaray G. Brain mechanisms of semantic interference in spoken word production: An anodal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (atDCS) study. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2016; 157-158:72-80. [PMID: 27180210 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2015] [Revised: 04/01/2016] [Accepted: 04/03/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
When naming pictures, categorically-related compared to unrelated contexts typically slow production. We investigated proposed roles for the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and posterior middle and superior temporal gyri (pMTG/STG) in mediating this semantic interference effect. In a three-way, cross-over, sham-controlled study, we applied online anodal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (atDCS) to LIFG or pMTG/STG while 24 participants performed parallel versions of the blocked cyclic naming paradigm. Significant effects of semantic context and cycle, and interactions of context and cycle, were observed on naming latencies in all three stimulation sessions. Additionally, atDCS over left pMTG/STG facilitated naming in related blocks from the second cycle onward, significantly reducing but not eliminating the interference effect. Applying atDCS over left LIFG likewise reduced the magnitude of interference compared to sham stimulation, although the facilitation was limited to the first few cycles of naming. We interpret these results as indicating semantic interference in picture naming reflects contributions of two complementary mechanisms: a relatively short-lived, top-down mechanism to bias selection and a more persistent lexical-level activation mechanism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcus Meinzer
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Özlem Yetim
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Katie McMahon
- Centre for Advanced Imaging, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Greig de Zubicaray
- Faculty of Health and Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
145
|
Kleinschmidt DF, Jaeger TF. Re-examining selective adaptation: Fatiguing feature detectors, or distributional learning? Psychon Bull Rev 2016; 23:678-91. [PMID: 26438255 PMCID: PMC4821823 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0943-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
When a listener hears many good examples of a /b/ in a row, they are less likely to classify other sounds on, e.g., a /b/-to-/d/ continuum as /b/. This phenomenon is known as selective adaptation and is a well-studied property of speech perception. Traditionally, selective adaptation is seen as a mechanistic property of the speech perception system, and attributed to fatigue in acoustic-phonetic feature detectors. However, recent developments in our understanding of non-linguistic sensory adaptation and higher-level adaptive plasticity in speech perception and language comprehension suggest that it is time to re-visit the phenomenon of selective adaptation. We argue that selective adaptation is better thought of as a computational property of the speech perception system. Drawing on a common thread in recent work on both non-linguistic sensory adaptation and plasticity in language comprehension, we furthermore propose that selective adaptation can be seen as a consequence of distributional learning across multiple levels of representation. This proposal opens up new questions for research on selective adaptation itself, and also suggests that selective adaptation can be an important bridge between work on adaptation in low-level sensory systems and the complicated plasticity of the adult language comprehension system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dave F Kleinschmidt
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA.
| | - T Florian Jaeger
- Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Computer Science, and Linguistics, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
146
|
Harvey DY, Schnur TT. Different Loci of Semantic Interference in Picture Naming vs. Word-Picture Matching Tasks. Front Psychol 2016; 7:710. [PMID: 27242621 PMCID: PMC4865493 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00710] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2014] [Accepted: 04/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Naming pictures and matching words to pictures belonging to the same semantic category impairs performance relative to when stimuli come from different semantic categories (i.e., semantic interference). Despite similar semantic interference phenomena in both picture naming and word-picture matching tasks, the locus of interference has been attributed to different levels of the language system - lexical in naming and semantic in word-picture matching. Although both tasks involve access to shared semantic representations, the extent to which interference originates and/or has its locus at a shared level remains unclear, as these effects are often investigated in isolation. We manipulated semantic context in cyclical picture naming and word-picture matching tasks, and tested whether factors tapping semantic-level (generalization of interference to novel category items) and lexical-level processes (interactions with lexical frequency) affected the magnitude of interference, while also assessing whether interference occurs at a shared processing level(s) (transfer of interference across tasks). We found that semantic interference in naming was sensitive to both semantic- and lexical-level processes (i.e., larger interference for novel vs. old and low- vs. high-frequency stimuli), consistent with a semantically mediated lexical locus. Interference in word-picture matching exhibited stable interference for old and novel stimuli and did not interact with lexical frequency. Further, interference transferred from word-picture matching to naming. Together, these experiments provide evidence to suggest that semantic interference in both tasks originates at a shared processing stage (presumably at the semantic level), but that it exerts its effect at different loci when naming pictures vs. matching words to pictures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Denise Y. Harvey
- Department of Neurology, University of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphia, PA, USA
- Moss Rehabilitation Research InstituteElkins Park, PA, USA
| | - Tatiana T. Schnur
- Baylor College of Medicine, Department of NeurosurgeryHouston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
147
|
Ladányi E, Lukács Á. Lexical conflict resolution in children with specific language impairment. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2016; 61:119-130. [PMID: 27135369 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2015] [Revised: 04/06/2016] [Accepted: 04/06/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The aim of our study is to examine the effect of conflict on naming latencies in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing (TD) children and to explore whether deficits in conflict resolution contribute to lexical problems in SLI. In light of previous results showing difficulties with inhibitory functions in SLI, we expected higher semantic conflict effect in the SLI than in the TD group. To investigate this question 13 children with SLI and 13 age- and gender-matched TD children performed a picture naming task in which the level of conflict was manipulated and naming latencies were measured. Children took longer to name pictures in high conflict conditions than in low conflict conditions. This effect was equally present in the SLI and TD groups. Our results suggest that word production is more effortful for children when conflict resolution is required but children with SLI manage competing lexical representations as efficiently as TD children. This result contradicts studies, which found difficulties with inhibitory functions and is in line with findings of intact inhibitory abilities in children with SLI. Further studies should rule out the possibility that in SLI lower level of conflict resulting from weaker lexical representations masks impairments in inhibition, and investigate the effect of linguistic conflict in other areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Enikő Ladányi
- Paris Descartes University, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, 45 Rue des Saints-Peres, 75006 Paris, France; BME Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Egry József u. 1, H-1111 Budapest, Hungary.
| | - Ágnes Lukács
- BME Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Egry József u. 1, H-1111 Budapest, Hungary.
| |
Collapse
|
148
|
Nozari N, Freund M, Breining B, Rapp B, Gordon B. Cognitive control during selection and repair in word production. LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE 2016; 31:886-903. [PMID: 28133620 PMCID: PMC5268164 DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1157194] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2015] [Accepted: 02/13/2016] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Production of an intended word entails selection processes, in which first the lexical item and then its segments are selected among competitors, as well as processes that covertly or overtly repair dispreferred words. In two experiments, we studied the locus of the control processes involved in selection (selection control) and intercepting errors (post-monitoring control). Selection control was studied by manipulating the overlap (contextual similarity) in either semantics or in segments between two objects that participants repeatedly named. Post-monitoring control was examined by asking participants to reverse, within each block, the name of the two objects that were either semantically- or segmentally-related, thus suppressing a potent, but incorrect, response in favor of an alternative (reversal). Results showed robust costs of both contextual similarity (which increased with the degree of similarity between target and context) and reversal, but the two did not interact with one another. Analysis of individual differences revealed no reliable correlation between the cost of contextual similarity when pairs were semantically- or segmentally-related, suggesting stage-specific selection control processes. On the other hand, the cost of reversal was reliably correlated between semantically- and segmentally-related pairs, implying a different control process that is shared by both stages of production. Collectively, these results support a model in which selection control operates separately at lexical and segmental selection stages, but post-monitoring control operates on the segmentally-encoded outcome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nazbanou Nozari
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University
| | - Michael Freund
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
| | | | - Brenda Rapp
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University
| | - Barry Gordon
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
- Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University
| |
Collapse
|
149
|
Mehta SV, Isaki E. A Modified Semantic Feature Analysis Approach With Two Individuals With Chronic Aphasia. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1044/cicsd_43_s_129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
150
|
Schwartz MF, Middleton EL, Brecher A, Gagliardi M, Garvey K. Does naming accuracy improve through self-monitoring of errors? Neuropsychologia 2016; 84:272-81. [PMID: 26863091 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2015] [Revised: 11/24/2015] [Accepted: 01/21/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
This study examined spontaneous self-monitoring of picture naming in people with aphasia. Of primary interest was whether spontaneous detection or repair of an error constitutes an error signal or other feedback that tunes the production system to the desired outcome. In other words, do acts of monitoring cause adaptive change in the language system? A second possibility, not incompatible with the first, is that monitoring is indicative of an item's representational strength, and strength is a causal factor in language change. Twelve PWA performed a 615-item naming test twice, in separate sessions, without extrinsic feedback. At each timepoint, we scored the first complete response for accuracy and error type and the remainder of the trial for verbalizations consistent with detection (e.g., "no, not that") and successful repair (i.e., correction). Data analysis centered on: (a) how often an item that was misnamed at one timepoint changed to correct at the other timepoint, as a function of monitoring; and (b) how monitoring impacted change scores in the Forward (Time 1 to Time 2) compared to Backward (Time 2 to Time 1) direction. The Strength hypothesis predicts significant effects of monitoring in both directions. The Learning hypothesis predicts greater effects in the Forward direction. These predictions were evaluated for three types of errors--Semantic errors, Phonological errors, and Fragments--using mixed-effects regression modeling with crossed random effects. Support for the Strength hypothesis was found for all three error types. Support for the Learning hypothesis was found for Semantic errors. All effects were due to error repair, not error detection. We discuss the theoretical and clinical implications of these novel findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Adelyn Brecher
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, PA, USA
| | | | - Kelly Garvey
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|