101
|
Hermens F, Walker R. The site of interference in the saccadic Stroop effect. Vision Res 2012; 73:10-22. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.09.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2012] [Revised: 09/18/2012] [Accepted: 09/18/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
102
|
Menger R, Van der Stigchel S, Dijkerman HC. How obstructing is an obstacle? The influence of starting posture on obstacle avoidance. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2012; 141:1-8. [PMID: 22820453 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2011] [Revised: 06/13/2012] [Accepted: 06/17/2012] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
The introduction of non-target objects into a workspace leads to temporal and spatial adjustments of reaching trajectories towards a target. Currently, there are two different explanations for this phenomenon: the non-target objects are considered as either physical obstacles to which we maintain a preferred distance (see Tresilian, 1998) or as distractors that interfere with movement planning (see Tipper, Howard, & Jackson, 1997). These components are difficult to disentangle, however. Our aim was to determine the unique contribution of the avoidance of a physical obstacle to the adjustments of reaching trajectories. In this study, we manipulate the degree of physical obstruction by non-target objects while keeping the a priori visual layout of the workspace more or less constant. This is achieved by placing participants in different starting postures with respect to the orientation of their limb segments. Participants reach towards and grasp target objects with non-targets present in the workspace in a frontal and a lateral starting posture. In the frontal conditions participants showed larger movements away from the non-target on the ipsilateral side of the workspace than in the lateral conditions. The results provide evidence for the interpretation that non-targets influence the movement trajectory partly because they are 'obstructing'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rudmer Menger
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
103
|
Liang WK, Juan CH. Modulation of motor control in saccadic behaviors by TMS over the posterior parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol 2012; 108:741-52. [PMID: 22552188 DOI: 10.1152/jn.01135.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC) has been found to be critical in shaping visual selection and distractor-induced saccade curvature in the context of predictive as well as nonpredictive visual cues by means of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) interference. However, the dynamic details of how distractor-induced saccade curvatures are affected by rPPC TMS have not yet been investigated. This study aimed to elucidate the key dynamic properties that cause saccades to curve away from distractors with different degrees of curvature in various TMS and target predictability conditions. Stochastic optimal feedback control theory was used to model the dynamics of the TMS saccade data. This allowed estimation of torques, which was used to identify the critical dynamic mechanisms producing saccade curvature. The critical mechanisms of distractor-induced saccade curvatures were found to be the motor commands and torques in the transverse direction. When an unpredictable saccade target occurred with rPPC TMS, there was an initial period of greater distractor-induced torque toward the side opposite the distractor in the transverse direction, immediately followed by a relatively long period of recovery torque that brought the deviated trace back toward the target. The results imply that the mechanisms of distractor-induced saccade curvature may be comprised of two mechanisms: the first causing the initial deviation and the second bringing the deviated trace back toward the target. The pattern of the initial torque in the transverse direction revealed the former mechanism. Conversely, the later mechanism could be well explained as a consequence of the control policy in this model. To summarize, rPPC TMS increased the initial torque away from the distractor as well as the recovery torque toward the target.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wei-Kuang Liang
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
| | | |
Collapse
|
104
|
Munneke J, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. Shifting attention within memory representations involves early visual areas. PLoS One 2012; 7:e35528. [PMID: 22558165 PMCID: PMC3338846 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035528] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2011] [Accepted: 03/18/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Prior studies have shown that spatial attention modulates early visual cortex retinotopically, resulting in enhanced processing of external perceptual representations. However, it is not clear whether the same visual areas are modulated when attention is focused on, and shifted within a working memory representation. In the current fMRI study participants were asked to memorize an array containing four stimuli. After a delay, participants were presented with a verbal cue instructing them to actively maintain the location of one of the stimuli in working memory. Additionally, on a number of trials a second verbal cue instructed participants to switch attention to the location of another stimulus within the memorized representation. Results of the study showed that changes in the BOLD pattern closely followed the locus of attention within the working memory representation. A decrease in BOLD-activity (V1–V3) was observed at ROIs coding a memory location when participants switched away from this location, whereas an increase was observed when participants switched towards this location. Continuous increased activity was obtained at the memorized location when participants did not switch. This study shows that shifting attention within memory representations activates the earliest parts of visual cortex (including V1) in a retinotopic fashion. We conclude that even in the absence of visual stimulation, early visual areas support shifting of attention within memorized representations, similar to when attention is shifted in the outside world. The relationship between visual working memory and visual mental imagery is discussed in light of the current findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaap Munneke
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
105
|
Upper-lower visual field asymmetries in oculomotor inhibition of emotional distractors. Vision Res 2012; 62:209-19. [PMID: 22554807 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.04.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2011] [Revised: 04/12/2012] [Accepted: 04/16/2012] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
The present study investigated oculomotor inhibition of emotional faces as indicated by saccade curvatures. In Experiment 1, participants saccaded towards a target that appeared above or below fixation while single facial distractors depicting neutral, happy, and angry expressions appeared in one of the four quadrants of the screen. In Experiment 2, participants selected between two objects on the screen by saccading towards a predefined target, while again single facial emotional distractors were presented in one of the four screen quadrants. In both experiments, saccade trajectories curved most strongly away from angry distractors indicating enhanced attentional capture by angry faces. This effect occurred with upright faces but not with inverted faces. The emotion effect was restricted to targets at the lower vertical meridian. The lower visual field has been argued to be specialized for action in peripersonal space and near vision. The modulation by target location might be attributed to activation of near space representation by saccades toward a lower target, inducing increased vigilance for stimuli of action relevance to protect the peripersonal space from interference.
Collapse
|
106
|
Wang Z, Kruijne W, Theeuwes J. Lateral interactions in the superior colliculus produce saccade deviation in a neural field model. Vision Res 2012; 62:66-74. [PMID: 22503807 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.03.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2011] [Revised: 03/27/2012] [Accepted: 03/28/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Contrary to human intuition, saccades (rapid eye movements) rarely go directly to their intended destination, but instead typically deviate from the optimal track. Previous studies have demonstrated that saccades may deviate toward or away from irrelevant distractors. Deviation toward distractors is generally explained with theories of "population coding", while deviation away from distractors is believed to be caused by top-down inhibition at the distractor location. With a Mexican-hat shaped lateral interaction kernel, we successfully simulated both deviation toward and away from distractors using a neural field model of the superior colliculus (SC). Our findings suggest that top-down inhibition of the SC is not necessary for the generation of saccade deviations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhiguo Wang
- Vrije Universiteit, Vander Boechorststraat 1, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
107
|
|
108
|
Schmidt LJ, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. The presence of threat affects saccade trajectories. VISUAL COGNITION 2012. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2012.658885] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
109
|
Veneri G, Rosini F, Federighi P, Federico A, Rufa A. Evaluating gaze control on a multi-target sequencing task: The distribution of fixations is evidence of exploration optimisation. Comput Biol Med 2012; 42:235-44. [DOI: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2011.11.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2010] [Revised: 10/29/2011] [Accepted: 11/28/2011] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
110
|
Attention spatiale et contrôle saccadique : données comportementales et neurobiologiques en faveur d’une conception motrice du contrôle attentionnel. ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2011. [DOI: 10.4074/s000350331100306x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
111
|
Somatosensory saccades reveal the timing of tactile spatial remapping. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:3046-52. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2011] [Revised: 07/01/2011] [Accepted: 07/08/2011] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
112
|
Wardak C, Olivier E, Duhamel JR. The relationship between spatial attention and saccades in the frontoparietal network of the monkey. Eur J Neurosci 2011; 33:1973-81. [PMID: 21645093 DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07710.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Claire Wardak
- Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive, UMR5229 CNRS - Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Bron Cedex, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
113
|
Coutté A, Faure S, Olivier G. Influence de la préparation d’une atteinte manuelle sur l’orientation initiale de l’attention lors d’une tâche de recherche visuelle. PSYCHOLOGIE FRANCAISE 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2011.01.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
114
|
A review of lateralization of spatial functioning in nonhuman primates. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2011; 67:56-72. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.11.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2010] [Revised: 10/20/2010] [Accepted: 11/01/2010] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
115
|
Weaver MD, Lauwereyns J, Theeuwes J. The effect of semantic information on saccade trajectory deviations. Vision Res 2011; 51:1124-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2010] [Revised: 03/03/2011] [Accepted: 03/04/2011] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
116
|
Ni J, Jiang H, Jin Y, Chen N, Wang J, Wang Z, Luo Y, Ma Y, Hu X. Dissociable modulation of overt visual attention in valence and arousal revealed by topology of scan path. PLoS One 2011; 6:e18262. [PMID: 21494331 PMCID: PMC3071806 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018262] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/17/2010] [Accepted: 03/01/2011] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotional stimuli have evolutionary significance for the survival of organisms; therefore, they are attention-grabbing and are processed preferentially. The neural underpinnings of two principle emotional dimensions in affective space, valence (degree of pleasantness) and arousal (intensity of evoked emotion), have been shown to be dissociable in the olfactory, gustatory and memory systems. However, the separable roles of valence and arousal in scene perception are poorly understood. In this study, we asked how these two emotional dimensions modulate overt visual attention. Twenty-two healthy volunteers freely viewed images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) that were graded for affective levels of valence and arousal (high, medium, and low). Subjects' heads were immobilized and eye movements were recorded by camera to track overt shifts of visual attention. Algebraic graph-based approaches were introduced to model scan paths as weighted undirected path graphs, generating global topology metrics that characterize the algebraic connectivity of scan paths. Our data suggest that human subjects show different scanning patterns to stimuli with different affective ratings. Valence salient stimuli (with neutral arousal) elicited faster and larger shifts of attention, while arousal salient stimuli (with neutral valence) elicited local scanning, dense attention allocation and deep processing. Furthermore, our model revealed that the modulatory effect of valence was linearly related to the valence level, whereas the relation between the modulatory effect and the level of arousal was nonlinear. Hence, visual attention seems to be modulated by mechanisms that are separate for valence and arousal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jianguang Ni
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
- Graduate School of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Huihui Jiang
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
- Graduate School of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Yixiang Jin
- Graduate School of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
- Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Nanhui Chen
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
| | - Jianhong Wang
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
| | - Zhengbo Wang
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
| | - Yuejia Luo
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Yuanye Ma
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Beijing, People's Republic of China
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
- Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
- * E-mail: (XH); (YM)
| | - Xintian Hu
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Beijing, People's Republic of China
- Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan, People's Republic of China
- Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
- * E-mail: (XH); (YM)
| |
Collapse
|
117
|
Hermens F, Sumner P, Walker R. Inhibition of masked primes as revealed by saccade curvature. Vision Res 2011; 50:46-56. [PMID: 19861134 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.10.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2009] [Revised: 10/05/2009] [Accepted: 10/06/2009] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
In masked priming, responses are often speeded when primes are similar to targets ('positive compatibility effect'). However, sometimes similarity of prime and target impairs responses ('negative compatibility effect'). A similar distinction has been found for the curvature of saccade trajectories. Here, we test whether the same inhibition processes are involved in the two phenomena, by directly comparing response times and saccade curvature within the same masked priming paradigm. Interestingly, we found a dissociation between the directions of masked priming and saccade curvature, which could indicate that multiple types of inhibition are involved in the suppression of unwanted responses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frouke Hermens
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
118
|
Veneri G, Federighi P, Rosini F, Federico A, Rufa A. Spike removal through multiscale wavelet and entropy analysis of ocular motor noise: A case study in patients with cerebellar disease. J Neurosci Methods 2011; 196:318-26. [PMID: 21262262 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2010] [Revised: 01/02/2011] [Accepted: 01/04/2011] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
119
|
Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. Selection within visual memory representations activates the oculomotor system. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:1605-10. [PMID: 21315747 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2010] [Revised: 11/09/2010] [Accepted: 12/02/2010] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Humans tend to create and maintain internal representations of the environment that help guiding actions during the everyday activities. Previous studies have shown that the oculomotor system is involved in coding and maintenance of locations in visual-spatial working memory. In these studies selection of the relevant location for maintenance in working memory took place on the screen (selecting the location of a dot presented on the screen). The present study extended these findings by showing that the oculomotor system also codes selection of location from an internal memory representation. Participants first memorized two locations and after a retention interval selected one location for further maintenance. The results show that saccade trajectories deviated away from the ultimately remembered location. Furthermore, selection of the location from the memorized representation produced sustained oculomotor preparation to it. The results show that oculomotor system is very flexible and plays an active role for coding and maintaining information selected within internal memory representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Artem V Belopolsky
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
120
|
Chao CM, Tseng P, Hsu TY, Su JH, Tzeng OJL, Hung DL, Muggleton NG, Juan CH. Predictability of saccadic behaviors is modified by transcranial magnetic stimulation over human posterior parietal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 32:1961-72. [PMID: 21259383 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21162] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2010] [Revised: 08/05/2010] [Accepted: 08/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
Predictability in the visual environment provides a powerful cue for efficient processing of scenes and objects. Recently, studies have suggested that the directionality and magnitude of saccade curvature can be informative as to how the visual system processes predictive information. The present study investigated the role of the right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC) in shaping saccade curvatures in the context of predictive and non-predictive visual cues. We used an orienting paradigm that incorporated manipulation of target location predictability and delivered transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over rPPC. Participants were presented with either an informative or uninformative cue to upcoming target locations. Our results showed that rPPC TMS generally increased saccade latency and saccade error rates. Intriguingly, rPPC TMS increased curvatures away from the distractor only when the target location was unpredictable and decreased saccadic errors towards the distractor. These effects on curvature and accuracy were not present when the target location was predictable. These results dissociate the strong contingency between saccade latency and saccade curvature and also indicate that rPPC plays an important role in allocating and suppressing attention to distractors when the target demands visual disambiguation. Furthermore, the present study suggests that, like the frontal eye fields, rPPC is critically involved in determining saccade curvature and the generation of saccadic behaviors under conditions of differing target predictability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chang-Mao Chao
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
121
|
Wardak C, Denève S, Ben Hamed S. Focused visual attention distorts distance perception away from the attentional locus. Neuropsychologia 2010; 49:535-45. [PMID: 21147135 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2010] [Revised: 12/03/2010] [Accepted: 12/06/2010] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Several lines of evidence show that visual perception is altered at the locus of visual attention: detection is faster, performance better and spatial resolution increased. It is however not known whether attention can affect visual perception further away from its locus. In the present study, we specifically question whether and how visual attention influences spatial perception away from its locus, independently from any saccadic preparation. We use a landmark task in which subjects have to estimate the location of a bisection stimulus relative to two landmark stimuli 15° apart, while fixating one of them. This task is combined with a highly demanding discrimination task performed on one of the two landmarks. This allows us to test for the effect of spatial attention allocation on distance perception, as measured by the subject estimation of the landmarks midpoint. We show that the estimated midpoint is displaced towards the attentional locus, both when attention is instructed on the central landmark or on the peripheral landmark. These results suggest an overrepresentation of space around the attentional locus that can affect perception up to 8° away, and question the existence of an objective spatial representation. They are in line with reports of spatial distortion in hemineglect patients while they strikingly contrast with the spatial compression reported around the time of saccadic execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire Wardak
- Centre de Neuroscience Cognitive, CNRS UMR 5529, 67 Bd Pinel, Université Claude Bernard Lyon I, 69675 Bron Cedex, France
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
122
|
Hannula DE, Althoff RR, Warren DE, Riggs L, Cohen NJ, Ryan JD. Worth a glance: using eye movements to investigate the cognitive neuroscience of memory. Front Hum Neurosci 2010; 4:166. [PMID: 21151363 PMCID: PMC2995997 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00166] [Citation(s) in RCA: 170] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2010] [Accepted: 07/26/2010] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Results of several investigations indicate that eye movements can reveal memory for elements of previous experience. These effects of memory on eye movement behavior can emerge very rapidly, changing the efficiency and even the nature of visual processing without appealing to verbal reports and without requiring conscious recollection. This aspect of eye movement based memory investigations is particularly useful when eye movement methods are used with special populations (e.g., young children, elderly individuals, and patients with severe amnesia), and also permits use of comparable paradigms in animals and humans, helping to bridge different memory literatures and permitting cross-species generalizations. Unique characteristics of eye movement methods have produced findings that challenge long-held views about the nature of memory, its organization in the brain, and its failures in special populations. Recently, eye movement methods have been successfully combined with neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI, single-unit recording, and magnetoencephalography, permitting more sophisticated investigations of memory. Ultimately, combined use of eye-tracking with neuropsychological and neuroimaging methods promises to provide a more comprehensive account of brain-behavior relationships and adheres to the "converging evidence" approach to cognitive neuroscience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Robert R. Althoff
- Department of Psychiatry, University of VermontBurlington, VT, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of VermontBurlington, VT, USA
- Department of Pediatrics, University of VermontBurlington, VT, USA
| | - David E. Warren
- Department of Neurology, University of IowaIowa City, IA, USA
| | - Lily Riggs
- Rotman Research Institute, BaycrestToronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of TorontoToronto, ON, Canada
| | - Neal J. Cohen
- Beckman Institute and Department of Psychology, University of IllinoisUrbana-Champaign, IL, USA
| | - Jennifer D. Ryan
- Rotman Research Institute, BaycrestToronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of TorontoToronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, University of TorontoToronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
123
|
Van der Stigchel S, Nijboer TCW, Bergsma DP, Abegg M, Barton JJS. Anomalous global effects induced by ‘blind’ distractors in visual hemifield defects. Brain Cogn 2010; 74:66-73. [PMID: 20637537 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2009] [Revised: 03/14/2010] [Accepted: 06/21/2010] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- S Van der Stigchel
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
124
|
Theeuwes J. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection: Reply to commentaries. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010. [DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
|
125
|
Oculomotor capture by colour singletons depends on intertrial priming. Vision Res 2010; 50:2116-26. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2009] [Revised: 07/24/2010] [Accepted: 08/02/2010] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
126
|
Deplancke A, Madelain L, Chauvin A, Cardoso-Leite P, Gorea A, Coello Y. Influence of near threshold visual distractors on perceptual detection and reaching movements. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:2249-56. [PMID: 20702742 DOI: 10.1152/jn.01123.2009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Providing evidence against a dissociation between conscious vision for perception and unconscious vision for action, recent studies have suggested that perceptual and motor decisions are based on a unique signal but distinct decisional thresholds. The aim of the present study was to provide a direct test of this assumption in a perceptual-motor dual task involving arm movements. In 300 trials, 10 participants performed speeded pointing movements toward a highly visible target located at 10° from the fixation point and ± 45° from the body midline. The target was preceded by one or two close to threshold distractor(s) (80 ms stimulus onset asynchrony) presented ± 30° according to the target location. After each pointing movement, participants judged whether the distractor was present or not on either side of the target. Results showed a robust reaction time facilitation effect and a deviation toward the distractor when the distractor was both present and consciously perceived (Hit). A small reaction time facilitation was also observed when two distractors were physically present but undetected (double-miss)--this facilitation being highly correlated with the physical contrast of the distractors. These results are compatible with the theory proposing that perceptual and motor decisions are based on a common signal but emerge from a contrast dependent fixed threshold for motor responses and a variable context dependent criterion for perceptual responses. This paper thus extends to arm movement control previous findings related to oculomotor control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Deplancke
- Unité de Recherche en sciences Cognitives et Affectives (URECA, EA 1059), Université Lille Nord de France, France
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
127
|
Oculomotor integration in patients with a pulvinar lesion. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:3497-504. [PMID: 20691714 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2009] [Revised: 07/13/2010] [Accepted: 07/28/2010] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
The pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus, with its connections to visual areas and to frontal and parietal oculomotor cortex, might serve as a nexus for integrating cortical control of voluntary eye movements with reflexive eye movements generated by the superior colliculus. To investigate this hypothesis, we tested five patients with a unilateral lesion of the pulvinar on the oculomotor capture paradigm. In this task, participants have to ignore a distractor item and make a saccade to a target in a visual search display. Results showed that the interference of the distractor was stronger when it was presented contralateral to their lesion compared to when it was presented in the ipsilesional visual field. These findings were confirmed by an additional single case experiment in which we measured saccade trajectory deviations as evoked by a single distractor. These results show that the pulvinar is involved in the successful influence of higher order signals (like our goals and intentions) on the guidance of our eye movements.
Collapse
|
128
|
Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010; 134:299-309. [PMID: 20378092 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2009] [Revised: 03/09/2010] [Accepted: 03/09/2010] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The present paper reviews research that focuses on the dissociation between bottom-up attention and consciousness. In particular, we focus on studies investigating spatial exogenous orienting in the absence of awareness. We discuss studies that use peripheral masked onset cues and studies that use gaze cueing. The results from these studies show that the classic biphasic pattern of facilitation and inhibition, which is characteristic of conscious exogenous cueing can also be obtained with subliminal spatial cues. It is hypothesized that unconscious attentional orienting is mediated by the subcortical retinotectal pathway. Moreover, a possible neural network including superior colliculus, pulvinar and amygdala is suggested as the underlying mechanism.
Collapse
|
129
|
Inhoff AW, Seymour BA, Schad D, Greenberg S. The size and direction of saccadic curvatures during reading. Vision Res 2010; 50:1117-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2009] [Revised: 03/29/2010] [Accepted: 03/30/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
130
|
Van der Stigchel S. Recent advances in the study of saccade trajectory deviations. Vision Res 2010; 50:1619-27. [PMID: 20553739 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2010] [Revised: 05/10/2010] [Accepted: 05/22/2010] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
In recent years, the number of studies that have used deviations of saccade trajectories as a measure has rapidly increased. This review discusses these recent studies and summarizes advances in this field. A division can be made into studies that have used saccade deviations to measure the amount of attention allocated in space and studies that have measured the strength of the activity of a distractor. Saccade deviations have also been used to measure target selection in special populations. Most importantly, recent studies have revealed novel knowledge concerning the spatial tuning and temporal dynamics of target selection in the oculomotor system. Deviations in saccade trajectories have shown to constitute a valuable measure of various processes that control and influence our behavior which can be applied to multiple domains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefan Van der Stigchel
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
131
|
Early oculomotor capture by new onsets driven by the contents of working memory. Vision Res 2010; 50:1590-7. [PMID: 20493900 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2009] [Revised: 05/06/2010] [Accepted: 05/17/2010] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Oculomotor capture can occur automatically in a bottom-up way through the sudden appearance of a new object or in a top-down fashion when a stimulus in the array matches the contents of working memory. However, it is not clear whether or not working memory processing can influence the early stages of oculomotor capture by abrupt onsets. Here we present clear evidence for an early modulation driven by stimulus matches to the contents of working memory in the colour dimension. Interestingly, verbal as well as visual information in working memory influenced the direction of the fastest saccades made in search, saccadic latencies and the curvature of the scan paths made to the search target. This pattern of results arose even though the contents of working memory were detrimental for search, demonstrating an early, automatic top-down mediation of oculomotor onset capture by the contents of working memory.
Collapse
|
132
|
Meeter M, Van der Stigchel S, Theeuwes J. A competitive integration model of exogenous and endogenous eye movements. BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS 2010; 102:271-291. [PMID: 20162429 DOI: 10.1007/s00422-010-0365-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2008] [Accepted: 01/23/2010] [Indexed: 05/28/2023]
Abstract
We present a model of the eye movement system in which the programming of an eye movement is the result of the competitive integration of information in the superior colliculi (SC). This brain area receives input from occipital cortex, the frontal eye fields, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, on the basis of which it computes the location of the next saccadic target. Two critical assumptions in the model are that cortical inputs are not only excitatory, but can also inhibit saccades to specific locations, and that the SC continue to influence the trajectory of a saccade while it is being executed. With these assumptions, we account for many neurophysiological and behavioral findings from eye movement research. Interactions within the saccade map are shown to account for effects of distractors on saccadic reaction time (SRT) and saccade trajectory, including the global effect and oculomotor capture. In addition, the model accounts for express saccades, the gap effect, saccadic reaction times for antisaccades, and recorded responses from neurons in the SC and frontal eye fields in these tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martijn Meeter
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Van Der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
133
|
Al-Aidroos N, Pratt J. Top-down control in time and space: Evidence from saccadic latencies and trajectories. VISUAL COGNITION 2010. [DOI: 10.1080/13506280802456939] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
134
|
Van der Stigchel S, Belopolsky AV, Peters JC, Wijnen JG, Meeter M, Theeuwes J. The limits of top-down control of visual attention. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2009; 132:201-12. [PMID: 19635610 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.07.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2008] [Revised: 06/26/2009] [Accepted: 07/01/2009] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The extent to which spatial selection is driven by the goals of the observer and by the properties of the environment is one of the major issues in the field of visual attention. Here we review recent experimental evidence from behavioral and eye movement studies suggesting that top-down control has temporal and spatial limits. More specifically, we argue that the first feedforward sweep of information is bottom-up, and that top-down control can influence selection only after the sweep is completed. In addition, top-down control can limit spatial selection through adjusting the size of attentional window, an area of visual space which receives priority in information sampling. Finally, we discuss the evidence found using brain imaging techniques for top-down control in an attempt to reconcile it with behavioral findings. We conclude by discussing theoretical implications of these results for the current models of visual selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefan Van der Stigchel
- Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
135
|
The effects of multisensory targets on saccadic trajectory deviations: eliminating age differences. Exp Brain Res 2009; 201:385-92. [PMID: 19851761 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-2045-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2009] [Accepted: 10/05/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The present study had two aims. First, to determine if bimodal audio-visual targets allow for greater inhibition of visual distractors, which in turn may lead to greater saccadic trajectory deviations away from those distractors. Second, to determine if bimodal targets can reduce age differences in the ability to generate deviations away, as older adults tend to benefit more from multisensory integration than younger adults. The results show that bimodal targets produced larger deviations away than unimodal targets, but only when the distractor preceded the target, and this effect was comparable across age groups. Furthermore, in contrast to previous research, older adults in this study showed similar deviations away from distractors to those of younger adults. These findings suggest that age differences in the production of trajectory deviations away are not inevitable and that multisensory integration may be an important means for increasing top-down inhibition of irrelevant distraction.
Collapse
|
136
|
Theeuwes J, Belopolsky A, Olivers CN. Interactions between working memory, attention and eye movements. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2009; 132:106-14. [PMID: 19233340 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 139] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2008] [Revised: 01/22/2009] [Accepted: 01/25/2009] [Indexed: 10/21/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper reviews the recent findings on working memory, attention and eye movements. We discuss the research that shows that many phenomena related to visual attention taking place when selecting relevant information from the environment are similar to processes needed to keep information active in working memory. We discuss new data that show that when retrieving information from working memory, people may allocate visual spatial attention to the empty location in space that used to contain the information that has to be retrieved. Moreover, we show that maintaining a location in working memory not only may involve attention rehearsal, but might also recruit the oculomotor system. Recent findings seem to suggest that remembering a location may involve attention-based rehearsal in higher brain areas, while at the same time there is inhibition of specific motor programs at lower brain areas. We discuss the possibility that working memory functions do not reside at a special area in the brain, but emerge from the selective recruitment of brain areas that are typically involved in spatial attention and motor control.
Collapse
|
137
|
Mulckhuyse M, Van der Stigchel S, Theeuwes J. Early and Late Modulation of Saccade Deviations by Target Distractor Similarity. J Neurophysiol 2009; 102:1451-8. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00068.2009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
In this study, we investigated the time course of oculomotor competition between bottom-up and top-down selection processes using saccade trajectory deviations as a dependent measure. We used a paradigm in which we manipulated saccade latency by offsetting the fixation point at different time points relative to target onset. In experiment 1, observers made a saccade to a filled colored circle while another irrelevant distractor circle was presented. The distractor was either similar (i.e., identical) or dissimilar to the target. Results showed that the strength of saccade deviation was modulated by target distractor similarity for short saccade latencies. To rule out the possibility that the similar distractor affected the saccade trajectory merely because it was identical to the target, the distractor in experiment 2 was a square shape of which only the color was similar or dissimilar to the target. The results showed that deviations for both short and long latencies were modulated by target distractor similarity. When saccade latencies were short, we found less saccade deviation away from a similar than from a dissimilar distractor. When saccade latencies were long, the opposite pattern was found: more saccade deviation away from a similar than from a dissimilar distractor. In contrast to previous findings, our study shows that task-relevant information can already influence the early processes of oculomotor control. We conclude that competition between saccadic goals is subject to two different processes with different time courses: one fast activating process signaling the saliency and task relevance of a location and one slower inhibitory process suppressing that location.
Collapse
|
138
|
Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks. Trends Cogn Sci 2009; 13:360-6. [PMID: 19647475 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 231] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2009] [Revised: 04/12/2009] [Accepted: 04/26/2009] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
139
|
Becker SI, Ansorge U, Horstmann G. Can intertrial priming account for the similarity effect in visual search? Vision Res 2009; 49:1738-56. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2008] [Revised: 03/26/2009] [Accepted: 04/01/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
140
|
Van der Stigchel S, Mulckhuyse M, Theeuwes J. Eye cannot see it: the interference of subliminal distractors on saccade metrics. Vision Res 2009; 49:2104-9. [PMID: 19500616 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2009] [Revised: 05/28/2009] [Accepted: 05/29/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The present study investigated whether subliminal (unconsciously perceived) visual information influences eye movement metrics, like saccade trajectories and endpoints. Participants made eye movements upwards and downwards while a subliminal distractor was presented in the periphery. Results showed that the subliminal distractor interfered with the execution of an eye movement, although the effects were smaller compared to a control experiment in which the distractor was presented supraliminal. Because saccade metrics are mediated by low level brain areas, this indicates that subliminal visual information evokes competition at a very low level in the oculomotor system.
Collapse
|
141
|
Theeuwes J, Van der Stigchel S. Saccade trajectory deviations and inhibition-of-return: Measuring the amount of attentional processing. Vision Res 2009; 49:1307-15. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2007] [Revised: 07/27/2008] [Accepted: 07/30/2008] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
142
|
Cardoso-Leite P, Gorea A. Comparison of Perceptual and Motor Decisions Via Confidence Judgments and Saccade Curvature. J Neurophysiol 2009; 101:2822-36. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.91269.2008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
This study investigated the effects on perceptual and motor decisions of low-contrast distractors, presented 5° on the left and/or the right of the fixation point. Perceptual decisions were assessed with a yes/no (distractor) detection task. Motor decisions were assessed via these distractors' effects on the trajectory of an impending saccade to a distinct imperative stimulus, presented 10° above fixation 50 ms after the distractor(s). Saccade curvature models postulate that distractors activate loci on a motor map that evoke reflexive saccades and that the distractor evoked activity is inhibited to prevent reflexive orienting to the cost of causing a saccade curvature away from the distractor. Depending on whether or not each of these processes depends on perceptual detection, one can predict the relationships between saccades' curvature and perceptual responses (classified as correct rejections, misses, false alarms, and hits). The results show that saccades curve away from distractors only when observers report them to be present. Furthermore, saccade deviation is correlated (on a trial-by-trial basis) with the inferred internal response associated with the perceptual report: the stronger the distractor-evoked perceptual response, the more saccades deviate away from the distractor. Also in contrast with a supersensitive motor system, perceptual sensitivity is systematically higher than the motor sensitivity derived from the distributions of the saccades' curvatures. Finally, when both distractors are present (and straight saccades are expected), the sign of saccades' curvature is correlated with observers' perceptual bias/criterion. Overall the results point to a strong perceptual-motor association.
Collapse
|
143
|
Campbell KL, Al-Aidroos N, Pratt J, Hasher L. Repelling the young and attracting the old: examining age-related differences in saccade trajectory deviations. Psychol Aging 2009; 24:163-8. [PMID: 19290747 DOI: 10.1037/a0014106] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In the present study, the authors examined age-related differences in saccade curvature as older and younger adults looked to an X target that appeared concurrently with an O distractor. They used a fixation gap procedure to introduce variance into the saccadic latencies of both groups. Consistent with earlier findings, younger adults' early onset saccades curved toward the distractor (as the distractor competed with the target for response selection), while late-onset saccades curved away from the distractor (as the distractor location became inhibited over time). In contrast, older adults' saccades gradually decreased in curvature toward the distractor, but at no point along the latency continuum did they show deviations away. These results suggest that while the local inhibitory mechanisms responsible for decreases in curvature toward distractors may be preserved with age, aging may lead to a selective decline in the frontal inhibitory mechanisms responsible for deviations away from distractors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen L Campbell
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
144
|
The spatial and temporal shape of oculomotor inhibition. Vision Res 2009; 49:608-14. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.01.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2008] [Revised: 01/16/2009] [Accepted: 01/19/2009] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
145
|
Walker R, Techawachirakul P, Haggard P. Frontal eye field stimulation modulates the balance of salience between target and distractors. Brain Res 2009; 1270:54-63. [PMID: 19285965 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2008] [Revised: 02/06/2009] [Accepted: 02/27/2009] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Natural scenes generally include several possible objects that can be the target for a shift of gaze and attention. The oculomotor system may select a single target by boosting neural activation representing the target, and also by inhibiting neural activity associated with competing alternatives (distractors). We examine the role of the frontal eye field (FEF) in these processes through the effects of single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) on the distractor-related modulation of saccade trajectories. Participants made voluntary saccades to peripheral locations specified by a central arrow-cue. On some trials, visual distractors appeared remote from the target location. The competing distractor produced a deviation of saccade trajectory, away from the distractor location. Single-pulse TMS stimulation of the right frontal eye field increased this distractor-related deviation compared that observed when stimulation was applied to a control site (vertex). The increase in distractor-related deviation of trajectory, following FEF stimulation, was observed for saccades made in both the left and right visual fields and could not be attributed to an effect of TMS on saccade latency. The enhanced distractor-related deviation following FEF stimulation could reflect increased inhibition of the competing distractor, or reduced salience of the endogenous saccade goal. The results are interpreted in light of neurophysiological evidence that the human FEF is involved in the dynamic interaction between competing stimuli for the selection of a candidate target.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robin Walker
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, Surrey TW20 OEX, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
146
|
Salmon JP, Trappenberg TP. Modeling the integration of expectations in visual search with centre-surround neural fields. Neural Netw 2008; 21:1476-92. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet.2008.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2008] [Revised: 08/07/2008] [Accepted: 09/22/2008] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
147
|
Van der Stigchel S, van Zoest W, Theeuwes J, Barton JJS. The Influence of “Blind” Distractors on Eye Movement Trajectories in Visual Hemifield Defects. J Cogn Neurosci 2008; 20:2025-36. [PMID: 18416675 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
There is evidence that some visual information in blind regions may still be processed in patients with hemifield defects after cerebral lesions (“blindsight”). We tested the hypothesis that, in the absence of retinogeniculostriate processing, residual retinotectal processing may still be detected as modifications of saccades to seen targets by irrelevant distractors in the blind hemifield. Six patients were presented with distractors in the blind and intact portions of their visual field and participants were instructed to make eye movements to targets in the intact field. Eye movements were recorded to determine if blind-field distractors caused deviation in saccadic trajectories. No deviation was found in one patient with an optic chiasm lesion, which affect both retinotectal and retinogeniculostriate pathways. In five patients with lesions of the optic radiations or the striate cortex, the results were mixed, with two of the five patients showing significant deviations of saccadic trajectory away from the “blind” distractor. In a second experiment, two of the five patients were tested with the target and the distractor more closely aligned. Both patients showed a “global effect,” in that saccades deviated toward the distractor, but the effect was stronger in the patient who also showed significant trajectory deviation in the first experiment. Although our study confirms that distractor effects on saccadic trajectory can occur in patients with damage to the retinogeniculostriate visual pathway but preserved retinotectal projections, there remain questions regarding what additional factors are required for these effects to manifest themselves in a given patient.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Wieske van Zoest
- 1Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- 2University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
148
|
Rommelse NNJ, Van der Stigchel S, Sergeant JA. A review on eye movement studies in childhood and adolescent psychiatry. Brain Cogn 2008; 68:391-414. [PMID: 18835079 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 103] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/26/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
The neural substrates of eye movement measures are largely known. Therefore, measurement of eye movements in psychiatric disorders may provide insight into the underlying neuropathology of these disorders. Visually guided saccades, antisaccades, memory guided saccades, and smooth pursuit eye movements will be reviewed in various childhood psychiatric disorders. The four aims of this review are (1) to give a thorough overview of eye movement studies in a wide array of psychiatric disorders occurring during childhood and adolescence (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional deviant disorder and conduct disorder, autism spectrum disorders, reading disorder, childhood-onset schizophrenia, Tourette's syndrome, obsessive compulsive disorder, and anxiety and depression), (2) to discuss the specificity and overlap of eye movement findings across disorders and paradigms, (3) to discuss the developmental aspects of eye movement abnormalities in childhood and adolescence psychiatric disorders, and (4) to present suggestions for future research. In order to make this review of interest to a broad audience, attention will be given to the clinical manifestation of the disorders and the theoretical background of the eye movement paradigms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nanda N J Rommelse
- Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Center, Reinier Postlaan 12, 6525 GC Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
149
|
Differences in distractor-induced deviation between horizontal and vertical saccade trajectories. Neuroreport 2008; 19:251-4. [PMID: 18185118 DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e3282f49b3f] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The present study systematically investigated the influence of a distractor on horizontal and vertical eye movements. Results showed that both horizontal and vertical eye movements deviated away from the distractor but these deviations were stronger for vertical than for horizontal movements. As trajectory deviations away from a distractor are generally attributed to inhibition applied to the distractor, this suggests that this deviation is not only due to differences in activity between the two collicular motor maps, but can also be evoked by local application of inhibitory processes in the same map as the target. Nonetheless, deviations were more dominant for vertical movements which suggests that for these movements more inhibition is applied than for horizontal movements.
Collapse
|
150
|
Abstract
In the present experiment, we investigated whether the memory of a location is affected by the occurrence of an irrelevant visual event. Participants had to memorize the location of a dot. During the retention interval, a task-irrelevant stimulus was presented with abrupt onset somewhere in the visual field. Results showed that the spatial memory representation was affected by the occurrence of the external irrelevant event relative to a control condition in which there was no external event. Specifically, the memorized location was shifted toward the location of the task-irrelevant stimulus. This effect was only present when the onset was close in space to the memory representation. These findings suggest that the "internal" spatial map used for keeping a location in spatial working memory and the "external" spatial map that is affected by exogenous events in the outside world are either the same or tightly linked.
Collapse
|