101
|
A Missense Variant at the Nrxn3 Locus Enhances Empathy Fear in the Mouse. Neuron 2018; 98:588-601.e5. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2017] [Revised: 02/19/2018] [Accepted: 03/22/2018] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
102
|
Betka S, Gould Van Praag C, Paloyelis Y, Bond R, Pfeifer G, Sequeira H, Duka T, Critchley H. Impact of intranasal oxytocin on interoceptive accuracy in alcohol users: an attentional mechanism? Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2018; 13:440-448. [PMID: 29618101 PMCID: PMC5928407 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsy027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2017] [Revised: 02/16/2018] [Accepted: 03/19/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Interoception, i.e. the perception and appraisal of internal bodily signals, is related to the phenomenon of craving, and is reportedly disrupted in alcohol use disorders. The hormone oxytocin influences afferent transmission of bodily signals and, through its potential modulation of craving, is proposed as a possible treatment for alcohol use disorders. However, oxytocin's impact on interoception in alcohol users remains unknown. Healthy alcohol users (n = 32) attended two laboratory sessions to perform tests of interoceptive ability (heartbeat tracking: attending to internal signals and, heartbeat discrimination: integrating internal and external signals) after intranasal administration of oxytocin or placebo. Effects of interoceptive accuracy, oxytocin administration and alcohol intake, were tested using mixed-effects models. On the tracking task, oxytocin reduced interoceptive accuracy, but did not interact with alcohol consumption. On the discrimination task, we found an interaction between oxytocin administration and alcohol intake: Oxytocin, compared with placebo, increased interoceptive accuracy in heavy drinkers, but not in light social drinkers. Our study does not suggest a pure interoceptive impairment in alcohol users but instead potentially highlights reduced flexibility of internal and external attentional resource allocation. Importantly, this impairment seems to be mitigated by oxytocin. This attentional hypothesis needs to be explicitly tested in future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie Betka
- Clinical Imaging Science Centre, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton BN1 9RY, UK
- Psychology Department, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RR, UK
- SCALab, CNRS UMR 9193, University of Lille, Lille 59045, France
| | | | - Yannis Paloyelis
- Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, UK
| | - Rod Bond
- Psychology Department, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RR, UK
| | - Gaby Pfeifer
- Clinical Imaging Science Centre, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton BN1 9RY, UK
| | | | - Theodora Duka
- Psychology Department, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RR, UK
- Sussex Addiction Research and Intervention Centre (SARIC), University of Sussex, BN1 9RR, UK
| | - Hugo Critchley
- Clinical Imaging Science Centre, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton BN1 9RY, UK
- Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, BN1 9QJ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
103
|
Kilsdonk C. Des animaux pour humaniser les soins : éthique des soins et récits de zoothérapie canine auprès de résidents de centres d’hébergement de soins de longue durée. BIOÉTHIQUEONLINE 2018. [DOI: 10.7202/1044263ar] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
En zoothérapie, les gens peuvent à la fois bénéficier d’interactions et de relations avec un humain et un animal. Ce texte présente quelques anecdotes vécues illustrant comment la zoothérapie peut contribuer au bien-être socioaffectif des patients. Ce texte souligne que notre nature sociale rend l’éthique des soins possible pour les soignants et nécessaire pour les patients, tout en faisant un parallèle entre les aptitudes de mes chiens et les miennes. Je plaide aussi pour qu’on ne néglige pas les besoins socioaffectifs des patients.
Collapse
|
104
|
Meyza K, Knapska E. What can rodents teach us about empathy? Curr Opin Psychol 2018; 24:15-20. [PMID: 29609059 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2017] [Revised: 02/08/2018] [Accepted: 03/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
While many consider empathy an exclusively human trait, non-human animals are capable of simple forms of empathy, such as emotional contagion, as well as consolation and helping behavior. Rodent models are particularly useful for describing the neuronal background of these phenomena. They offer the possibility of employing single-cell resolution mapping of the neuronal activity as well as novel techniques for manipulation of in vivo activity, which are currently unavailable in human studies. Here, we review recent developments in the field of rodent empathy research with special emphasis on behavioral paradigms and data on neuronal correlates of emotional contagion. We hope that the use of rodent models will enhance our understanding of social deficits in neuropsychiatric disorders characterized with empathy impairments and the evolutionary continuity of the empathic trait.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ksenia Meyza
- Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology of Polish Academy of Sciences, Poland
| | - Ewelina Knapska
- Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology of Polish Academy of Sciences, Poland.
| |
Collapse
|
105
|
Empathy from infancy to adolescence: An attachment perspective on the development of individual differences. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2017.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
|
106
|
Mony TJ, Hong M, Lee HJ. Empathy Study in Rodent Model of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Psychiatry Investig 2018; 15:104-110. [PMID: 29475229 PMCID: PMC5900397 DOI: 10.30773/pi.2017.06.20] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2017] [Revised: 05/14/2017] [Accepted: 06/20/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
There is a highly cognitive and social context to empathy behavior in human. In various social contexts, rodents also display remarkable affective sensitivity and exhibit primitive forms of empathy similar to human. Therefore, we aimed to elaborate the concept of empathy about various components of empathetic behavior in rodents with the similar contexts of a human. In this review, we highlighted the behavioral paradigm that already examined different aspects of rodent empathetic behavior in response to conspecific distress. Additionally, we summarized homologous brain parts of human and rodents to express the empathetic behavior. Integrating the findings with corresponding experiments in the human will provide a novel insight into therapeutic intervention or expanded experimental approaches for neuropsychiatric disorders like autism spectrum disorders associated with empathetic behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tamanna Jahan Mony
- Department of Pharmacology, School of Medicine, Kangwon National University, Chuncheon, Republic of Korea
| | - Minha Hong
- Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Seonam University, Goyang, Republic of Korea
| | - Hee Jae Lee
- Department of Pharmacology, School of Medicine, Kangwon National University, Chuncheon, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
107
|
Gazzillo F, Gorman B, De Luca E, Faccini F, Bush M, Silberschatz G, Dazzi N. Preliminary Data about the Validation of a Self-Report for the Assessment of Interpersonal Guilt: The Interpersonal Guilt Rating Scales-15s (IGRS-15s). Psychodyn Psychiatry 2018; 46:23-48. [PMID: 29480782 DOI: 10.1521/pdps.2018.46.1.23] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
The aim of this article is to present validation data about a self-report rating scale for the assessment of interpersonal guilt according to Control-Mastery Theory (CMT; Silbershatz, 2005; Weiss, 1993; Weiss, Sampson, & The Mount Zion Psychotherapy Research Group, 1986), the Interpersonal Guilt Rating Scale-15s (IGRS-15s). In order to perform the validation of this tool in an Italian sample we have collected a sample of 645 nonclinical subjects. They had to complete the IGRS-15s, the Scale for the Measurement of the Impending Punishment (SMIP; Caprara et al., 1990), the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-67 (IGQ-67; O'Connor et al., 1997), the Psychological General Well-Being Index (PGWBI; Dupuy, 1984), and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS; Davis, Panksepp, & Normansell, 2003), together with an ad-hoc questionnaire for collecting demographic data, the Socio-Demographical Schedule. We have performed a confirmatory factor analysis to verify if the four-factor solution based on CMT and replicated in previous research (Gazzillo et al., 2017) was confirmed. Then, we checked the retest reliability of IGRS-15s after four weeks in a random subsample of 54 subjects. In order to assess its concurrent and discriminant validity, we calculated the correlations between IGRS-15s assessment and SMIP and IGQ-67. Finally, to test its construct validity, we assessed the relationships between the IGRS-15s and the affective systems using the ANPS and the wellbeing assessed with the PGWBI. The data collected support the retest reliability and the concurrent and discriminant validity of the measure, and we have collected preliminary data about its construct validity. Examples of the possible clinical and research applications of this tool are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Gazzillo
- Researcher, Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, "Sapienza" University of Rome, Italy
| | - Bernard Gorman
- Professor Emeritus, Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies, Adelphi University, Garden City, New York
| | - Emma De Luca
- Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, "Sapienza" University of Rome, Italy
| | - Filippo Faccini
- Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, "Sapienza" University of Rome, Italy
| | - Marshall Bush
- San Francisco Psychotherapy Research Group, San Francisco, CA
| | | | - Nino Dazzi
- Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, "Sapienza" University of Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
108
|
Nakahashi W, Ohtsuki H. Evolution of emotional contagion in group-living animals. J Theor Biol 2017; 440:12-20. [PMID: 29253506 DOI: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2017.12.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2017] [Revised: 12/13/2017] [Accepted: 12/14/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Emotional contagion refers to an instantaneous matching of an emotional state between a subject and an object. It is believed to form one of the bases of empathy and it causes consistent group behavior in many animals. However, how this emotional process relates to group size remains unclear. Individuals with the ability of emotional contagion can instantaneously copy the emotion of another group member and can take relevant behavior driven by this emotion, but this would entail both cost and benefit to them because the behavior can be either appropriate or inappropriate depending on the situation. For example, emotional contagion may help them escape from a predator but sometimes induce mass panic. We theoretically study how these two aspects of emotional contagion affect its evolution in group-living animals. We consider a situation where an environmental cue sometimes indicates a serious event and individuals have to make a decision whether to react to them. We show that, as the group size increases, individuals with the ability of emotional contagion would evolutionarily weaken their sensitivity to environmental cues. We also show that a larger group yields a larger benefit to them through such evolutionary change. However, larger group size prevents the invasion of mutants with the ability of emotional contagion into the population of residents who react to environmental cues independently of other group members. These results provide important suggestions on the evolutionary relationship between emotional contagion and group living.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wataru Nakahashi
- School of Advanced Sciences, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies), Shonan Village, Hayama, Kanagawa 240-0193, Japan.
| | - Hisashi Ohtsuki
- School of Advanced Sciences, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies), Shonan Village, Hayama, Kanagawa 240-0193, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
109
|
Abstract
Empathy is fundamental to human relations, but its neural substrates remain largely unknown. Here we characterize the involvement of oxytocin in the capacity of mice to display emotional state-matching, an empathy-like behavior. When exposed to a familiar conspecific demonstrator in distress, an observer mouse becomes fearful, as indicated by a tendency to freeze and subsequent efforts to escape. Both intranasal oxytocin administration and chemogenetic stimulation of oxytocin neurons render males sensitive to the distress of an unfamiliar mouse. Acute intranasal oxytocin penetrates the brain and enhances cellular activity within the anterior cingulate cortex, whereas chronic administration produces long-term facilitation of observational fear and downregulates oxytocin receptor expression in the amygdala. None of these manipulations affect fear acquired as a result of direct experience with the stressor. Hence, these results implicate oxytocin in observational fear in mice (rather than fear itself) and provide new avenues for examining the neural substrates of empathy. Oxytocin modulates social behaviours in mammals. Here the authors demonstrate that observational fear, a measure of empathy-like behaviour in rodents, is modulated by oxytocin.
Collapse
|
110
|
Laviola G, Zoratto F, Ingiosi D, Carito V, Huzard D, Fiore M, Macrì S. Low empathy-like behaviour in male mice associates with impaired sociability, emotional memory, physiological stress reactivity and variations in neurobiological regulations. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0188907. [PMID: 29200428 PMCID: PMC5714342 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188907] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2017] [Accepted: 11/15/2017] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Deficits in empathy have been proposed to constitute a hallmark of several psychiatric disturbances like conduct disorder, antisocial and narcissistic personality disorders. Limited sensitivity to punishment, shallow or deficient affect and reduced physiological reactivity to environmental stressors have been often reported to co-occur with limited empathy and contribute to the onset of antisocial phenotypes. Empathy in its simplest form (i.e. emotional contagion) is addressed in preclinical models through the evaluation of the social transmission of emotional states: mice exposed to a painful stimulus display a higher response if in the presence of a familiar individual experiencing a higher degree of discomfort, than in isolation. In the present study, we investigated whether a reduction of emotional contagion can be considered a predictor of reduced sociality, sensitivity to punishment and physiological stress reactivity. To this aim, we first evaluated emotional contagion in a group of Balb/cJ mice and then discretised their values in four quartiles. The upper (i.e. Emotional Contagion Prone, ECP) and the lower (i.e. Emotional Contagion Resistant, ECR) quartiles constituted the experimental groups. Our results indicate that mice in the lower quartile are characterized by reduced sociability, impaired memory of negative events and dampened hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical reactivity to external stressors. Furthermore, in the absence of changes in oxytocin receptor density, we show that these mice exhibit elevated concentrations of oxytocin and vasopressin and reduced density of BDNF receptors in behaviourally-relevant brain areas. Thus, not only do present results translate to the preclinical investigation of psychiatric disturbances, but also they can contribute to the study of emotional contagion in terms of its adaptive significance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giovanni Laviola
- Reference Centre for Behavioural Sciences and Mental Health, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Rome, Italy
- * E-mail:
| | - Francesca Zoratto
- Reference Centre for Behavioural Sciences and Mental Health, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Rome, Italy
| | - Danilo Ingiosi
- Reference Centre for Behavioural Sciences and Mental Health, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Rome, Italy
| | - Valentina Carito
- Institute of Cell Biology and Neurobiology, National Research Council of Italy (CNR), Rome, Italy
| | - Damien Huzard
- Laboratory of Behavioural Genetics, Brain Mind Institute, Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Marco Fiore
- Institute of Cell Biology and Neurobiology, National Research Council of Italy (CNR), Rome, Italy
| | - Simone Macrì
- Reference Centre for Behavioural Sciences and Mental Health, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
111
|
Gilbert P. Shame and the vulnerable self in medical contexts: the compassionate solution. MEDICAL HUMANITIES 2017; 43:211-217. [PMID: 29030410 DOI: 10.1136/medhum-2016-011159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/07/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Shame is a powerful experience that plays a vital role in a whole range of aspects of the clinical encounter. Shame experiences can have an impact on our psychological and physiological state and on how we experience ourselves, others and our relationships. The medical encounter is an obvious arena for shame because we are presenting (aspects of) our bodies and minds that can be seen as unattractive and undesirable, diseased, decayed and injured with the various excretions that typically might invite disgust. In contrast, experiences of compassion of acceptance, validation and kindness and can increase approach, openness and preparedness to engage with painful difficult scenarios. While shame is an experience that separates, segregates, marginalises and disengages people, caring and compassion facilitate integration, (re)connection and support. Given the potential opposite impacts of these different types of social experience, this paper will outline their evolutionary origins and compare and contrast them with particular reference to the medical context.
Collapse
|
112
|
Coll MP, Viding E, Rütgen M, Silani G, Lamm C, Catmur C, Bird G. Are we really measuring empathy? Proposal for a new measurement framework. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 83:132-139. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2017] [Revised: 10/09/2017] [Accepted: 10/09/2017] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
|
113
|
Lü YF, Yang Y, Li CL, Wang Y, Li Z, Chen J. The Locus Coeruleus-Norepinephrine System Mediates Empathy for Pain through Selective Up-Regulation of P2X3 Receptor in Dorsal Root Ganglia in Rats. Front Neural Circuits 2017; 11:66. [PMID: 28979194 PMCID: PMC5611373 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2017.00066] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2017] [Accepted: 09/01/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Empathy for pain (vicariously felt pain), an ability to feel, recognize, understand and share the painful emotions of others, has been gradually accepted to be a common identity in both humans and rodents, however, the underlying neural and molecular mechanisms are largely unknown. Recently, we have developed a rat model of empathy for pain in which pain can be transferred from a cagemate demonstrator (CD) in pain to a naïve cagemate observer (CO) after 30 min dyadic priming social interaction. The naïve CO rats display both mechanical pain hypersensitivity (hyperalgesia) and enhanced spinal nociception. Chemical lesions of bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) abolish the empathic pain response completely, suggesting existence of a top-down facilitation system in production of empathy for pain. However, the social transfer of pain was not observed in non-cagemate observer (NCO) after dyadic social interaction with a non-cagemate demonstrator (NCD) in pain. Here we showed that dyadic social interaction with a painful CD resulted in elevation of circulating norepinephrine (NE) and increased neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus (LC) in the CO rats. Meanwhile, CO rats also had over-expression of P2X3, but not TRPV1, in the dorsal root ganglia (DRG). Chemical lesion of the LC-NE neurons by systemic DSP-4 and pharmacological inhibition of central synaptic release of NE by clonidine completely abolished increase in circulating NE and P2X3 receptor expression, as well as the sympathetically-maintained development of empathic mechanical hyperalgesia. However, in the NCO rats, neither the LC-NE neuronal activity nor the P2X3 receptor expression was altered after dyadic social interaction with a painful NCD although the circulating corticosterone and NE were elevated. Finally, in the periphery, both P2X3 receptor and α1 adrenergic receptor were found to be involved in the development of empathic mechanical hyperalgesia. Taken together with our previous results, empathy for pain observed in the CO rats is likely to be mediated by activation of the top-down mPFC-LC/NE-sympathoadrenomedullary (SAM) system that further up-regulates P2X3 receptors in the periphery, however, social stress observed in the NCO rats is mediated by activation of both hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis and SAM axis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yun-Fei Lü
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, The Fourth Military Medical UniversityXi'an, China.,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLAXi'an, China.,Anesthesia and Operation Center, 302 Military HospitalBeijing, China
| | - Yan Yang
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, The Fourth Military Medical UniversityXi'an, China.,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLAXi'an, China
| | - Chun-Li Li
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, The Fourth Military Medical UniversityXi'an, China.,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLAXi'an, China
| | - Yan Wang
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, The Fourth Military Medical UniversityXi'an, China.,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLAXi'an, China
| | - Zhen Li
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, The Fourth Military Medical UniversityXi'an, China.,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLAXi'an, China
| | - Jun Chen
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, The Fourth Military Medical UniversityXi'an, China.,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLAXi'an, China.,Beijing Institute for Brain DisordersBeijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
114
|
Boivin GP, Hickman DL, Creamer-Hente MA, Pritchett-Corning KR, Bratcher NA. Review of CO₂ as a Euthanasia Agent for Laboratory Rats and Mice. JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN ASSOCIATION FOR LABORATORY ANIMAL SCIENCE : JAALAS 2017; 56:491-499. [PMID: 28903819 PMCID: PMC5605172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2017] [Revised: 04/19/2017] [Accepted: 05/18/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Selecting an appropriate, effective euthanasia agent is controversial. Several recent publications provide clarity on the use of CO2 in laboratory rats and mice. This review examines previous studies on CO2 euthanasia and presents the current body of knowledge on the subject. Potential areas for further investigation and recommendations are provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory P Boivin
- Department of Pathology, Wright State University, Dayton, Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio
| | - Debra L Hickman
- Laboratory Animal Resource Center, Indiana University, Indianapolis, Indiana;,
| | | | - Kathleen R Pritchett-Corning
- Office of Animal Resources, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, Department of Comparative Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
| | | |
Collapse
|
115
|
Webb CE, Romero T, Franks B, de Waal FBM. Long-term consistency in chimpanzee consolation behaviour reflects empathetic personalities. Nat Commun 2017; 8:292. [PMID: 28819207 PMCID: PMC5561193 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-017-00360-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2016] [Accepted: 06/22/2017] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
In contrast to a wealth of human studies, little is known about the ontogeny and consistency of empathy-related capacities in other species. Consolation-post-conflict affiliation from uninvolved bystanders to distressed others-is a suggested marker of empathetic concern in non-human animals. Using longitudinal data comprising nearly a decade of observations on over 3000 conflict interactions in 44 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), we provide evidence for relatively stable individual differences in consolation behaviour. Across development, individuals consistently differ from one another in this trait, with higher consolatory tendencies predicting better social integration, a sign of social competence. Further, similar to recent results in other ape species, but in contrast to many human self-reported findings, older chimpanzees are less likely to console than are younger individuals. Overall, given the link between consolation and empathy, these findings help elucidate the development of individual socio-cognitive and -emotional abilities in one of our closest relatives.Non-human animals are known to exhibit behaviours suggestive of empathy, but the development and maintenance of these traits is unexplored. Here, Webb and colleagues quantify individual consolation tendencies over 10 years across two chimpanzee groups and show evidence of consistent 'empathetic personalities'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine E Webb
- Living Links, Yerkes National Primate Research Center and Psychology Department, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, 30322, USA.
- Utrecht University, Utrecht, 3508 TC, The Netherlands.
| | - Teresa Romero
- Living Links, Yerkes National Primate Research Center and Psychology Department, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, 30322, USA.
- School of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, LN6 7TS, UK.
- Department of Cognitive and Behavioural Science, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, 153-0041, Japan.
| | - Becca Franks
- Animal Welfare Program, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4
| | - Frans B M de Waal
- Living Links, Yerkes National Primate Research Center and Psychology Department, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, 30322, USA
- Utrecht University, Utrecht, 3508 TC, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
116
|
Identifying affective personality profiles: A latent profile analysis of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Sci Rep 2017; 7:4548. [PMID: 28674393 PMCID: PMC5495783 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-04738-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2016] [Accepted: 05/23/2017] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Based on evolutionary theory, a recent model in affective neuroscience delineated six emotional brain systems at the core of human personality: SEEKING, CARING, PLAYFULNESS, FEAR, ANGER, SADNESS. The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) assess their functioning. Using a person-centred approach of the ANPS, this study: (i) examined the existence of latent personality profiles, (ii) studied their gender invariance, (iii) assessed their longitudinal (4 years) stability, and (iv) explored how they relate to several intrapersonal, interpersonal, and emotion regulation skills. Latent Profile Analysis in 2 samples (Canadian, longitudinal, N = 520; French, cross-sectional, N = 830) found that, qualitatively, 3 profiles characterized both populations and genders, with one distinction for the second profile where the French women endorsed slightly higher and lower scores for, respectively, the negative and positive emotions. Whilst not being quantitatively similar across genders, the personality profiles remained consistent across time in the longitudinal sample. Associations between profiles and intrapersonal (e.g. depression), interpersonal (e.g. empathy), and emotion regulation skills measures (e.g. emotional intelligence) offered concurrent validity evidence. This person centred approach to ANPS offers a holistic and parsimonious way to study affective personality dimensions. It opens promising avenues for future studies on the predictive value of ANPS profiles, and for personality-targeted interventions.
Collapse
|
117
|
Kraaijenvanger EJ, Hofman D, Bos PA. A neuroendocrine account of facial mimicry and its dynamic modulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 77:98-106. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2016] [Revised: 02/15/2017] [Accepted: 03/12/2017] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
|
118
|
Pérez-Manrique A, Gomila A. The comparative study of empathy: sympathetic concern and empathic perspective-taking in non-human animals. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2017; 93:248-269. [DOI: 10.1111/brv.12342] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2016] [Revised: 04/25/2017] [Accepted: 05/03/2017] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ana Pérez-Manrique
- Department of Psychology; Human Evolution and Cognition Group (EvoCog), UIB, IFISC, Associated Unit to CSIC; 07122 Palma Spain
| | - Antoni Gomila
- Department of Psychology; Human Evolution and Cognition Group (EvoCog), UIB, IFISC, Associated Unit to CSIC; 07122 Palma Spain
| |
Collapse
|
119
|
McBride EA. Small prey species’ behaviour and welfare: implications for veterinary professionals. J Small Anim Pract 2017; 58:423-436. [DOI: 10.1111/jsap.12681] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2015] [Revised: 03/01/2017] [Accepted: 03/03/2017] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- E. Anne McBride
- School of Psychology; University of Southampton; Southampton, SO17 1BJ UK
| |
Collapse
|
120
|
Chen J. Empathy for Distress in Humans and Rodents. Neurosci Bull 2017; 34:216-236. [PMID: 28493169 DOI: 10.1007/s12264-017-0135-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2017] [Accepted: 03/01/2017] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Empathy is traditionally thought to be a unique ability of humans to feel, understand, and share the emotional state of others. However, the notion has been greatly challenged by the emerging discoveries of empathy for pain or distress in rodents. Because empathy is believed to be fundamental to the formation of prosocial, altruistic, and even moral behaviors in social animals and humans, studies associated with decoding the neural circuits and unraveling the underlying molecular and neural mechanisms of empathy for pain or distress in rodents would be very important and encouraging. In this review, the author set out to outline and update the concept of empathy from the evolutionary point of view, and introduce up-to-date advances in the study of empathy and its neural correlates in both humans and rodents. Finally, the author highlights the perspectives and challenges for the further use of rodent models in the study of empathy for pain or distress.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jun Chen
- Institute for Biomedical Sciences of Pain, Tangdu Hospital, Fourth Military Medical University, Xi'an, 710038, China. .,Key Laboratory of Brain Stress and Behavior, PLA, Xi'an, 710038, China. .,Beijing Institute for Brain Disorders, Beijing, 100069, China.
| |
Collapse
|
121
|
Meyza KZ, Bartal IBA, Monfils MH, Panksepp JB, Knapska E. The roots of empathy: Through the lens of rodent models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 76:216-234. [PMID: 27825924 PMCID: PMC5418107 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 110] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2015] [Revised: 10/18/2016] [Accepted: 10/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Empathy is a phenomenon often considered dependent on higher-order emotional control and an ability to relate to the emotional state of others. It is, by many, attributed only to species having well-developed cortical circuits capable of performing such complex tasks. However, over the years, a wealth of data has been accumulated showing that rodents are capable not only of sharing emotional states of their conspecifics, but also of prosocial behavior driven by such shared experiences. The study of rodent empathic behaviors is only now becoming an independent research field. Relevant animal models allow precise manipulation of neural networks, thereby offering insight into the foundations of empathy in the mammalian brains. Here we review the data on empathic behaviors in rat and mouse models, their neurobiological and neurophysiological correlates, and the factors influencing these behaviors. We discuss how simple rodent models of empathy enhance our understanding of how brain controls empathic behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Z Meyza
- Laboratory of Emotions' Neurobiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Warsaw, Poland.
| | - I Ben-Ami Bartal
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
| | - M H Monfils
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas, Austin, TX, USA
| | - J B Panksepp
- Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
| | - E Knapska
- Laboratory of Emotions' Neurobiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Warsaw, Poland.
| |
Collapse
|
122
|
Elevated emotional contagion in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease is associated with increased synchronization in the insula and amygdala. Sci Rep 2017; 7:46262. [PMID: 28387348 PMCID: PMC5384199 DOI: 10.1038/srep46262] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2016] [Accepted: 03/14/2017] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotional contagion, a primitive form of empathy, is heightened in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD); however, the mechanism underlying this attribute has not been thoroughly elucidated. In this study, observational fear conditioning was performed to measure emotional contagion levels in a mouse model of AD. Simultaneous recording of local field potentials in the bilateral anterior insula, basolateral amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, and retrosplenial cortex was also conducted to investigate related brain network changes. Consistent with the results obtained with AD patients, 11-month-old AD model mice exhibited significantly higher freezing levels in observational fear conditioning, indicating elevated emotional contagion compared to age-matched wild-type mice. Furthermore, the left anterior insula and right basolateral amygdala of 11-months-old AD model mice indicated sustained increases in synchronization when they observed the suffering of conspecifics. These changes did not appear in other age groups or wild-type controls. Additionally, the amyloid plaque burden within the anterior insula was significantly correlated with the freezing levels in observational fear conditioning. Taken together, this study reveals increased and sustained network synchrony between the anterior insula and basolateral amygdala, which comprise a salience network in humans, as a potential mechanism for elevated emotional contagion in a mouse model of AD.
Collapse
|
123
|
Tremblay S, Sharika KM, Platt ML. Social Decision-Making and the Brain: A Comparative Perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 2017; 21:265-276. [PMID: 28214131 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2016] [Revised: 01/06/2017] [Accepted: 01/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The capacity and motivation to be social is a key component of the human adaptive behavioral repertoire. Recent research has identified social behaviors remarkably similar to our own in other animals, including empathy, consolation, cooperation, and strategic deception. Moreover, neurobiological studies in humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents have identified shared brain structures (the so-called 'social brain') apparently specialized to mediate such functions. Neuromodulators may regulate social interactions by 'tuning' the social brain, with important implications for treating social impairments. Here, we survey recent findings in social neuroscience from a comparative perspective, and conclude that the very social behaviors that make us human emerge from mechanisms shared widely with other animals, as well as some that appear to be unique to humans and other primates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sébastien Tremblay
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - K M Sharika
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Michael L Platt
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA; Department of Marketing, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
124
|
Abstract
Over the past two decades, the question of how our brain makes us sensitive to the state of conspecifics and how that affects our behaviour has undergone a profound change. Twenty years ago what would now be called social neuroscience was focused on the visual processing of facial expressions and body movements in temporal lobe structures of primates (Puce and Perrett 2003). With the discovery of mirror neurons, this changed rapidly towards the modern field of social neuroscience, in which high-level vision is but one of many focuses of interest. In this essay, we will argue that for the further progress of the field, the integration of animal neuroscience and human neuroscience is paramount. We will do so, by focusing on the field of embodied social cognition. We will first show how the combination of animal and human neuroscience was critical in how the discovery of mirror neurons placed the motor system on the map of social cognition. We will then argue why an integrated cross-species approach will be pivotal to our understanding of the neural basis of emotional empathy and its link to prosocial behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Keysers
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, A Research Institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Meibergdreef 47, 1105BA, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | - Valeria Gazzola
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, A Research Institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Meibergdreef 47, 1105BA, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
125
|
Yamamoto S. Primate empathy: three factors and their combinations for empathy-related phenomena. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2016; 8. [PMID: 27977913 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1431] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2015] [Revised: 10/05/2016] [Accepted: 10/14/2016] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Empathy as a research topic is receiving increasing attention, although there seems some confusion on the definition of empathy across different fields. Frans de Waal (de Waal FBM. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu Rev Psychol 2008, 59:279-300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625) used empathy as an umbrella term and proposed a comprehensive model for the evolution of empathy with some of its basic elements in nonhuman animals. In de Waal's model, empathy consists of several layers distinguished by required cognitive levels; the perception-action mechanism plays the core role for connecting ourself and others. Then, human-like empathy such as perspective-taking develops in outer layers according to cognitive sophistication, leading to prosocial acts such as targeted helping. I agree that animals demonstrate many empathy-related phenomena; however, the species differences and the level of cognitive sophistication of the phenomena might be interpreted in another way than this simple linearly developing model. Our recent studies with chimpanzees showed that their perspective-taking ability does not necessarily lead to proactive helping behavior. Herein, as a springboard for further studies, I reorganize the empathy-related phenomena by proposing a combination model instead of the linear development model. This combination model is composed of three organizing factors: matching with others, understanding of others, and prosociality. With these three factors and their combinations, most empathy-related matters can be categorized and mapped to appropriate context; this may be a good first step to discuss the evolution of empathy in relation to the neural connections in human and nonhuman animal brains. I would like to propose further comparative studies, especially from the viewpoint of Homo-Pan (chimpanzee and bonobo) comparison. WIREs Cogn Sci 2017, 8:e1431. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1431 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shinya Yamamoto
- Graduate School of Intercultural Studies, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
126
|
Schwartz LP, Silberberg A, Casey AH, Kearns DN, Slotnick B. Does a rat release a soaked conspecific due to empathy? Anim Cogn 2016; 20:299-308. [PMID: 27822786 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-016-1052-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2016] [Revised: 10/27/2016] [Accepted: 10/31/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In Experiment 1, rats choosing in an E maze preferred to release a rat standing in a pool of water to dry ground over a rat already standing on dry ground. Five additional experiments showed that the choosing rat's preference for releasing the wet rat was maintained by two separable outcomes: (1) the social contact offered by the released rat and (2) the reinforcing value of proximity to a pool of water. These results call into question Sato et al.'s (Anim Cogn 18:1039-1047, 2015) claim to have demonstrated that a rat's releasing of a wet rat to dry ground is empathically motivated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Alan Silberberg
- Department of Psychology, American University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Anna H Casey
- Department of Psychology, American University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - David N Kearns
- Department of Psychology, American University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Burton Slotnick
- Department of Psychology, American University, Washington, DC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
127
|
Karterud S, Pedersen G, Johansen M, Wilberg T, Davis K, Panksepp J. Primary emotional traits in patients with personality disorders. Personal Ment Health 2016; 10:261-273. [PMID: 27257161 DOI: 10.1002/pmh.1345] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2015] [Revised: 03/30/2016] [Accepted: 04/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
There is a longstanding tradition that connects temperament pathology and personality disorders. Emotions are the major constituents of temperament. In mammals, seven primary emotions have been identified: SEEKING, FEAR, CARE, RAGE, SADNESS/PANIC, LUST and PLAY. The study aimed at exploring the relationship between primary emotions and personality disorders (PDs). Five hundred forty-six patients with different degrees and qualities of personality pathology, admitted to treatment in specialized PD services, were diagnosed according to Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders, and their primary emotional profiles were assessed by the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales explained 19% of the variance in borderline and avoidant criteria. The DSM-IV PD categories displayed different patterns of association to the primary emotions, e.g. the borderline PD profile suggested low thresholds for RAGE and SADNESS, but on the positive side a propensity for SEEKING. In contrast, the dependent PD profile suggested a low threshold for SADNESS but a high threshold for RAGE and SEEKING. The results are promising for a more coherent and evolution-based overall theory of PDs, and the correlations found in this study indicate testable causal pathways to PDs. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sigmund Karterud
- Institute for Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway. .,Oslo University Hospital, Ullevaal, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo, Norway.
| | - Geir Pedersen
- Oslo University Hospital, Ullevaal, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo, Norway
| | - Merete Johansen
- Oslo University Hospital, Ullevaal, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo, Norway
| | - Theresa Wilberg
- Institute for Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.,Oslo University Hospital, Ullevaal, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo, Norway
| | - Ken Davis
- University of North Carolina Charlotte, Department of Psychology, Charlotte, NC, USA
| | - Jaak Panksepp
- Department of Integrative Physiology and Neuroscience, Washington State University, Pullman, WA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
128
|
Bethlehem RAI, Allison C, van Andel EM, Coles AI, Neil K, Baron-Cohen S. Does empathy predict altruism in the wild? Soc Neurosci 2016; 12:743-750. [DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1249944] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Carrie Allison
- Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Emma M. van Andel
- Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Alexander I. Coles
- Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Kimberley Neil
- Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Simon Baron-Cohen
- Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
129
|
Abstract
A large body of research has been dedicated to the effects of gastrointestinal peptides on vagal afferent fibres, yet multiple lines of evidence indicate that gastrointestinal peptides also modulate brainstem vagal neurocircuitry, and that this modulation has a fundamental role in the physiology and pathophysiology of the upper gastrointestinal tract. In fact, brainstem vagovagal neurocircuits comprise highly plastic neurons and synapses connecting afferent vagal fibres, second order neurons of the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), and efferent fibres originating in the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus (DMV). Neuronal communication between the NTS and DMV is regulated by the presence of a variety of inputs, both from within the brainstem itself as well as from higher centres, which utilize an array of neurotransmitters and neuromodulators. Because of the circumventricular nature of these brainstem areas, circulating hormones can also modulate the vagal output to the upper gastrointestinal tract. This Review summarizes the organization and function of vagovagal reflex control of the upper gastrointestinal tract, presents data on the plasticity within these neurocircuits after stress, and discusses the gastrointestinal dysfunctions observed in Parkinson disease as examples of physiological adjustment and maladaptation of these reflexes.
Collapse
|
130
|
Feinberg TE, Mallatt J. The nature of primary consciousness. A new synthesis. Conscious Cogn 2016; 43:113-27. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.05.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2016] [Accepted: 05/20/2016] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
|
131
|
Ku KM, Weir RK, Silverman JL, Berman RF, Bauman MD. Behavioral Phenotyping of Juvenile Long-Evans and Sprague-Dawley Rats: Implications for Preclinical Models of Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0158150. [PMID: 27351457 PMCID: PMC4924796 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158150] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2016] [Accepted: 06/10/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The laboratory rat is emerging as an attractive preclinical animal model of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), allowing investigators to explore genetic, environmental and pharmacological manipulations in a species exhibiting complex, reciprocal social behavior. The present study was carried out to compare two commonly used strains of laboratory rats, Sprague-Dawley (SD) and Long-Evans (LE), between the ages of postnatal day (PND) 26-56 using high-throughput behavioral phenotyping tools commonly used in mouse models of ASD that we have adapted for use in rats. We detected few differences between young SD and LE strains on standard assays of exploration, sensorimotor gating, anxiety, repetitive behaviors, and learning. Both SD and LE strains also demonstrated sociability in the 3-chamber social approach test as indexed by spending more time in the social chamber with a constrained age/strain/sex matched novel partner than in an identical chamber without a partner. Pronounced differences between the two strains were, however, detected when the rats were allowed to freely interact with a novel partner in the social dyad paradigm. The SD rats in this particular testing paradigm engaged in play more frequently and for longer durations than the LE rats at both juvenile and young adult developmental time points. Results from this study that are particularly relevant for developing preclinical ASD models in rats are threefold: (i) commonly utilized strains exhibit unique patterns of social interactions, including strain-specific play behaviors, (ii) the testing environment may profoundly influence the expression of strain-specific social behavior and (iii) simple, automated measures of sociability may not capture the complexities of rat social interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine M. Ku
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America
| | - Ruth K. Weir
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America
| | - Jill L. Silverman
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America
- The MIND Institute, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America
| | - Robert F. Berman
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of California, Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
| | - Melissa D. Bauman
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America
- The MIND Institute, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America
- California National Primate Research Center, Davis, California, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
132
|
Simons LE, Goubert L, Vervoort T, Borsook D. Circles of engagement: Childhood pain and parent brain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 68:537-546. [PMID: 27320958 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2016] [Revised: 05/17/2016] [Accepted: 06/16/2016] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
Social interaction can have a profound effect on individual behavior, perhaps most salient in interactions between sick suffering children and their parents. Chronic pain is a difficult condition that can produce considerable changes in children's behaviors that can secondarily have profound effects on their parents. It may create a functionally disabling negative feedback loop. Research supports the notion of alterations in the brain of individuals who observe and empathize with loved ones in acute pain. However, neural activity in relation to empathic responses in the context of chronic pain has not been examined. Ongoing suffering with chronic pain in a child can result in child's brain circuit alterations. Moreover, prolonged suffering jointly experienced by the parent may putatively produce maladaptive changes in their neural networks and consequently in parental behaviors. Here we put forth the conceptual framework for 'Chronic pain contagion' (CPC). We review the underlying processes in CPC and discuss implications for devising and implementing treatments for children in chronic pain and their parents.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura E Simons
- Center for Pain and the Brain, Boston Childrens Hospital (BCH), Boston, USA; Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative, and Pain Medicine, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA.
| | - Liesbet Goubert
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Tine Vervoort
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - David Borsook
- Center for Pain and the Brain, Boston Childrens Hospital (BCH), Boston, USA; Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative, and Pain Medicine, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA
| |
Collapse
|
133
|
Sivaselvachandran S, Acland EL, Abdallah S, Martin LJ. Behavioral and mechanistic insight into rodent empathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 91:130-137. [PMID: 27311631 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2016] [Revised: 05/20/2016] [Accepted: 06/10/2016] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Empathy is a psychological construct that allows individuals to understand and share the emotions of others. The ability to share emotional states relies on basic social mechanisms, such as mimicry and emotional contagion, which are considered building blocks for empathy. Mimicking another's emotional or physical state is essential for successful social interactions and is found in a number of animal species. For the current review we focus on emotional state sharing in rodents, a core feature of empathy that is often measured using pain and fear as proxies; we also discuss prosociality in rodents. The evidence for empathy in rodents shows that rats and mice consistently imitate arousal states and behaviors of conspecifics and will even sacrifice personal gain to relieve the distress of a conspecific. These behaviors support basic processes that are crucial for the survival of individual animals and give us insight into the neural mechanisms that govern empathy-related behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Erinn L Acland
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C5 Canada
| | - Salsabil Abdallah
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C5 Canada
| | - Loren J Martin
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C5 Canada; Department of Cell Systems and Biology, University of Toronto Mississauga, ON L5L 1C5 Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
134
|
van Goozen SH, Langley K, Northover C, Hubble K, Rubia K, Schepman K, O'Donovan MC, Thapar A. Identifying mechanisms that underlie links between COMT genotype and aggression in male adolescents with ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2016; 57:472-80. [PMID: 26395975 PMCID: PMC5102627 DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12464] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/10/2015] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND There is a known strong genetic contribution to aggression in those with ADHD. In a previous investigation of a large population cohort, impaired 'emotional/social cognitive' processing, assessed by questionnaire, was observed to mediate the link between COMT Val158Met and aggression in individuals with ADHD. We set out to replicate and extend this finding in a clinical sample, using task-based and physiological assessments of emotional and cognitive processing. Our aim was to test the hypothesis that directly assessed emotional processing mediates the link between COMT Val158Met and aggression in young people with ADHD. METHODS Males aged 10-17 years with ADHD were recruited from UK community clinics (n = 194). Research diagnostic interviews (parent and child) were used to assess psychopathology and generate DSM-IV Conduct Disorder symptom scores. Participants completed tasks assessing executive function (response inhibition and set shifting), empathy for fear, sadness and happiness, and fear conditioning [measured using skin conductance responses (SCR) to aversive stimuli]. RESULTS COMT Val allele carriers showed poorer response inhibition (F = 5.27, p = .02) and set shifting abilities (F = 6.45, p = .01), reduced fear empathy (F = 4.33, p = .04) and reduced autonomic responsiveness (lower SCRs) to the conditioned aversive stimulus (F = 11.74, p = .001). COMT Val158Met did not predict impairments in recognising others' emotions or affective empathy for happiness or sadness. Mediation analysis revealed that impaired fear-related mechanisms indirectly mediated the link between COMT Val158Met and aggression. CONCLUSION Our findings suggest fear mechanisms as possible targets for psychological interventions to disrupt links between genetic risk and aggressive outcomes in ADHD. Our findings also reveal the potential of hypothesis-driven approaches for identifying neuropsychological mechanisms that mediate genetic risk effects on behaviour and psychopathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Kate Langley
- School of PsychologyCardiff UniversityCardiffUK
- MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and GenomicsCardiff UniversityCardiffUK
| | | | | | - Katya Rubia
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and NeuroscienceKings College LondonLondonUK
| | - Karen Schepman
- MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and GenomicsCardiff UniversityCardiffUK
- Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical NeurosciencesCardiff University School of MedicineCardiffUK
| | - Michael C. O'Donovan
- MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and GenomicsCardiff UniversityCardiffUK
- Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical NeurosciencesCardiff University School of MedicineCardiffUK
| | - Anita Thapar
- MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and GenomicsCardiff UniversityCardiffUK
- Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical NeurosciencesCardiff University School of MedicineCardiffUK
| |
Collapse
|
135
|
Park C, Lee YJ, Hong M, Jung CH, Synn Y, Kwack YS, Ryu JS, Park TW, Lee SA, Bahn GH. A Multicenter Study Investigating Empathy and Burnout Characteristics in Medical Residents with Various Specialties. J Korean Med Sci 2016; 31:590-7. [PMID: 27051244 PMCID: PMC4810343 DOI: 10.3346/jkms.2016.31.4.590] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2015] [Accepted: 12/22/2015] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
We assessed empathy in medical residents, including factors modifying empathy and the relationship between empathy and burnout. Participants (n = 317 residents, response rate = 42%) from 4 university hospitals completed a socio-demographic questionnaire, the Jefferson Scale of Empathy (Health Professional version, Korean edition), and the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI). Participants were classified by medical specialty: "people-oriented specialty" (POS group) or "technology-oriented specialty" (TOS group), with more women in the POS than in the TOS group, χ(2) = 14.12, P < 0.001. Being female, married, and having children were factors related to higher empathy (gender, t = -2.129, P = 0.034; marriage, t = -2.078, P = 0.038; children, t = 2.86, P = 0.005). Within specialty group, POS residents showed higher empathy scores in the fourth as compared to the first year, F = 3.166, P = 0.026. Comparing POS and TOS groups by year, fourth year POS residents had significantly higher scores than did fourth year TOS residents, t = 3.349, P = 0.002. There were negative correlations between empathy scores and 2 MBI subscales, emotional exhaustion (EE) and depersonalization (DP). Additionally, first year POS residents had higher DP scores than did first year TOS residents, t = 2.183, P = 0.031. We suggest that factors important for empathy are type of medical specialty, marriage, siblings, and children. Burnout state may be related to decreasing empathy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chanmin Park
- Department of Psychiatry, Kyung Hee University Hospital, Seoul, Korea
| | - Yeon Jung Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, Soonchunhyang University Hospital, Seoul, Korea
| | - Minha Hong
- Department of Psychiatry, Myongji Hospital, Goyang, Korea
| | - Chul-Ho Jung
- Department of Psychiatry, Keimyung University College of Medicine, Daegu, Korea
| | - Yeni Synn
- Department of Psychiatry, Keimyung University College of Medicine, Daegu, Korea
| | - Young-Sook Kwack
- Department of Psychiatry, Jeju National University College of Medicine, Jeju, Korea
| | - Jae-Sung Ryu
- Department of Psychiatry, Jeju National University College of Medicine, Jeju, Korea
| | - Tae Won Park
- Department of Psychiatry, Chonbuk National University College of Medicine, Jeonju, Korea
| | - Seong Ae Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, Kyung Hee University Hospital, Seoul, Korea
| | - Geon Ho Bahn
- Department of Psychiatry, Kyung Hee University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| |
Collapse
|
136
|
Betti V, Aglioti SM. Dynamic construction of the neural networks underpinning empathy for pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 63:191-206. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2015] [Revised: 02/08/2016] [Accepted: 02/09/2016] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
|
137
|
Taschereau-Dumouchel V, Hétu S, Bagramian A, Labrecque A, Racine M, Chagnon YC, Jackson PL. BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Is Associated with Self-Reported Empathy. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0149911. [PMID: 26901829 PMCID: PMC4764204 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2015] [Accepted: 02/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Empathy is an important driver of human social behaviors and presents genetic roots that have been studied in neuroimaging using the intermediate phenotype approach. Notably, the Val66Met polymorphism of the Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene has been identified as a potential target in neuroimaging studies based on its influence on emotion perception and social cognition, but its impact on self-reported empathy has never been documented. Using a neurogenetic approach, we investigated the association between the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and self-reported empathy (Davis' Interpersonal Reactivity Index; IRI) in a sample of 110 young adults. Our results indicate that the BDNF genotype is significantly associated with the linear combination of the four facets of the IRI, one of the most widely used self-reported empathy questionnaire. Crucially, the effect of BDNF Val66Met goes beyond the variance explained by two polymorphisms of the oxytocin transporter gene previously associated with empathy and its neural underpinnings (OXTR rs53576 and rs2254298). These results represent the first evidence suggesting a link between the BDNF gene and self-reported empathy and warrant further studies of this polymorphism due to its potential clinical significance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vincent Taschereau-Dumouchel
- École de psychologie, Université Laval, Québec, Québec, Canada
- Centre interdisciplinaire de recherche en réadaptation et intégration sociale (CIRRIS), Québec, Québec, Canada
- Centre de recherche de l’institut universitaire en santé mentale de Québec (CRIUSMQ), Québec, Québec, Canada
- * E-mail:
| | - Sébastien Hétu
- Human Neuroimaging Laboratory, Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, Roanoke, Virginia, United States of America
| | - Anaït Bagramian
- École de psychologie, Université Laval, Québec, Québec, Canada
| | | | - Marion Racine
- École de psychologie, Université Laval, Québec, Québec, Canada
| | - Yvon C. Chagnon
- Centre de recherche de l’institut universitaire en santé mentale de Québec (CRIUSMQ), Québec, Québec, Canada
- Département de Psychiatrie et des Neurosciences, Université Laval, Québec, Québec, Canada
| | - Philip L. Jackson
- École de psychologie, Université Laval, Québec, Québec, Canada
- Centre interdisciplinaire de recherche en réadaptation et intégration sociale (CIRRIS), Québec, Québec, Canada
- Centre de recherche de l’institut universitaire en santé mentale de Québec (CRIUSMQ), Québec, Québec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
138
|
Panksepp JB, Lahvis GP. Differential influence of social versus isolate housing on vicarious fear learning in adolescent mice. Behav Neurosci 2016; 130:206-11. [PMID: 26881314 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000133] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Laboratory rodents can adopt the pain or fear of nearby conspecifics. This phenotype conceptually lies within the domain of empathy, a bio-psycho-social process through which individuals come to share each other's emotion. Using a model of cue-conditioned fear, we show here that the expression of vicarious fear varies with respect to whether mice are raised socially or in solitude during adolescence. The impact of the adolescent housing environment was selective: (a) vicarious fear was more influenced than directly acquired fear, (b) "long-term" (24-h postconditioning) vicarious fear memories were stronger than "short-term" (15-min postconditioning) memories in socially reared mice whereas the opposite was true for isolate mice, and (c) females were more fearful than males. Housing differences during adolescence did not alter the general mobility of mice or their vocal response to receiving the unconditioned stimulus. Previous work with this mouse model underscored a genetic influence on vicarious fear learning, and the present study complements these findings by elucidating an interaction between the adolescent social environment and vicarious experience. Collectively, these findings are relevant to developing models of empathy amenable to mechanistic exploitation in the laboratory. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jules B Panksepp
- Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health and Science University
| | - Garet P Lahvis
- Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health and Science University
| |
Collapse
|
139
|
Lee H, Noh J. Pair exposure with conspecific during fear conditioning induces the link between freezing and passive avoidance behaviors in rats. Neurosci Res 2016; 108:40-5. [PMID: 26827818 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2016.01.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2015] [Revised: 01/19/2016] [Accepted: 01/19/2016] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
Social factor plays an important role in dealing with posttraumatic stress disorder related to excessive physiological fear response and insufficient fear memory extinction of the brain. However, although social circumstances occurred not only during contextual retrieval but also during fear conditioning, most previous studies focused on the advantageous aspects of social buffering in fear retrieval period. To demonstrate the association between fear responses and fear memory from social stimuli during fear conditioning, pair exposed rats with conspecific as social buffering were subjected to a fear conditioning of passive avoidance test to evaluate memory function and freezing behavior. Whereas single exposed rats showed the significant increase of freezing behaviors and passive avoidance behaviors compared to control rats, pair exposed rats showed significant alleviation of the freezing behaviors and passive avoidance behaviors compared to single exposed rats. Furthermore, we determined a significant correlation between freezing and passive avoidance behavioral alteration in pair exposed rats. Taken together, we suggest that pair exposure with conspecific during fear conditioning helps to cope with both freezing response and fear memory systems and their reciprocal interaction has a crucial potential as a resource for the relief of unreasonable stress responses in posttraumatic stress disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hyunchan Lee
- Department of Science Education, College of Education, Dankook University, 152 Jukjeon-ro, Suji-gu, Yongin-si, Gyeonggi-do 16890, Republic of Korea
| | - Jihyun Noh
- Department of Science Education, College of Education, Dankook University, 152 Jukjeon-ro, Suji-gu, Yongin-si, Gyeonggi-do 16890, Republic of Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
140
|
Boivin GP, Bottomley MA, Grobe N. Responses of Male C57BL/6N Mice to Observing the Euthanasia of Other Mice. JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN ASSOCIATION FOR LABORATORY ANIMAL SCIENCE : JAALAS 2016; 55:406-411. [PMID: 27423146 PMCID: PMC4943610] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2015] [Revised: 11/02/2015] [Accepted: 12/14/2015] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
The AVMA Panel on Euthanasia recommends that sensitive animals should not be present during the euthanasia of others, especially of their own species, but does not provide guidelines on how to identify a sensitive species. To determine if mice are a sensitive species we reviewed literature on empathy in mice, and measured the cardiovascular and activity response of mice observing euthanasia of conspecifics. We studied male 16-wk-old C57BL/6N mice and found no increase in cardiovascular parameters or activity in the response of the mice to observing CO2 euthanasia. Mice observing decapitation had an increase in all values, but this was paralleled by a similar increase during mock decapitations in which no animals were handled or euthanized. We conclude that CO2 euthanasia of mice does not have an impact on other mice in the room, and that euthanasia by decapitation likely only has an effect due to the noise of the guillotine. We support the conceptual idea that mice are both a sensitive species and display empathy, but under the controlled circumstances of the euthanasia procedures used in this study there was no signaling of stress to witnessing inhabitants in the room.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory P Boivin
- Department of Pathology, Wright State University, Dayton, Ohio, USA; Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, USA.
| | - Michael A Bottomley
- Department of Mathematical and Microbiologic Sciences, Wright State University, Dayton, Ohio, USA
| | - Nadja Grobe
- Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, Boonshoft School of Medicine, Wright State University, Dayton, Ohio, USA
| |
Collapse
|
141
|
Pereira M, Ferreira A. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of parenting: Dynamic coordination of motivational, affective and cognitive processes. Horm Behav 2016; 77:72-85. [PMID: 26296592 DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2015] [Revised: 08/13/2015] [Accepted: 08/13/2015] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
This article is part of a Special Issue "Parental Care". Becoming a parent is arguably the most profound transforming experience in life. It is also inherently very emotionally and physically demanding, such that the reciprocal interaction with the young changes the brain and behavior of the parents. In this review, we examine the neurobiological mechanisms of parenting primarily discussing recent research findings in rodents and primates, especially humans. We argue that it is essential to consider parenting within a conceptual framework that recognizes the dynamics of the reciprocal mother-young relationship, including both the complexity and neuroplasticity of its underlying mechanisms. Converging research suggests that the concerted activity of a distributed network of subcortical and cortical brain structures regulates different key aspects of parenting, including the sensory analysis of infant stimuli as well as motivational, affective and cognitive processes. The interplay among these processes depends on the action of various neurotransmitters and hormones that modulate the timely and coordinated execution of caregiving responses of the maternal circuitry exquisitely attuned to the young's affect, needs and developmental stage. We conclude with a summary and a set of questions that may guide future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariana Pereira
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, USA.
| | - Annabel Ferreira
- Sección Fisiología y Nutrición, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de la República, Uruguay
| |
Collapse
|
142
|
Marsh AA. Neural, cognitive, and evolutionary foundations of human altruism. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2015; 7:59-71. [PMID: 26685796 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1377] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2015] [Revised: 10/16/2015] [Accepted: 11/20/2015] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
This article considers three forms of altruism from both a psychological and a neural perspective, with an emphasis on homologies that can be observed across species and potentially illuminate altruism's evolutionary origins. Kin-based altruism benefits biological relatives and, according to the theory of inclusive fitness, is ultimately beneficial to the altruist from a genetic standpoint. Kin selection adequately explains some altruistic behavior, but it is not applicable to much human altruism. Little is known about the neural processes that support it, but they may include cortical regions involved in processing autobiographical memory and the identities of familiar others. Reciprocity-based altruism is performed in expectation of future rewards and is supported by dopaminergic cortico-striatal networks that guide behavior according to anticipated rewards. Care-based altruism is aimed at improving the well-being of distressed and vulnerable individuals and is closely linked to empathic concern. This form of altruism is thought to rely on the subcortical neural systems that support parental care, particularly structures densely populated with receptors for the hormones oxytocin and vasopressin, including the amygdala, stria terminalis, and striatum. The amygdala may be a particularly important convergence point for care-based altruism because of its dual role in responding both to cues that signal infantile vulnerability and those that signal distress. Research on altruism continues to converge across disciplines, but more research linking molecular-level neural processes to altruistic behavior in humans and other species is needed, as is research on how various forms of altruism intersect. For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Abigail A Marsh
- Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
143
|
Sex differences in social modulation of learning in rats. Sci Rep 2015; 5:18114. [PMID: 26655917 PMCID: PMC4677340 DOI: 10.1038/srep18114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2015] [Accepted: 11/12/2015] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In its simplest form, empathy can be characterized as the capacity to share the emotional experiences among individuals, a phenomenon known as emotional contagion. Recent research shows that emotional contagion and its adaptive role can be studied in rodents. However, it is not known whether sex differences observed in human empathy extend to its more primitive forms. In the present study, we used a rat model of emotional contagion to compare the behavioral consequences of social transfer of information about threat, and the subsequent neural activation patterns in male and female rats. We found that: (1) males and females display a similar behavioral pattern during the interaction with either a fear-conditioned or a control rat; (2) interaction with a fear-conditioned conspecific positively modulates two-way avoidance learning in male and diestral female rats but not in estral females; and (3) such interaction results in increased c-Fos expression in the central and lateral nuclei of the amygdala and the prelimbic and infralimbic cortex in males, whereas in females no such changes were observed. Collectively, our results point to the occurrence of sex and estrus cycle phase differences in susceptibility to emotional contagion and underlying neuronal activation in rodents.
Collapse
|
144
|
Palagi E, Nicotra V, Cordoni G. Rapid mimicry and emotional contagion in domestic dogs. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2015; 2:150505. [PMID: 27019737 PMCID: PMC4807458 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.150505] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2015] [Accepted: 11/23/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
Emotional contagion is a basic form of empathy that makes individuals able to experience others' emotions. In human and non-human primates, emotional contagion can be linked to facial mimicry, an automatic and fast response (less than 1 s) in which individuals involuntary mimic others' expressions. Here, we tested whether body (play bow, PBOW) and facial (relaxed open-mouth, ROM) rapid mimicry is present in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) during dyadic intraspecific play. During their free playful interactions, dogs showed a stronger and rapid mimicry response (less than 1 s) after perceiving PBOW and ROM (two signals typical of play in dogs) than after perceiving JUMP and BITE (two play patterns resembling PBOW and ROM in motor performance). Playful sessions punctuated by rapid mimicry lasted longer that those sessions punctuated only by signals. Moreover, the distribution of rapid mimicry was strongly affected by the familiarity linking the subjects involved: the stronger the social bonding, the higher the level of rapid mimicry. In conclusion, our results demonstrate the presence of rapid mimicry in dogs, the involvement of mimicry in sharing playful motivation and the social modulation of the phenomenon. All these findings concur in supporting the idea that a possible linkage between rapid mimicry and emotional contagion (a building-block of empathy) exists in dogs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elisabetta Palagi
- Natural History Museum, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
- Unit of Cognitive Primatology and Primate Center, ISTC, CNR, Rome, Italy
| | - Velia Nicotra
- Natural History Museum, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
| | - Giada Cordoni
- Natural History Museum, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
145
|
Abstract
Primates live in highly social environments, where prosocial behaviors promote social bonds and cohesion and contribute to group members' fitness. Despite a growing interest in the biological basis of nonhuman primates' social interactions, their underlying motivations remain a matter of debate. We report that macaque monkeys take into account the welfare of their peers when making behavioral choices bringing about pleasant or unpleasant outcomes to a monkey partner. Two macaques took turns in making decisions that could impact their own welfare or their partner's. Most monkeys were inclined to refrain from delivering a mildly aversive airpuff and to grant juice rewards to their partner. Choice consistency between these two types of outcome suggests that monkeys display coherent motivations in different social interactions. Furthermore, spontaneous affilitative group interactions in the home environment were mostly consistent with the measured social decisions, thus emphasizing the impact of preexisting social bonds on decision-making. Interestingly, unique behavioral markers predicted these decisions: benevolence was associated with enhanced mutual gaze and empathic eye blinking, whereas indifference or malevolence was associated with lower or suppressed such responses. Together our results suggest that prosocial decision-making is sustained by an intrinsic motivation for social affiliation and controlled through positive and negative vicarious reinforcements.
Collapse
|
146
|
Freidin E, Carballo F, Bentosela M. Direct reciprocity in animals: The roles of bonding and affective processes. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2015; 52:163-170. [PMID: 26354082 DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12215] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2014] [Revised: 07/23/2015] [Accepted: 08/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
The presence of direct reciprocity in animals is a debated topic, because, despite its evolutionary plausibility, it is believed to be uncommon. Some authors claim that stable reciprocal exchanges require sophisticated cognition which has acted as a constraint on its evolution across species. In contrast, a more recent trend of research has focused on the possibility that direct reciprocity occurs within long-term bonds and relies on simple as well as more complex affective mechanisms such as emotional book-keeping, rudimentary and higher forms of empathy, and inequity aversion, among others. First, we present evidence supporting the occurrence of long-term reciprocity in the context of existing bonds in social birds and mammals. Second, we discuss the evidence for affective responses which, modulated by bonding, may underlie altruistic behaviours in different species. We conclude that the mechanisms that may underlie reciprocal exchanges are diverse, and that some act in interaction with bonding processes. From simple associative learning in social contexts, through emotional contagion and behavioural mimicry, to empathy and a sense of fairness, widespread and diverse social affective mechanisms may explain why direct reciprocity may not be a rare phenomenon among social vertebrates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esteban Freidin
- Grupo de Investigación del Comportamiento en Cánidos (ICOC), Instituto de Investigaciones Médicas "Alfredo Lanari", CONICET/UBA, Buenos Aires, Argentina.,Instituto de Investigaciones Económicas y Sociales del Sur (IIESS), CONICET, Bahía Blanca, Argentina
| | - Fabricio Carballo
- Grupo de Investigación del Comportamiento en Cánidos (ICOC), Instituto de Investigaciones Médicas "Alfredo Lanari", CONICET/UBA, Buenos Aires, Argentina.,Instituto de Investigaciones Biológicas y Biomédicas del Sur (INBIOSUR), CONICET/UNS, Bahía Blanca, Argentina
| | - Mariana Bentosela
- Grupo de Investigación del Comportamiento en Cánidos (ICOC), Instituto de Investigaciones Médicas "Alfredo Lanari", CONICET/UBA, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
147
|
Carrillo M, Migliorati F, Bruls R, Han Y, Heinemans M, Pruis I, Gazzola V, Keysers C. Repeated Witnessing of Conspecifics in Pain: Effects on Emotional Contagion. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136979. [PMID: 26356506 PMCID: PMC4565705 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136979] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2015] [Accepted: 08/11/2015] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Witnessing of conspecifics in pain has been shown to elicit socially triggered freezing in rodents. It is unknown how robust this response is to repeated exposure to a cage-mate experiencing painful stimulation. To address this question, shock-experienced Observer rats repeatedly witnessed familiar Demonstrators receive painful footshocks (six sessions). Results confirm that Observers freeze during the first testing session. The occurrence of this behaviour however gradually diminished as the experimental sessions progressed, reaching minimal freezing levels by the end of the experiments. In contrast, the appearance and continuous increase in the frequency of yawning, a behavior that was inhibited by metyrapone (i.e,. a glucocorticoid synthesis blocker), might represent an alternative coping strategy, suggesting that the observer’s reduced freezing does not necessarily indicate a disappearance in the affective response to the Demonstrator’s distress.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Carrillo
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- * E-mail: (MC); (VG); (CK)
| | - Filippo Migliorati
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Rune Bruls
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Yingying Han
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Mirjam Heinemans
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam (UvA), Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Ilanah Pruis
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Earth and Life Sciences, VU university, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Valeria Gazzola
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam (UvA), Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- * E-mail: (MC); (VG); (CK)
| | - Christian Keysers
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the KNAW, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, University of Amsterdam (UvA), Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- * E-mail: (MC); (VG); (CK)
| |
Collapse
|
148
|
Perez EC, Elie JE, Boucaud ICA, Crouchet T, Soulage CO, Soula HA, Theunissen FE, Vignal C. Physiological resonance between mates through calls as possible evidence of empathic processes in songbirds. Horm Behav 2015; 75:130-41. [PMID: 26407661 DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2014] [Revised: 09/13/2015] [Accepted: 09/18/2015] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Physiological resonance - where the physiological state of a subject generates the same state in a perceiver - has been proposed as a proximate mechanism facilitating pro-social behaviours. While mainly described in mammals, state matching in physiology and behaviour could be a phylogenetically shared trait among social vertebrates. Birds show complex social lives and cognitive abilities, and their monogamous pair-bond is a highly coordinated partnership, therefore we hypothesised that birds express state matching between mates. We show that calls of male zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata produced during corticosterone treatment (after oral administration of exogenous corticosterone and during visual separation from the partner) provoke both an increase in corticosterone concentrations and behavioural changes in their female partner compared to control calls (regular calls emitted by the same male during visual separation from the partner only), whereas calls produced during corticosterone treatment by unfamiliar males have no such effect. Irrespective of the caller status (mate/non-mate), calls' acoustic properties were predictive of female corticosterone concentration after playback, but the identity of mate calls was necessary to fully explain female responses. Female responses were unlikely due to a failure of the call-based mate recognition system: in a discrimination task, females perceive calls produced during corticosterone treatment as being more similar to the control calls of the same male than to control calls of other males, even after taking acoustical differences into account. These results constitute the first evidence of physiological resonance solely on acoustic cues in birds, and support the presence of empathic processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emilie C Perez
- Université de Lyon/Saint-Etienne, Neuro-PSI/ENES CNRS UMR9197, F-42023 Saint-Etienne, France.
| | - Julie E Elie
- University of California, Berkeley, Department of Psychology & Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, CA 94720, USA.
| | - Ingrid C A Boucaud
- Université de Lyon/Saint-Etienne, Neuro-PSI/ENES CNRS UMR9197, F-42023 Saint-Etienne, France.
| | - Thomas Crouchet
- Université de Lyon/Saint-Etienne, Neuro-PSI/ENES CNRS UMR9197, F-42023 Saint-Etienne, France.
| | - Christophe O Soulage
- Université de Lyon, INSA de Lyon, CarMeN, INSERM U1060, F-69621 Villeurbanne, France.
| | - Hédi A Soula
- Université de Lyon, INSA de Lyon, CarMeN, INSERM U1060, F-69621 Villeurbanne, France; EPI Beagle, INRIA Rhône-Alpes, F-69603 Villeurbanne, France.
| | - Frédéric E Theunissen
- University of California, Berkeley, Department of Psychology & Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, CA 94720, USA.
| | - Clémentine Vignal
- Université de Lyon/Saint-Etienne, Neuro-PSI/ENES CNRS UMR9197, F-42023 Saint-Etienne, France.
| |
Collapse
|
149
|
Nakahashi W, Ohtsuki H. When is emotional contagion adaptive? J Theor Biol 2015; 380:480-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2015.06.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2015] [Revised: 06/05/2015] [Accepted: 06/08/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
150
|
Meyza K, Nikolaev T, Kondrakiewicz K, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ, Knapska E. Neuronal correlates of asocial behavior in a BTBR T (+) Itpr3(tf)/J mouse model of autism. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:199. [PMID: 26300749 PMCID: PMC4526814 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2015] [Accepted: 07/13/2015] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized, in part, by an inability to adequately respond to social cues. Patients diagnosed with ASD are often devoid of empathy and impaired in understanding other people's emotional perspective. The neuronal correlates of this impairment are not fully understood. Replicating such a behavioral phenotype in a mouse model of autism would allow us insight into the neuronal background of the problem. Here we tested BTBR T(+)Itpr3(tf)/J (BTBR) and c57BL/6J (B6) mice in two behavioral paradigms: the Transfer of Emotional Information test and the Social Proximity test. In both tests BTBR mice displayed asocial behavior. We analyzed c-Fos protein expression in several brain regions after each of these tests, and found that, unlike B6 mice, BTBR mice react to a stressed cagemate exposure in the Transfer of Emotional Information test with no increase of c-Fos expression in either the prefrontal cortex or the amygdala. However, after Social Proximity exposure we observed a strong increase in c-Fos expression in the CA3 field of the hippocampus and two hypothalamic regions of BTBR brains. This response was accompanied by a strong activation of periaqueductal regions related to defensiveness, which suggests that BTBR mice find unavoidable social interaction highly aversive.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ksenia Meyza
- Laboratory of Emotions' Neurobiology, Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology PAS Warsaw, Poland
| | - Tomasz Nikolaev
- Laboratory of Emotions' Neurobiology, Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology PAS Warsaw, Poland
| | - Kacper Kondrakiewicz
- Laboratory of Emotions' Neurobiology, Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology PAS Warsaw, Poland
| | | | - Robert J Blanchard
- Pacific Biosciences Research Center, University of Hawaii at Manoa Honolulu, HI, USA
| | - Ewelina Knapska
- Laboratory of Emotions' Neurobiology, Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology PAS Warsaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|