101
|
Roth D, Latoschik ME. Construction of the Virtual Embodiment Questionnaire (VEQ). IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS 2020; 26:3546-3556. [PMID: 32941148 DOI: 10.1109/tvcg.2020.3023603] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
User embodiment is important for many virtual reality (VR) applications, for example, in the context of social interaction, therapy, training, or entertainment. However, there is no data-driven and validated instrument to empirically measure the perceptual aspects of embodiment, necessary to reliably evaluate this important phenomenon. To provide a method to assess components of virtual embodiment in a reliable and consistent fashion, we constructed a Virtual Embodiment Questionnaire (VEQ). We reviewed previous literature to identify applicable constructs and questionnaire items, and performed a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on the data from three experiments ( N=196). The analysis confirmed three factors: (1) ownership of a virtual body, (2) agency over a virtual body, and (3) the perceived change in the body schema. A fourth study ( N=22) was conducted to confirm the reliability and validity of the scale, by investigating the impacts of latency and latency jitter present in the simulation. We present the proposed scale and study results and discuss resulting implications.
Collapse
|
102
|
Guterstam A, Larsson DEO, Szczotka J, Ehrsson HH. Duplication of the bodily self: a perceptual illusion of dual full-body ownership and dual self-location. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2020; 7:201911. [PMID: 33489299 PMCID: PMC7813251 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.201911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2020] [Accepted: 11/02/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Previous research has shown that it is possible to use multisensory stimulation to induce the perceptual illusion of owning supernumerary limbs, such as two right arms. However, it remains unclear whether the coherent feeling of owning a full-body may be duplicated in the same manner and whether such a dual full-body illusion could be used to split the unitary sense of self-location into two. Here, we examined whether healthy human participants can experience simultaneous ownership of two full-bodies, located either close in parallel or in two separate spatial locations. A previously described full-body illusion, based on visuo-tactile stimulation of an artificial body viewed from the first-person perspective (1PP) via head-mounted displays, was adapted to a dual-body setting and quantified in five experiments using questionnaires, a behavioural self-location task and threat-evoked skin conductance responses. The results of experiments 1-3 showed that synchronous visuo-tactile stimulation of two bodies viewed from the 1PP lying in parallel next to each other induced a significant illusion of dual full-body ownership. In experiment 4, we failed to find support for our working hypothesis that splitting the visual scene into two, so that each of the two illusory bodies was placed in distinct spatial environments, would lead to dual self-location. In a final exploratory experiment (no. 5), we found preliminary support for an illusion of dual self-location and dual body ownership by using dynamic changes between the 1PPs of two artificial bodies and/or a common third-person perspective in the ceiling of the testing room. These findings suggest that healthy people, under certain conditions of multisensory perceptual ambiguity, may experience dual body ownership and dual self-location. These findings suggest that the coherent sense of the bodily self located at a single place in space is the result of an active and dynamic perceptual integration process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arvid Guterstam
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | | | - Joanna Szczotka
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - H. Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
103
|
Barra J, Giroux M, Metral M, Cian C, Luyat M, Kavounoudias A, Guerraz M. Functional properties of extended body representations in the context of kinesthesia. Neurophysiol Clin 2020; 50:455-465. [PMID: 33176990 DOI: 10.1016/j.neucli.2020.10.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2020] [Revised: 10/16/2020] [Accepted: 10/16/2020] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
A person's internal representation of his/her body is not fixed. It can be substantially modified by neurological injuries and can also be extended (in healthy participants) to incorporate objects that have a corporeal appearance (such as fake body segments, e.g. a rubber hand), virtual whole bodies (e.g. avatars), and even objects that do not have a corporeal appearance (e.g. tools). Here, we report data from patients and healthy participants that emphasize the flexible nature of body representation and question the extent to which incorporated objects have the same functional properties as biological body parts. Our data shed new light by highlighting the involvement of visual motion information from incorporated objects (rubber hands, full body avatars and hand-held tools) in the perception of one's own movement (kinesthesia). On the basis of these findings, we argue that incorporated objects can be treated as body parts, especially when kinesthesia is involved.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julien Barra
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Marion Giroux
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Morgane Metral
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, LIP/PC2S, Grenoble, France
| | - Corinne Cian
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France; Institut de Recherche Biomédicale des Armées, Brétigny sur Orge, France
| | - Marion Luyat
- Univ. Lille, URL 4072 - PSITEC - Psychologie : Interactions, Temps, Emotions, Cognition, F-59000 Lille, France
| | - Anne Kavounoudias
- Aix-Marseille University, CNRS, LNSC UMR 7260, F-13331 Marseille, France
| | - Michel Guerraz
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France.
| |
Collapse
|
104
|
Chancel M, Ehrsson HH. Which hand is mine? Discriminating body ownership perception in a two-alternative forced-choice task. Atten Percept Psychophys 2020; 82:4058-4083. [PMID: 32856222 PMCID: PMC7593318 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-020-02107-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
The experience of one's body as one's own is referred to as the sense of body ownership. This central part of human conscious experience determines the boundary between the self and the external environment, a crucial distinction in perception, action, and cognition. Although body ownership is known to involve the integration of signals from multiple sensory modalities, including vision, touch, and proprioception, little is known about the principles that determine this integration process, and the relationship between body ownership and perception is unclear. These uncertainties stem from the lack of a sensitive and rigorous method to quantify body ownership. Here, we describe a two-alternative forced-choice discrimination task that allows precise and direct measurement of body ownership as participants decide which of two rubber hands feels more like their own in a version of the rubber hand illusion. In two experiments, we show that the temporal and spatial congruence principles of multisensory stimulation, which determine ownership discrimination, impose tighter constraints than previously thought and that texture congruence constitutes an additional principle; these findings are compatible with theoretical models of multisensory integration. Taken together, our results suggest that body ownership constitutes a genuine perceptual multisensory phenomenon that can be quantified with psychophysics in discrimination experiments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie Chancel
- Department of Neuroscience, Brain, Body and Self Laboratory, Karolinska Institute, SE-171 77, Stockholm, Sweden.
| | - H Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Brain, Body and Self Laboratory, Karolinska Institute, SE-171 77, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
105
|
Tacikowski P, Weijs ML, Ehrsson HH. Perception of Our Own Body Influences Self-Concept and Self-Incoherence Impairs Episodic Memory. iScience 2020; 23:101429. [PMID: 32853552 PMCID: PMC7520895 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.101429] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2020] [Revised: 07/08/2020] [Accepted: 07/30/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
How does our body affect the way we think about our personality? We addressed this question by eliciting the perceptual illusion that pairs of friends swapped bodies with each other. We found that during the illusion, the participants rated their own personality characteristics more similarly to the way they previously rated their friend's personality, and this flexible adjustment of self-concept to the "new" bodily self was related to the strength of illusory ownership of the friend's body. Moreover, a subsequent memory test showed that personality traits rated during the friend-body-swap illusion were generally remembered worse than traits rated during the control conditions; importantly, however, this impairment of episodic recognition memory was reduced for the participants who considerably adjusted their self-concept during the illusory body swapping. These findings demonstrate that our beliefs about own personality are dynamically shaped by the perception of our body and that coherence between the bodily and conceptual self-representations is important for the normal encoding of episodic memories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pawel Tacikowski
- Karolinska Institutet, Department of Neuroscience, Solnavägen 9, 17165 Stockholm, Sweden
- University of California Los Angeles, Department of Neurosurgery, 300 Stein Plaza Drwy., 90024 Los Angeles, USA
| | - Marieke L. Weijs
- Karolinska Institutet, Department of Neuroscience, Solnavägen 9, 17165 Stockholm, Sweden
- University of Zurich, Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology, Binzmühlestrasse 14, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland
| | - H. Henrik Ehrsson
- Karolinska Institutet, Department of Neuroscience, Solnavägen 9, 17165 Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
106
|
Horváth Á, Ferentzi E, Bogdány T, Szolcsányi T, Witthöft M, Köteles F. Proprioception but not cardiac interoception is related to the rubber hand illusion. Cortex 2020; 132:361-373. [PMID: 33031978 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.08.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2020] [Revised: 07/07/2020] [Accepted: 08/24/2020] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
The rubber hand illusion (RHI) is a widely used tool in the study of multisensory integration. It develops as the interaction of temporally consistent visual and tactile input, which can overwrite proprioceptive information. Theoretically, the accuracy of proprioception may influence the proneness to the RHI but this has received little research attention to date. Concerning the role of cardioceptive information, the available empirical evidence is equivocal. The current study aimed to test the impact of proprioceptive and cardioceptive input on the RHI. 60 undergraduate students (32 females) completed sensory tasks assessing proprioceptive accuracy with respect to the angle of the elbow joint, a heartbeat tracking task assessing cardioceptive accuracy (the Schandry-task) and the RHI. We found that those with more consistent joint position judgements (i.e., less variable error) in the proprioceptive task were less prone to the illusion, particularly with respect to disembodiment ratings in the asynchronous condition. Systematic error, indicating a systematic distortion in position judgements influenced the illusion in the synchronous condition. Participants with more proprioceptive bias toward the direction of the rubber hand in the proprioceptive test reported a stronger felt embodiment. The results are in accordance with Bayesian causal inference models of multisensory integration. Cardioceptive accuracy, however, was not associated with the strength of the illusion. We concluded that individual differences in proprioceptive processing impact the RHI, while cardioceptive accuracy is unrelated to it. Theoretical and practical relevance of the findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Áron Horváth
- Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary; Institute of Health Promotion and Sport Sciences, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary.
| | - Eszter Ferentzi
- Institute of Health Promotion and Sport Sciences, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary
| | - Tamás Bogdány
- Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary; Institute of Health Promotion and Sport Sciences, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary
| | - Tibor Szolcsányi
- Institute of Behavioral Sciences, Medical Faculty, University of Pécs, Hungary
| | - Michael Witthöft
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychotherapy, and Experimental Psychopathology, University of Mainz, Germany
| | - Ferenc Köteles
- Institute of Health Promotion and Sport Sciences, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
107
|
Proprioceptive drift is affected by the intermanual distance rather than the distance from the body's midline in the rubber hand illusion. Atten Percept Psychophys 2020; 82:4084-4095. [PMID: 32914338 PMCID: PMC7593391 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-020-02119-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In the rubber hand illusion (RHI), simultaneous brush stroking of a subject’s hidden hand and a visible rubber hand induces a transient illusion of the latter to “feel like it’s my hand” and a proprioceptive drift of the hidden own hand toward the rubber hand. Recent accounts of the RHI have suggested that the illusion would only occur if weighting of conflicting sensory information and their subsequent integration results in a statistically plausible compromise. In three different experiments, we investigated the role of distance between the two hands as well as their proximity to the body’s midline in influencing the occurrence of the illusion. Overall, the results suggest that the illusion is abolished when placing the two hands apart, therefore increasing the mismatch between the visual and proprioceptive modality, whereas the proximity of the two hands to the body’s midline plays only a minor role on the subjective report of the illusion. This might be driven by the response properties of visuotactile bimodal cells encoding the peripersonal space around the hand.
Collapse
|
108
|
Having a live huntsman spider on a rubber hand does not modulate the rubber-hand illusion in a top-down manner. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238:2053-2066. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05869-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2019] [Accepted: 06/18/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
109
|
Tacikowski P, Fust J, Ehrsson HH. Fluidity of gender identity induced by illusory body-sex change. Sci Rep 2020; 10:14385. [PMID: 32873869 PMCID: PMC7463009 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-71467-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2019] [Accepted: 08/11/2020] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Gender identity is a collection of thoughts and feelings about one’s own gender, which may or may not correspond to the sex assigned at birth. How this sense is linked to the perception of one’s own masculine or feminine body remains unclear. Here, in a series of three behavioral experiments conducted on a large group of control volunteers (N = 140), we show that a perceptual illusion of having the opposite-sex body is associated with a shift toward a more balanced identification with both genders and less gender-stereotypical beliefs about own personality characteristics, as indicated by subjective reports and implicit behavioral measures. These findings demonstrate that the ongoing perception of one’s own body affects the sense of one’s own gender in a dynamic, robust, and automatic manner.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pawel Tacikowski
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden. .,Department of Neurosurgery, Univeristy of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, USA.
| | - Jens Fust
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden.,Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - H Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
110
|
Waldmann A, Volkmann J, Zeller D. Parkinson's disease may reduce sensitivity to visual-tactile asynchrony irrespective of dopaminergic treatment: Evidence from the rubber hand illusion. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2020; 78:100-104. [PMID: 32805636 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2020.07.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2019] [Revised: 07/08/2020] [Accepted: 07/16/2020] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The feeling of body ownership relies on the binding of multisensory body-related signals. Various sensory abnormalities have been described in Parkinson's disease (PD). OBJECTIVE To assess the rubber hand illusion (RHI) in patients with PD (PwPD) and age-matched healthy controls (CTRL). To evaluate the influence of the dopaminergic system in a PwPD subgroup OFF medication. METHODS The RHI paradigm was applied to 42 PwPD and 48 CTRL. In this experimental setup, stroking a visible plastic hand simultaneously with the covered real hand elicits the feeling of ownership over the seen hand. Asynchronous stroking served as a control condition. Proprioceptive bias and an illusion score based on a questionnaire were used as measures of the RHI. Seventeen PwPD additionally underwent the experiments "OFF medication". RESULTS Compared to CTRL, PwPD showed higher proprioceptive bias independent of the stroking condition (p = 0.015), and had higher illusion scores in the asynchronous condition (p < 0.05). In PwPD, there were no significant differences between ON- and OFF-medication state. CONCLUSION In PwPD, responses to the RHI are less specific with respect to the degree of synchronicity of brushstrokes. This might be attributed to a less stable body representation, internal "noise" during multisensory integration, or a blur of temporal discrimination in PD. The fact that RHI measures did not differ between ON- and OFF-medication states indicates an involvement of non-dopaminergic transmitter systems in this finding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amelie Waldmann
- Dept. of Neurology, University of Würzburg, 97080, Würzburg, Germany.
| | - Jens Volkmann
- Dept. of Neurology, University of Würzburg, 97080, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Daniel Zeller
- Dept. of Neurology, University of Würzburg, 97080, Würzburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
111
|
Debats NB, Heuer H. Exploring the time window for causal inference and the multisensory integration of actions and their visual effects. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2020; 7:192056. [PMID: 32968497 PMCID: PMC7481684 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.192056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2019] [Accepted: 07/13/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Successful computer use requires the operator to link the movement of the cursor to that of his or her hand. Previous studies suggest that the brain establishes this perceptual link through multisensory integration, whereby the causality evidence that drives the integration is provided by the correlated hand and cursor movement trajectories. Here, we explored the temporal window during which this causality evidence is effective. We used a basic cursor-control task, in which participants performed out-and-back reaching movements with their hand on a digitizer tablet. A corresponding cursor movement could be shown on a monitor, yet slightly rotated by an angle that varied from trial to trial. Upon completion of the backward movement, participants judged the endpoint of the outward hand or cursor movement. The mutually biased judgements that typically result reflect the integration of the proprioceptive information on hand endpoint with the visual information on cursor endpoint. We here manipulated the time period during which the cursor was visible, thereby selectively providing causality evidence either before or after sensory information regarding the to-be-judged movement endpoint was available. Specifically, the cursor was visible either during the outward or backward hand movement (conditions Out and Back, respectively). Our data revealed reduced integration in the condition Back compared with the condition Out, suggesting that causality evidence available before the to-be-judged movement endpoint is more powerful than later evidence in determining how strongly the brain integrates the endpoint information. This finding further suggests that sensory integration is not delayed until a judgement is requested.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nienke B. Debats
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Universität Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany
- Cognitive Interaction Technology Center of Excellence (CITEC), Universität Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Herbert Heuer
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Universität Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany
- Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors, Dortmund, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
112
|
Uhlmann L, Pazen M, van Kemenade BM, Steinsträter O, Harris LR, Kircher T, Straube B. Seeing your own or someone else's hand moving in accordance with your action: The neural interaction of agency and hand identity. Hum Brain Mapp 2020; 41:2474-2489. [PMID: 32090439 PMCID: PMC7268012 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.24958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2019] [Revised: 01/17/2020] [Accepted: 02/11/2020] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Forward models can predict sensory consequences of self-action, which is reflected by less neural processing for actively than passively generated sensory inputs (BOLD suppression effect). However, it remains open whether forward models take the identity of a moving body part into account when predicting the sensory consequences of an action. In the current study, fMRI was used to investigate the neural correlates of active and passive hand movements during which participants saw either an on-line display of their own hand or someone else's hand moving in accordance with their movement. Participants had to detect delays (0-417 ms) between their movement and the displays. Analyses revealed reduced activation in sensory areas and higher delay detection thresholds for active versus passive movements. Furthermore, there was increased activation in the hippocampus, the amygdala, and the middle temporal gyrus when someone else's hand was seen. Most importantly, in posterior parietal (angular gyrus and precuneus), frontal (middle, superior, and medial frontal gyrus), and temporal (middle temporal gyrus) regions, suppression for actively versus passively generated feedback was stronger when participants were viewing their own compared to someone else's hand. Our results suggest that forward models can take hand identity into account when predicting sensory action consequences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lukas Uhlmann
- Department of Psychiatry and PsychotherapyUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB)University of MarburgMarburgGermany
| | - Mareike Pazen
- Department of Psychiatry and PsychotherapyUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB)University of MarburgMarburgGermany
| | - Bianca M. van Kemenade
- Department of Psychiatry and PsychotherapyUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB)University of MarburgMarburgGermany
| | - Olaf Steinsträter
- Department of Psychiatry and PsychotherapyUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
- Core Facility Brain ImagingUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
| | | | - Tilo Kircher
- Department of Psychiatry and PsychotherapyUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB)University of MarburgMarburgGermany
| | - Benjamin Straube
- Department of Psychiatry and PsychotherapyUniversity of MarburgMarburgGermany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior (CMBB)University of MarburgMarburgGermany
| |
Collapse
|
113
|
Serino S, Scarpina F, Chirico A, Dakanalis A, Di Lernia D, Colombo D, Catallo V, Pedroli E, Riva G. Gulliver's virtual travels: active embodiment in extreme body sizes for modulating our body representations. Cogn Process 2020; 21:509-520. [PMID: 32506217 PMCID: PMC7679308 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-020-00977-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2019] [Accepted: 04/27/2020] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
It is noted that the perceptual experience of body and space can be modulated by changing the action capabilities or by manipulating the perceived body dimensions through a multisensory stimulation. This study adds to pre-existing literature by investigating the alterations in bodily experience following embodiment to both enlarged and shrunked bodies, while participants actively navigated in a virtual environment. A normal-sized body served as a reference condition. After each embodied navigation, participants estimated the height and width of three different body parts. Results revealed that the embodiment over shrunked body induced a significant reduction in participants' body image, while no changes were reported after the embodiment over the enlarged body. Findings were discussed in terms of previous literature exploring the constraints implicated in the ownership over different bodies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Serino
- Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Via Magnasco 2, 20149, Milan, Italy.
- Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Gemelli, 1, 20100, Milan, Italy.
- MySpace Lab, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, University Hospital Lausanne (CHUV), Avenue Pierre Decker 5 CH-1011, Lausanne, Switzerland.
| | - F Scarpina
- Istituto Auxologico Italiano, IRCCS, U.Odi Neurologia e Neuoriabilitazione, Ospedale San Giuseppe, Via Cadorna, 90, 28824, Piancavallo (VCO), Italy
- "Rita Levi Montalcini" Department of Neuroscience, University of Turin, Via Cherasco, 15, 10126, Turin, Italy
| | - A Chirico
- Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Gemelli, 1, 20100, Milan, Italy
| | - A Dakanalis
- Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia Italy, Piazza Botta, 6, 27100, Pavia, Italy
- Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Milano Bicocca, Via Cadore 48, 20900, Monza, Italy
| | - D Di Lernia
- Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Gemelli, 1, 20100, Milan, Italy
| | - D Colombo
- Department of Basic Psychology, Clinic and Psychobiology, Universitat Jaume I, Av. Sos Baynat, s/n, 12071, Castellón, Spain
| | - V Catallo
- Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Via Magnasco 2, 20149, Milan, Italy
| | - E Pedroli
- Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Via Magnasco 2, 20149, Milan, Italy
- Faculty of Psychology, eCampus University, Via Isimbardi, 10, 22060, Novedrate, Italy
| | - G Riva
- Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Via Magnasco 2, 20149, Milan, Italy
- Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Gemelli, 1, 20100, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
114
|
Beaudoin M, Barra J, Dupraz L, Mollier-Sabet P, Guerraz M. The impact of embodying an "elderly" body avatar on motor imagery. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238:1467-1478. [PMID: 32417944 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05828-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2020] [Accepted: 05/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
When an individual embodies an avatar, the latter's characteristics or stereotype can change the individual's behavior and attitudes; this is known as the Proteus effect. Here, we looked at whether the embodiment of an avatar resembling an elderly adult (seen from a first-person perspective and facing a virtual mirror) changed mentally represented physical activity in a motor imagery task performed by young adult participants (N = 52). To ensure that the impact of embodiment of an elderly avatar on the motor imagery task was not influenced by a potentially confounded stereotype assimilation effect (due to the mere presence of an avatar), a "young" avatar and an "elderly" avatar were always present together in the virtual environment-even though only one (the self-avatar) was embodied at a given time. We found that it took longer for the participants to perform the motor imagery task with the elderly self-avatar than with the young self-avatar. The more negative the participant's beliefs about motor activity in the elderly, the greater the observed effect of the avatar on motor imagery performance. We conclude that knowledge about the characteristics of an embodied avatar can modify the subject's level of mentally represented physical activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marine Beaudoin
- University of Savoie Mont Blanc, Chambéry, France
- University of Grenoble Alpes, University of Savoie Mont Blanc, LIP\PC2S, 38000, Grenoble, France
| | - Julien Barra
- University of Savoie Mont Blanc, Chambéry, France
- University of Grenoble Alpes, University of Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000, Grenoble, France
| | | | | | - Michel Guerraz
- University of Savoie Mont Blanc, Chambéry, France.
- University of Grenoble Alpes, University of Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000, Grenoble, France.
| |
Collapse
|
115
|
Guterstam A, Collins KL, Cronin JA, Zeberg H, Darvas F, Weaver KE, Ojemann JG, Ehrsson HH. Direct Electrophysiological Correlates of Body Ownership in Human Cerebral Cortex. Cereb Cortex 2020; 29:1328-1341. [PMID: 30496342 PMCID: PMC6373693 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhy285] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2018] [Accepted: 10/23/2018] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Over the past decade, numerous neuroimaging studies based on hemodynamic markers of brain activity have examined the feeling of body ownership using perceptual body-illusions in humans. However, the direct electrophysiological correlates of body ownership at the cortical level remain unexplored. To address this, we studied the rubber hand illusion in 5 patients (3 males and 2 females) implanted with intracranial electrodes measuring cortical surface potentials. Increased high-γ (70–200 Hz) activity, an index of neuronal firing rate, in premotor and intraparietal cortices reflected the feeling of ownership. In both areas, high-γ increases were intimately coupled with the subjective illusion onset and sustained both during and in-between touches. However, intraparietal activity was modulated by tactile stimulation to a higher degree than the premotor cortex through effective connectivity with the hand-somatosensory cortex, which suggests different functional roles. These findings constitute the first intracranial electrophysiological characterization of the rubber hand illusion and extend our understanding of the dynamic mechanisms of body ownership.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arvid Guterstam
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, New Jersey, USA.,Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Kelly L Collins
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Jeneva A Cronin
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Hugo Zeberg
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Felix Darvas
- Departments Biomedical Engineering and Radiology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Kurt E Weaver
- Department of Radiology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA.,Integrated Brain Imaging Center, UW Radiology, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - Jeffrey G Ojemann
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
| | - H Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
116
|
Gonzalez-Liencres C, Zapata LE, Iruretagoyena G, Seinfeld S, Perez-Mendez L, Arroyo-Palacios J, Borland D, Slater M, Sanchez-Vives MV. Being the Victim of Intimate Partner Violence in Virtual Reality: First- Versus Third-Person Perspective. Front Psychol 2020; 11:820. [PMID: 32457681 PMCID: PMC7225265 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00820] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2019] [Accepted: 04/02/2020] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Immersive virtual reality is widely used for research and clinical purposes. Here we explored the impact of an immersive virtual scene of intimate partner violence experienced from the victim’s perspective (first person), as opposed to witnessing it as an observer (third person). We are ultimately interested in the potential of this approach to rehabilitate batterers and in understanding the mechanisms underlying this process. For this, non-offender men experienced the scene either from the perspective of the victim’s virtual body (a female avatar), which moved synchronously with the participants’ real movements, or from the perspective of an observer, while we recorded their behavior and physiological responses. We also evaluated through questionnaires, interviews and implicit association tests their subjective impressions and potential pre/post changes in implicit gender bias following the experience. We found that in all participants, regardless of perspective, the magnitude of the physiological reactions to virtual threatening stimuli was related to how vulnerable they felt for being a woman, the sensation that they could be assaulted, how useful the scene could be for batterer rehabilitation, and how different it would have been to experience the scenario on TV. Furthermore, we found that their level of identification with the female avatar correlated with the decrease in prejudice against women. Although the first-person perspective (1PP) facilitated taking the scene personally, generated a sensation of fear, helplessness, and vulnerability, and tended to induce greater behavioral and physiological reactions, we show that the potential for batterer rehabilitation originates from presence and identification with the victim, which in turn is more easily, but not exclusively, achieved through 1PP. This study is relevant for the development of advanced virtual reality tools for clinical purposes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Luis E Zapata
- Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.,Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Guillermo Iruretagoyena
- Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.,Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Sofia Seinfeld
- Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.,Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | - Jorge Arroyo-Palacios
- Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.,Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - David Borland
- Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.,Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Mel Slater
- Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Maria V Sanchez-Vives
- Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer, Barcelona, Spain.,Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Department of Cognition, Development and Educational Science, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,ICREA, Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
117
|
Mine D, Ogawa N, Narumi T, Yokosawa K. The relationship between the body and the environment in the virtual world: The interpupillary distance affects the body size perception. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0232290. [PMID: 32330199 PMCID: PMC7182220 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232290] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2019] [Accepted: 04/11/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research suggests that the size of one's body is used as a metric to scale the external world. On the other hand, the influence of information from the external world on the perception of body size is unclear. It has been suggested that increased inter-pupillary distance (IPD) leads people to perceive the external world as smaller than it actually is. The present study investigated the effect of the IPD on body size perception, and the relationship between the perceived scale of the body and the external world when the IPD is manipulated. To this end, in a virtual environment, we manipulated the IPD as well as the size and presence of participants' hands, while participant's eye height was increased vertically. Results showed that, when participants' eye height was increased and their hands were enlarged, people with a fixed IPD perceived the size of their body to be large (like a giant) while the external world was perceived to be changed minimally. Alternatively, people with increased IPD perceived that the external world as having shrank, whereas their perception of their body size changed little. However, when a viewers' virtual hands were not shown, the IPD did not affect the individual's percept of body size, although the IPD did affect one's perception of the external world. These results suggest that, when the ratio of the size between one's body and the external world are explicit, the perceived size of one's body is affected by the IPD or perceived scale of the external world that is affected by the IPD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daisuke Mine
- Department of Psychology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- * E-mail:
| | - Nami Ogawa
- Graduate School of Engineering, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Takuji Narumi
- Graduate School of Information Science and Technology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- JST PRESTO, Tokyo, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
118
|
French RL, DeAngelis GC. Multisensory neural processing: from cue integration to causal inference. CURRENT OPINION IN PHYSIOLOGY 2020; 16:8-13. [PMID: 32968701 DOI: 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Neurophysiological studies of multisensory processing have largely focused on how the brain integrates information from different sensory modalities to form a coherent percept. However, in the natural environment, an important extra step is needed: the brain faces the problem of causal inference, which involves determining whether different sources of sensory information arise from the same environmental cause, such that integrating them is advantageous Behavioral and computational studies have provided a strong foundation for studying causal inference, but studies of its neural basis have only recently been undertaken. This review focuses on recent advances regarding how the brain infers the causes of sensory inputs and uses this information to make robust perceptual estimates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ranran L French
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY
| | - Gregory C DeAngelis
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY
| |
Collapse
|
119
|
Fiorio M, Modenese M, Cesari P. The rubber hand illusion in hypnosis provides new insights into the sense of body ownership. Sci Rep 2020; 10:5706. [PMID: 32235881 PMCID: PMC7109052 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-62745-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2019] [Accepted: 03/16/2020] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Body ownership can be experimentally investigated with the rubber hand illusion (RHI), in which watching a rubber hand stroked synchronously with one’s own hidden hand induces a feeling of ownership over the rubber hand. The aim of this study was to investigate response to the RHI in high (N = 21) and low (N = 19) hypnotizable individuals in normal waking state and in hypnosis. Response to the RHI was measured via a question on the illusory feeling of ownership and with proprioceptive drift. The Highs expressed an overall feeling of more ownership over the rubber hand in both the normal waking state and hypnosis, although both groups gave higher ownership scores after synchronous than after asynchronous stroking and the difference between conditions was similar across groups. Conversely, the proprioceptive drift appeared to be differentially modulated by hypnosis and hypnotic suggestibility: it was increased in the Highs and decreased in the Lows after hypnosis induction. These findings hint at an interplay between hypnotic suggestibility and hypnosis in modulating response to the RHI. The selective breakdown of proprioceptive drift among the Lows suggests resistance to recalibrate one’s own limb in hypnosis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mirta Fiorio
- Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy.
| | - Michele Modenese
- Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
| | - Paola Cesari
- Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
120
|
Roel Lesur M, Weijs ML, Simon C, Kannape OA, Lenggenhager B. Psychometrics of Disembodiment and Its Differential Modulation by Visuomotor and Visuotactile Mismatches. iScience 2020; 23:100901. [PMID: 32109678 PMCID: PMC7044746 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.100901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2019] [Revised: 11/12/2019] [Accepted: 01/20/2020] [Indexed: 10/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Altered states of embodiment are fundamental to the scientific understanding of bodily self consciousness. The feeling of disembodiment during everyday activities is common to clinical conditions; however, the direct study of disembodiment in experimental setups is rare compared to the extensive investigation of illusory embodiment of an external object. Using mixed reality to modulate embodiment through temporally mismatching sensory signals from the own body, we assessed how such mismatches affect phenomenal and physiological aspects of embodiment and measured perceptual thresholds for these across multimodal signals. The results of a principal component analysis suggest that multimodal mismatches generally induce disembodiment by increasing the sense of disownership and deafference and decreasing embodiment; however, this was not generally reflected in physiological changes. Although visual delay decreased embodiment both during active movement and passive touch, the effect was stronger for the former. We discuss the relevance of these findings for understanding bodily self plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marte Roel Lesur
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
| | - Marieke Lieve Weijs
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Colin Simon
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Oliver Alan Kannape
- School of Psychology, University of Central Lancashire, Preston PR1 2HE, UK; Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Center for Neuroprosthetics and Brain Mind Institute, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Bigna Lenggenhager
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Neuropsychology with Focus on Body, Self, and Plasticity, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
121
|
Toet A, Kuling IA, Krom BN, van Erp JBF. Toward Enhanced Teleoperation Through Embodiment. Front Robot AI 2020; 7:14. [PMID: 33501183 PMCID: PMC7805894 DOI: 10.3389/frobt.2020.00014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2019] [Accepted: 01/21/2020] [Indexed: 12/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Telerobotics aims to transfer human manipulation skills and dexterity over an arbitrary distance and at an arbitrary scale to a remote workplace. A telerobotic system that is transparent enables a natural and intuitive interaction. We postulate that embodiment (with three sub-components: sense of ownership, agency, and self-location) of the robotic system leads to optimal perceptual transparency and increases task performance. However, this has not yet been investigated directly. We reason along four premises and present findings from the literature that substantiate each of them: (1) the brain can embody non-bodily objects (e.g., robotic hands), (2) embodiment can be elicited with mediated sensorimotor interaction, (3) embodiment is robust against inconsistencies between the robotic system and the operator's body, and (4) embodiment positively correlates to dexterous task performance. We use the predictive encoding theory as a framework to interpret and discuss the results reported in the literature. Numerous previous studies have shown that it is possible to induce embodiment over a wide range of virtual and real extracorporeal objects (including artificial limbs, avatars, and android robots) through mediated sensorimotor interaction. Also, embodiment can occur for non-human morphologies including for elongated arms and a tail. In accordance with the predictive encoding theory, none of the sensory modalities is critical in establishing ownership, and discrepancies in multisensory signals do not necessarily lead to loss of embodiment. However, large discrepancies in terms of multisensory synchrony or visual likeness can prohibit embodiment from occurring. The literature provides less extensive support for the link between embodiment and (dexterous) task performance. However, data gathered with prosthetic hands do indicate a positive correlation. We conclude that all four premises are supported by direct or indirect evidence in the literature, suggesting that embodiment of a remote manipulator may improve dexterous performance in telerobotics. This warrants further implementation testing of embodiment in telerobotics. We formulate a first set of guidelines to apply embodiment in telerobotics and identify some important research topics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Toet
- Perceptual and Cognitive Systems, Netherlands Organisation for Applied Scientific Research (TNO), Soesterberg, Netherlands
| | - Irene A. Kuling
- Perceptual and Cognitive Systems, Netherlands Organisation for Applied Scientific Research (TNO), Soesterberg, Netherlands
| | - Bouke N. Krom
- Intelligent Autonomous Systems, Netherlands Organisation for Applied Scientific Research (TNO), The Hague, Netherlands
| | - Jan B. F. van Erp
- Perceptual and Cognitive Systems, Netherlands Organisation for Applied Scientific Research (TNO), Soesterberg, Netherlands
- Human Media Interaction, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
122
|
Swinkels LM, van Schie HT, Veling H, ter Horst AC, Dijksterhuis A. The self-generated full body illusion is accompanied by impaired detection of somatosensory stimuli. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2020; 203:102987. [PMID: 31923880 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102987] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2019] [Revised: 12/05/2019] [Accepted: 12/18/2019] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Research has found that body illusions may be accompanied by consequences for the real body whereby various somatosensory and homeostatic bodily functions may be impaired. These findings stem from research where an experimenter induced the body illusions. In line with advances in the domains of videogames and virtual reality where the real body is used as a controller we investigate if these consequences also accompany self-generated body illusions. In two preregistered experiments we made use of a head-mounted display set-up to induce the full body illusion (FBI) whereby touch is felt to originate from a 3PP body, and examined effects in the simple detection of supra-threshold vibrotactile stimuli presented to the participants' back and head. Results of both experiments indicate that it is possible to induce a FBI through self-stroking of the neck and that the FBI is accompanied by reduced accuracy and delayed reaction times in detection of somatosensory stimuli. In an additional preregistered control experiment the alternative explanation that a difference in motion presented in the conditions was responsible for these findings was ruled out. Our findings corroborate previous studies that have found body illusions to be accompanied by bodily consequences and further extend these findings to the domain of self-induced body illusions. These results are relevant for video games and VR setups that are geared towards virtual embodiment as they advance our understanding of the conditions and mechanisms in which bodily consequences may express themselves.
Collapse
|
123
|
Buckmaster CL, Rathmann-Bloch JE, de Lecea L, Schatzberg AF, Lyons DM. Multisensory modulation of body ownership in mice. Neurosci Conscious 2020; 2020:niz019. [PMID: 31988796 PMCID: PMC6977007 DOI: 10.1093/nc/niz019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2019] [Revised: 11/22/2019] [Accepted: 12/01/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Body ownership is a fundamental aspect of self-consciousness that reflects more than the presence of physical body parts. As demonstrated by the rubber hand illusion (RHI), human brains construct body ownership experiences using available multisensory information. Experimental conditions similar to those that induce the RHI in humans have been recently adapted to induce the rubber tail illusion (RTI) in mice. Here, we show that the RTI is enhanced in both sexes of mice by repetitive synchronous stroking comprised of correlated visual and tactile stimulation of real and rubber tails compared to visual-only mimicked stroking conducted without tactile stimulation. The RTI also appears to be enhanced in female but not male mice by slow compared to fast stroking that reflects an interoceptive manipulation associated with affective touch in humans. Sex differences in slow stroking effects are exploratory and require replication in mice. Sex differences have not been reported for the RHI in healthy humans, but women rate slow stroking as more affectively pleasant compared to the ratings of men. Results suggest that the RHI in humans resembles aspects of the RTI in mice. Studies of mice may therefore provide neurobiological insights on evolutionarily conserved mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine L Buckmaster
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
| | - Julia E Rathmann-Bloch
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
| | - Luis de Lecea
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
| | - Alan F Schatzberg
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
| | - David M Lyons
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
124
|
Krol SA, Thériault R, Olson JA, Raz A, Bartz JA. Self-Concept Clarity and the Bodily Self: Malleability Across Modalities. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2019; 46:808-820. [DOI: 10.1177/0146167219879126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The self has fascinated scholars for centuries. Although theory suggests that the self-concept (cognitive self-understanding) and bodily self (pre-reflective awareness of one’s body) are related, little work has examined this notion. To this end, in Study 1, participants reported on self-concept clarity (SCC) and completed the rubber hand illusion (RHI), a paradigm in which synchronous (vs. asynchronous) stimulation between a prosthetic hand and one’s own hand leads one to “embody” the prosthetic hand. Whereas participants were equally susceptible to the RHI during synchronous stroking, low-SCC individuals were more vulnerable to the illusion during asynchronous stroking, when the effect is unwarranted. Conceptually replicating and extending this finding, in Study 2, low-SCC individuals were more susceptible to the body-swap illusion—the impression that another person’s body is one’s own. These findings suggest that a clear sense of self implies clarity and stability of both the self-concept and the bodily self.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Rémi Thériault
- McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
- Chapman University, Irvine, CA, USA
| | | | - Amir Raz
- McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
- Chapman University, Irvine, CA, USA
- Jewish General Hospital, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
125
|
Borrego A, Latorre J, Alcañiz M, Llorens R. Embodiment and Presence in Virtual Reality After Stroke. A Comparative Study With Healthy Subjects. Front Neurol 2019; 10:1061. [PMID: 31649608 PMCID: PMC6795691 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2019] [Accepted: 09/20/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability of virtual reality (VR) to recreate controlled, immersive, and interactive environments that provide intensive and customized exercises has motivated its therapeutic use after stroke. Interaction and bodily presence in VR-based interventions is usually mediated through virtual selves, which synchronously represent body movements or responses to events on external input devices. Embodied self-representations in the virtual world not only provide an anchor for visuomotor tasks, but their morphologies can have behavioral implications. While research has focused on the underlying subjective mechanisms of exposure to VR on healthy individuals, the transference of these findings to individuals with stroke is not evident and remains unexplored, which could affect the experience and, ultimately, the clinical effectiveness of neurorehabilitation interventions. This study determined and compared the sense of embodiment and presence elicited by a virtual environment under different perspectives and levels of immersion in healthy subjects and individuals with stroke. Forty-six healthy subjects and 32 individuals with stroke embodied a gender-matched neutral avatar in a virtual environment that was displayed in a first-person perspective with a head-mounted display and in a third-person perspective with a screen, and the participants were asked to interact in a virtual task for 10 min under each condition in counterbalanced order, and to complete two questionnaires about the sense of embodiment and presence experienced during the interaction. The sense of body-ownership, self-location, and presence were more vividly experienced in a first-person than in a third-person perspective by both healthy subjects (p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.212; p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.101; p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.401, respectively) and individuals with stroke (p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.070; p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.135; p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.077, respectively). In contrast, no agency perspective-related differences were found in any group. All measures were consistently higher for healthy controls than for individuals with stroke, but differences between groups only reached statistical significance in presence under the first-person condition (p < 0.010, ηp2 = 0.084). In spite of these differences, the participants experienced a vivid sense of embodiment and presence in almost all conditions. These results provide first evidence that, although less intensively, embodiment and presence are similarly experienced by individuals who have suffered a stroke and by healthy individuals, which could support the vividness of their experience and, consequently, the effectiveness of VR-based interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adrián Borrego
- Neurorehabilitation and Brain Research Group, Instituto de Investigación e Innovación en Bioingeniería, Universitat Politècnica de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Jorge Latorre
- Neurorehabilitation and Brain Research Group, Instituto de Investigación e Innovación en Bioingeniería, Universitat Politècnica de València, Valencia, Spain.,NEURORHB-Servicio de Neurorrehabilitación de Hospitales Vithas, Valencia, Spain
| | - Mariano Alcañiz
- Neurorehabilitation and Brain Research Group, Instituto de Investigación e Innovación en Bioingeniería, Universitat Politècnica de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Roberto Llorens
- Neurorehabilitation and Brain Research Group, Instituto de Investigación e Innovación en Bioingeniería, Universitat Politècnica de València, Valencia, Spain.,NEURORHB-Servicio de Neurorrehabilitación de Hospitales Vithas, Valencia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
126
|
Reader AT, Ehrsson HH. Weakening the subjective sensation of own hand ownership does not interfere with rapid finger movements. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0223580. [PMID: 31585001 PMCID: PMC6777829 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223580] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2019] [Accepted: 09/24/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
When we perform a movement we generally have a clear distinction between which parts of the world constitute our body and which parts do not. However, how the sense of ownership over our body supports movement is not yet fully understood. We aimed to see whether a sense of ownership over the hand supports the performance of rapid hand movements. In three experiments (n = 48, n = 30, n = 24), we presented participants with congruent and incongruent visuotactile and visuoproprioceptive information regarding their own hand. In keeping with previous experiments, multisensory disintegration resulted in a reduction in the subjective sensation of ownership over the hand, as reflected in questionnaire responses. Following sensory stimulation, participants were required to rapidly abduct their index finger whilst the movement was tracked. We examined the hypothesis that, should a sense of ownership over the limb be necessary for generating rapid movements with that limb, reaction time would increase when hand ownership was reduced, whilst the acceleration and velocity of the movement would decrease. We observed that reductions in own hand ownership did not interfere with rapid index finger abduction, suggesting that the motor system may not be reliant on a subjective sense of ownership over the body in order to generate movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arran T. Reader
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
- * E-mail:
| | - H. Henrik Ehrsson
- Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
127
|
Abstract
The sense of one's own body is a pillar of self-consciousness and could be investigated by inducing human illusions of artificial objects as part of the self. Here, we present a nonhuman primate version of a rubber-hand illusion that allowed us to determine its computational and neuronal mechanisms. We implemented a video-based system in a reaching task in monkeys and combined a casual inference model to establish an objective and quantitative signature for the monkey's body representation. Similar to humans, monkeys were more likely to perceive an external object as part of the self when the dynamics (spatial disparity) and the features (shape and structure) of visual (V) input was closer to proprioceptive (P) signals. Neural signals in the monkey's premotor cortex reflected the strength of illusion and the likelihood of misattributing the illusory hand to oneself, thus, revealing a cortical representation of body ownership.
Collapse
|
128
|
Premotor cortex implements causal inference in multisensory own-body perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:19771-19773. [PMID: 31506356 PMCID: PMC6778256 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1914000116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023] Open
|
129
|
Abstract
How humans and potentially other animals construct the experience of ”being a self" remains one of the most intriguing questions in neuroscience. Over the past years, substantial advances have been made in understanding psychological mechanisms of bodily self through experimental manipulations in humans, such as rubber-hand illusion in which arbitrary objects may be ascribed with ownership and perceived as part of the self. The present study provides a demonstration in macaque monkeys and establishes objective and quantitative signatures of body representation at a single-trial level. Furthermore, we show that neural signals in macaque’s premotor cortex reflect the strength of illusion and the likelihood of misattributing the illusory arm to oneself, thus, revealing a cortical representation of bodily self-consciousness. The sense of one’s own body is a pillar of self-consciousness and could be investigated by inducing human illusions of artificial objects as part of the self. Here, we present a nonhuman primate version of a rubber-hand illusion that allowed us to determine its computational and neuronal mechanisms. We implemented a video-based system in a reaching task in monkeys and combined a casual inference model to establish an objective and quantitative signature for the monkey’s body representation. Similar to humans, monkeys were more likely to perceive an external object as part of the self when the dynamics (spatial disparity) and the features (shape and structure) of visual (V) input was closer to proprioceptive (P) signals. Neural signals in the monkey’s premotor cortex reflected the strength of illusion and the likelihood of misattributing the illusory hand to oneself, thus, revealing a cortical representation of body ownership.
Collapse
|
130
|
Riemer M, Trojan J, Beauchamp M, Fuchs X. The rubber hand universe: On the impact of methodological differences in the rubber hand illusion. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 104:268-280. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2019] [Revised: 07/04/2019] [Accepted: 07/15/2019] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
|
131
|
Gordon C, Barbullushi A, Tombolini S, Margiotta F, Ciacci A, Yosef LS, Barker L, Martini M. Visuo-tactile stimulation, but not type of movement, modulates pain during the vision of a moving virtual limb. Pain Manag 2019; 9:449-460. [PMID: 31452455 DOI: 10.2217/pmt-2019-0019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Aim: Evidence has revealed a relationship between pain and the observation of limb movement, but it is unknown whether different types of movements have diverse modulating effects. In this immersive virtual reality study, we explored the effect of the vision of different virtual arm movements (arm vs wrist) on heat pain threshold of healthy participants. Patients & methods: 40 healthy participants underwent four conditions in virtual reality, while heat pain thresholds were measured. Visuo-tactile stimulation was used to attempt to modulate the feeling of virtual limb ownership while the participants kept their arms still. Results: Effects on pain threshold were present for type of stimulation but not type of movement. Conclusion: The type of observed movement does not appear to influence pain modulation, at least not during acute pain states.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Calum Gordon
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| | - Alba Barbullushi
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| | - Stefano Tombolini
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| | - Federica Margiotta
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| | - Alessia Ciacci
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| | - Lama Shekh Yosef
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| | - Leon Barker
- Department of Arts & Digital Industries, University of East London, University Way, London E16 2RD, UK
| | - Matteo Martini
- Department of Psychology, University of East London, Water Lane, London E15 4LZ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
132
|
The Effect of a Virtual-Reality Full-Body Illusion on Body Representation in Obesity. J Clin Med 2019; 8:jcm8091330. [PMID: 31466376 PMCID: PMC6780081 DOI: 10.3390/jcm8091330] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2019] [Revised: 08/21/2019] [Accepted: 08/22/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Background. The effective illusory ownership over an artificial body in modulating body representations in healthy and eating disorders population has been repeatedly reported in recent literature. In this study, we extended this research in the field of obesity: specifically, we investigated whether ownership over a virtual body with a skinny abdomen might be successfully experienced by participants affected by obesity. Methods. Fifteen participants with obesity and fifteen healthy-weight participants took part at this study in which the VR-Full-Body Illusion was adopted. The strength of illusion was investigated through the traditional Embodiment Questionnaire, while changes in bodily experience were measured through a body size estimation task. Results. Participants with obesity as well as healthy-weight participants reported to experience the illusion. About the body size estimation task, both groups reported changes only in the estimation of the abdomen’s circumference after the experimental condition, in absence of any another difference. Discussion. Participants with obesity reported to experience the illusion over a skinny avatar, but the modulation of the bodily experience seems controversial. Future lines of research exploiting this technique for modulating body representations in obesity, specifically in terms of potential therapeutic use, were discussed.
Collapse
|
133
|
Gadsby S. Manipulating body representations with virtual reality: Clinical implications for anorexia nervosa. PHILOSOPHICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2019.1632425] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen Gadsby
- Cognition and Philosophy Lab, School of Philosophical, Historical, and International Studies, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
134
|
Hülsmann F, Frank C, Senna I, Ernst MO, Schack T, Botsch M. Superimposed Skilled Performance in a Virtual Mirror Improves Motor Performance and Cognitive Representation of a Full Body Motor Action. Front Robot AI 2019; 6:43. [PMID: 33501059 PMCID: PMC7805859 DOI: 10.3389/frobt.2019.00043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2018] [Accepted: 05/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Feedback is essential for skill acquisition as it helps identifying and correcting performance errors. Nowadays, Virtual Reality can be used as a tool to guide motor learning, and to provide innovative types of augmented feedback that exceed real world opportunities. Concurrent feedback has shown to be especially beneficial for novices. Moreover, watching skilled performances helps novices to acquire a motor skill, and this effect depends on the perspective taken by the observer. To date, however, the impact of watching one's own performance together with full body superimposition of a skilled performance, either from the front or from the side, remains to be explored. Here we used an immersive, state-of-the-art, low-latency cave automatic virtual environment (CAVE), and we asked novices to perform squat movements in front of a virtual mirror. Participants were assigned to one of three concurrent visual feedback groups: participants either watched their own avatar performing full body movements or were presented with the movement of a skilled individual superimposed on their own performance during movement execution, either from a frontal or from a side view. Motor performance and cognitive representation were measured in order to track changes in movement quality as well as motor memory across time. Consistent with our hypotheses, results showed an advantage of the groups that observed their own avatar performing the squat together with the superimposed skilled performance for some of the investigated parameters, depending on perspective. Specifically, for the deepest point of the squat, participants watching the squat from the front adapted their height, while those watching from the side adapted their backward movement. In a control experiment, we ruled out the possibility that the observed improvements were due to the mere fact of performing the squat movements—irrespective of the type of visual feedback. The present findings indicate that it can be beneficial for novices to watch themselves together with a skilled performance during execution, and that improvement depends on the perspective chosen.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felix Hülsmann
- Computer Graphics and Geometry Processing, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany.,Social Cognitive Systems, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Cornelia Frank
- Neurocognition and Action, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Irene Senna
- Applied Cognitive Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
| | - Marc O Ernst
- Applied Cognitive Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
| | - Thomas Schack
- Neurocognition and Action, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Mario Botsch
- Computer Graphics and Geometry Processing, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
135
|
Czub M, Kowal M. Respiration Entrainment in Virtual Reality by Using a Breathing Avatar. CYBERPSYCHOLOGY BEHAVIOR AND SOCIAL NETWORKING 2019; 22:494-499. [PMID: 31210540 DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0700] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Increasing evidence points to the role of interoception in body perception and in constructing the bodily self. Body ownership illusion (BOI) in virtual reality (VR) is a widely used paradigm to study body perception. However, existing research has focused mainly on exteroceptive sensory modalities, and studies on interoception and BOI remain scarce. The postulated mechanism of BOI is related to the multisensory integration of information and visuo-tactile or visuo-kinesthetic sensory conflict resolution. In this within-subjects experimental study, we introduced systematic visuo-interoceptive (visuo-respiratory) conflicts and tested if participants would resolve them by adjusting their respiration rate (RR). Participants observed a virtual breathing avatar body from the first-person perspective while their own RR was recorded. The VR system was connected to a respiration monitor. The avatar was first breathing for 60 seconds in accordance with the participant's RR; then, it was either slowing down or speeding up, each condition lasting for 180 seconds. The dependent variable was a change in participants' RR, expressed as a linear regression slope coefficient. Forty participants were included in each experimental condition, in a counterbalanced order. There was a change in RR in the predicted direction in both conditions. Participants' RR decreased on average by 0.48 breaths/minute and increased by 0.64 breaths/minute, leading to a change of 1.45 and 1.93 breaths/minute, respectively, over the entire timespan of the experiment. The difference between conditions was statistically significant (V = 192, p < 0.01). Because a change in RR of even 1 breath/minute is considered clinically significant, the results of this study-apart from demonstrating visuo-respiratory conflict resolution-may have an applied significance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marcin Czub
- Faculty of Historical and Pedagogical Sciences, Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland
| | - Marta Kowal
- Faculty of Historical and Pedagogical Sciences, Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
136
|
Active control as evidence in favor of sense of ownership in the moving Virtual Hand Illusion. Conscious Cogn 2019; 71:123-135. [PMID: 31005748 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2018] [Revised: 04/10/2019] [Accepted: 04/10/2019] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The sense of ownership, the feeling that our body belongs to ourselves, relies on multiple sources of sensory information. Among these sources, the contribution of visuomotor information is still debated. We tested the effect of active control in the sense of ownership in the moving Virtual Hand Illusion. Participants reported sense of ownership and sense of agency over a virtual arm in which we manipulated the morphological congruence of the hand and the visuomotor information. We found that congruent active control enhanced and maintained the reported sense of ownership over a hand that appeared detached from the body, but not in a morphological congruent limb. Also, incongruent active control, achieved by adding noise to the trajectory of the movement, decreased both reported sense of agency and ownership. Overall, our results are consistent with a framework in which active control acts as evidence for eliciting a sense of ownership.
Collapse
|
137
|
Pia L, Garbarini F, Kalckert A, Wong HY. Editorial: Owning a Body + Moving a Body = Me? Front Hum Neurosci 2019; 13:88. [PMID: 30936826 PMCID: PMC6431611 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2019] [Accepted: 02/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Lorenzo Pia
- SAMBA (SpAtial, Motor & Bodily Awareness) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy.,Neuroscience Institute of Turin, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Francesca Garbarini
- MANIBUS (Movement ANd Body In Behavioral and Physiological Neuroscience) Research Group, Psychology Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
| | - Andreas Kalckert
- Department of Psychology, University of Reading Malaysia, Iskandar, Malaysia
| | - Hong Yu Wong
- Philosophy Department, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.,Philosophy of Neuroscience (PONS) Research Group, Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
138
|
Kalckert A, Bico I, Fong JX. Illusions With Hands, but Not With Balloons - Comparing Ownership and Referral of Touch for a Corporal and Noncorporal Object After Visuotactile Stimulation. Perception 2019; 48:447-455. [PMID: 30939992 DOI: 10.1177/0301006619839286] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The rubber hand illusion is a perceptual illusion of perceiving an object like a model hand as part of the own body. The question whether the illusion can be induced with noncorporal objects that do not look like a human body part is not perfectly resolved yet. In this study, we directly assessed the subjective experience of two different components within the illusion (i.e., ownership and referral of touch) when a model hand and a balloon are stimulated. We observed significantly stronger illusion ratings for the hand as compared with the balloon, and only the hand ratings showed a clear affirmation of the illusion. We further conclude that (a) a significant difference between synchronous and asynchronous conditions may not be sufficient to argue for the successful induction of the illusion and (b) the subcomponents show a different pattern in the different conditions, which may lead to alternative interpretations. These observations call for a more fine-grained interpretation of questionnaire data in rubber hand illusion studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ian Bico
- University of Reading Malaysia, Iskandar Puteri, Malaysia
| | - Jia Xi Fong
- University of Reading Malaysia, Iskandar Puteri, Malaysia
| |
Collapse
|
139
|
The Effect of Visual Capture Towards Subjective Embodiment Within the Full Body Illusion. Sci Rep 2019; 9:2889. [PMID: 30814561 PMCID: PMC6393432 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-39168-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2018] [Accepted: 12/31/2018] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Typically, multisensory illusion paradigms emphasise the importance of synchronous visuotactile integration to induce subjective embodiment towards another body. However, the extent to which embodiment is due to the 'visual capture' of congruent visuoproprioceptive information alone remains unclear. Thus, across two experiments (total N = 80), we investigated how mere visual observation of a mannequin body, viewed from a first-person perspective, influenced subjective embodiment independently from concomitant visuotactile integration. Moreover, we investigated whether slow, affective touch on participants' own, unseen body (without concomitant touch on the seen mannequin) disrupted visual capture effects to a greater degree than fast, non-affective touch. In total, 40% of participants experienced subjective embodiment towards the mannequin body following mere visual observation, and this effect was significantly higher than conditions which included touch to participants own, unseen body. The velocity of the touch that participants received (affective/non-affective) did not differ in modulating visual capture effects. Furthermore, the effects of visual capture and perceived pleasantness of touch was not modulated by subthreshold eating disorder psychopathology. Overall, this study suggests that congruent visuoproprioceptive cues can be sufficient to induce subjective embodiment of a whole body, in the absence of visuotactile integration and beyond mere confabulatory responses.
Collapse
|
140
|
Preserved multisensory body representations in advanced age. Sci Rep 2019; 9:2663. [PMID: 30804474 PMCID: PMC6389982 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-39270-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2018] [Accepted: 01/21/2019] [Indexed: 12/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The internal representation of the body emerges via the integration of multisensory body cues. Sensory signal transfer and the ability to integrate multisensory information deteriorate significantly with increasing age. However, there is little empirical evidence on age-related changes in body representations based on multisensory integration. Here, we used a standard paradigm for evaluating body representations based on multisensory integration, the rubber hand illusion, and compared the amount of proprioceptive drift and changes in perceived body ownership triggered by the integration of visual, tactile, and proprioceptive cues between younger and older adults. To account for potential age-related differences in the temporal stability of the illusion, proprioceptive drift was measured at five different time points. Our results show that older adults used synchronous visuo-tactile cues similarly to younger adults to update both the position of their own hand, and their feeling of ownership over the artificial hand. Independent of visuo-tactile synchrony, older adults perceived their hand as closer to their body than younger adults did, and showed a less stable representation of this in-depth hand position. This proprioceptive bias towards the body did not correlate with the strength of the illusion. Our results indicate that the integration of visual and tactile cues is largely preserved in advanced age when used to update limb position, whereas proprioception worsens with age. This may be linked to two different pathways that underlie changes in body representations over the life span.
Collapse
|
141
|
Filippetti ML, Kirsch LP, Crucianelli L, Fotopoulou A. Affective certainty and congruency of touch modulate the experience of the rubber hand illusion. Sci Rep 2019; 9:2635. [PMID: 30796333 PMCID: PMC6385173 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-38880-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2018] [Accepted: 01/03/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Our sense of body ownership relies on integrating different sensations according to their temporal and spatial congruency. Nevertheless, there is ongoing controversy about the role of affective congruency during multisensory integration, i.e. whether the stimuli to be perceived by the different sensory channels are congruent or incongruent in terms of their affective quality. In the present study, we applied a widely used multisensory integration paradigm, the Rubber Hand Illusion, to investigate the role of affective, top-down aspects of sensory congruency between visual and tactile modalities in the sense of body ownership. In Experiment 1 (N = 36), we touched participants with either soft or rough fabrics in their unseen hand, while they watched a rubber hand been touched synchronously with the same fabric or with a 'hidden' fabric of 'uncertain roughness'. In Experiment 2 (N = 50), we used the same paradigm as in Experiment 1, but replaced the 'uncertainty' condition with an 'incongruent' one, in which participants saw the rubber hand being touched with a fabric of incongruent roughness and hence opposite valence. We found that certainty (Experiment 1) and congruency (Experiment 2) between the felt and vicariously perceived tactile affectivity led to higher subjective embodiment compared to uncertainty and incongruency, respectively, irrespective of any valence effect. Our results suggest that congruency in the affective top-down aspects of sensory stimulation is important to the multisensory integration process leading to embodiment, over and above temporal and spatial properties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Laura Filippetti
- Centre for Brain Science, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, CO4 3SQ, Colchester, UK. .,Research Department of Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, WC1E 7HB, London, UK.
| | - Louise P Kirsch
- Research Department of Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, WC1E 7HB, London, UK
| | - Laura Crucianelli
- Research Department of Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, WC1E 7HB, London, UK
| | - Aikaterini Fotopoulou
- Research Department of Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, WC1E 7HB, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
142
|
Separate multisensory integration processes for ownership and localization of body parts. Sci Rep 2019; 9:652. [PMID: 30679685 PMCID: PMC6345910 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-37375-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2018] [Accepted: 12/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The experiences that body parts are owned and localized in space are two key aspects of body awareness. Although initial work assumed that the perceived location of one’s body part can be used as a behavioral measure to assess the feeling of owning a body part, recent studies call into question the relationship between localization and ownership of body parts. Yet, little is known about the processes underlying these two aspects of body-part awareness. Here, I applied a statistically optimal cue combination paradigm to a perceptual illusion in which ownership over an artificial hand is experienced, and found that variances predicted by a model of optimal cue combination are similar to those observed in localization of the participant’s hand, but systematically diverge from those observed in ownership of the artificial hand. These findings provide strong evidence for separate processes between ownership and localization of body parts, and indicate a need to revise current models of body part ownership. Results from this study suggest that the neural substrates for perceptual identification of one’s body parts—such as body ownership—are distinct from those underlying spatial localization of the body parts, thus implying a functional distinction between “who” and “where” in the processing of body part information.
Collapse
|
143
|
Burin D, Kilteni K, Rabuffetti M, Slater M, Pia L. Body ownership increases the interference between observed and executed movements. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0209899. [PMID: 30605454 PMCID: PMC6317814 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209899] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2018] [Accepted: 12/13/2018] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
When we successfully achieve willed actions, the feeling that our moving body parts belong to the self (i.e., body ownership) is barely required. However, how and to what extent the awareness of our own body contributes to the neurocognitive processes subserving actions is still debated. Here we capitalized on immersive virtual reality in order to examine whether and how body ownership influences motor performance (and, secondly, if it modulates the feeling of voluntariness). Healthy participants saw a virtual body either from a first or a third person perspective. In both conditions, they had to draw continuously straight vertical lines while seeing the virtual arm doing the same action (i.e., drawing lines) or deviating from them (i.e., drawing ellipses). Results showed that when there was a mismatch between the intended and the seen movements (i.e., participants had to draw lines but the avatar drew ellipses), motor performance was strongly "attracted" towards the seen (rather than the performed) movement when the avatar's body part was perceived as own (i.e., first person perspective). In support of previous studies, here we provide direct behavioral evidence that the feeling of body ownership modulates the interference of seen movements to the performed movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dalila Burin
- Smart Aging Research Center, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Konstantina Kilteni
- EVENT LAB- Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Marco Rabuffetti
- Biomedical Technology Department, IRCCS Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation, Milan, Italy
| | - Mel Slater
- EVENT LAB- Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology Laboratory, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- Institute of Neuroscience, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- Department of Computer Science, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Lorenzo Pia
- SAMBA- SpAtial Motor and Bodily Awareness research group- Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
- NIT- Neuroscience Institute of Turin, Orbassano- Turin, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
144
|
Do movements contribute to sense of body ownership? Rubber hand illusion in expert pianists. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 83:185-195. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1137-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2018] [Accepted: 12/10/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
145
|
Effects of horizontal distance and limb crossing on perceived hand spacing and ownership: Differential sensory processing across hand configurations. Sci Rep 2018; 8:17699. [PMID: 30531927 PMCID: PMC6286308 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-35895-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2018] [Accepted: 10/01/2018] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
We have previously shown that, with the hands apart vertically, passively grasping an artificial finger induces a sense of ownership over the artificial finger and coming-together of the hands. The present study investigated this grasp illusion in the horizontal plane. Thirty healthy participants were tested in two conditions (grasp and no grasp) with their hands at different distances apart, either crossed or uncrossed. After 3 min, participants reported perceived spacing between index fingers, perceived index finger location, and, for the grasp condition, perceived ownership over the artificial finger. On average, there was no ownership at any of the hand configurations. With the hands uncrossed 7.5, 15 or 24 cm apart, there was no difference in perceived spacing between the grasp and no grasp conditions. With the hands crossed and 15 cm apart, perceived spacing between index fingers was 3.2 cm [0.7 to 5.7] (mean [95% CI]) smaller during the grasp condition compared to no grasp. Therefore, compared to when the hands are vertically separated, there is an almost complete lack of a grasp illusion in the horizontal plane which indicates the brain may process sensory inputs from the hands differently based on whether the hands are horizontally or vertically apart.
Collapse
|
146
|
Perez-Marcos D. Virtual reality experiences, embodiment, videogames and their dimensions in neurorehabilitation. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2018; 15:113. [PMID: 30477527 PMCID: PMC6258149 DOI: 10.1186/s12984-018-0461-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2018] [Accepted: 11/12/2018] [Indexed: 12/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Background In the context of stroke rehabilitation, new training approaches mediated by virtual reality and videogames are usually discussed and evaluated together in reviews and meta-analyses. This represents a serious confounding factor that is leading to misleading, inconclusive outcomes in the interest of validating these new solutions. Main body Extending existing definitions of virtual reality, in this paper I put forward the concept of virtual reality experience (VRE), generated by virtual reality systems (VRS; i.e. a group of variable technologies employed to create a VRE). Then, I review the main components composing a VRE, and how they may purposely affect the mind and body of participants in the context of neurorehabilitation. In turn, VRS are not anymore exclusive from VREs but are currently used in videogames and other human-computer interaction applications in different domains. Often, these other applications receive the name of virtual reality applications as they use VRS. However, they do not necessarily create a VRE. I put emphasis on exposing fundamental similarities and differences between VREs and videogames for neurorehabilitation. I also recommend describing and evaluating the specific features encompassing the intervention rather than evaluating virtual reality or videogames as a whole. Conclusion This disambiguation between VREs, VRS and videogames should help reduce confusion in the field. This is important for databases searches when looking for specific studies or building metareviews that aim at evaluating the efficacy of technology-mediated interventions.
Collapse
|
147
|
Abstract
Self-orientation perception is a necessary ability for everyday life that heavily depends on visual and vestibular information. To perceive the orientation of oneself with respect to the external environment would seem to first require that one has a clear sense of one’s own body (‘sense of body ownership’). However, the experimental evidence for this is sparse. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to investigate how the sense of body ownership affects perceived self-orientation. We combined a self-orientation illusion – where the visual scene, i.e., a fully furnished room, was rotated slowly around the roll axis – with a full-body ownership illusion paradigm – where the ownership of a stranger’s body seen from the first-person perspective in the center of the scene was manipulated by synchronous (illusion) or asynchronous (control) visual-tactile stimulation. Participants were asked to judge the appearance of shaded disk stimuli (a shape-from-shading test), which are perceived as three-dimensional (3D) spheres; this perception depends on perceived self-orientation. Illusory body ownership influenced self-orientation as reported subjectively in questionnaires and as evident from the objective shape-from-shading test data. Thus, body ownership determines self-orientation perception, presumably by boosting the weighting of visual cues over the gravitational forces detected by the vestibular system.
Collapse
|
148
|
Abstract
It is currently not well understood whether people experience themselves to be located in one or more specific part(s) of their body. Virtual reality (VR) is increasingly used as a tool to study aspects of bodily perception and self-consciousness, due to its strong experimental control and ease in manipulating multi-sensory aspects of bodily experience. To investigate where people self-locate in their body within virtual reality, we asked participants to point directly at themselves with a virtual pointer, in a VR headset. In previous work employing a physical pointer, participants mainly located themselves in the upper face and upper torso. In this study, using a VR headset, participants mainly located themselves in the upper face. In an additional body template task where participants pointed at themselves on a picture of a simple body outline, participants pointed most often to the upper torso, followed by the (upper) face. These results raise the question as to whether head-mounted virtual reality might alter where people locate themselves making them more "head-centred".
Collapse
|
149
|
Body ownership and the absence of touch: approaching the rubber hand inside and outside peri-hand space. Exp Brain Res 2018; 236:3251-3265. [PMID: 30220004 PMCID: PMC6267689 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5361-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2017] [Accepted: 08/11/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
It is widely accepted that the integration of visual and tactile information is a necessity to induce ownership over a rubber hand. This idea has recently been challenged by Ferri et al. (Proc R Soc B 280:1–7, 2013), as they found that sense of ownership was evident by mere expectation of touch. In our study, we aimed to further investigate this finding, by studying whether the mere potential for touch yields a sense of ownership similar in magnitude to that resulting from actually being touched. We conducted two experiments. In the first experiment, our set-up was the classical horizontal set-up (similar to Botvinick and Cohen, Nature 391:756, 1998). Sixty-three individuals were included and performed the classical conditions (synchronous, asynchronous), an approached but not touched (potential for touch), and a ‘visual only’ condition. In the second experiment, we controlled for differences between the current set-up and the vertical set-up used by Ferri et al. (Proc R Soc B 280:1–7, 2013). Fifteen individuals were included and performed a synchronous and various approaching conditions [i.e., vertical approach, horizontal approach, and a control approach (no hands)]. In our first experiment, we found that approaching the rubber hand neither induced a larger proprioceptive drift nor a stronger subjective sense of ownership than asynchronous stimulation did. Generally, our participants gained most sense of ownership in the synchronous condition, followed by the visual only condition. When using a vertical set-up (second experiment), we confirmed previous suggestions that tactile expectation was able to induce embodiment over a foreign hand, similar in magnitude to actual touch, but only when the real and rubber hand were aligned on the vertical axis, thus along the trajectory of the approaching stimulus. These results indicate that our brain uses bottom-up sensory information, as well as top-down predictions for building a representation of our body.
Collapse
|
150
|
Serino S, Scarpina F, Dakanalis A, Keizer A, Pedroli E, Castelnuovo G, Chirico A, Catallo V, di Lernia D, Riva G. The Role of Age on Multisensory Bodily Experience: An Experimental Study with a Virtual Reality Full-Body Illusion. CYBERPSYCHOLOGY BEHAVIOR AND SOCIAL NETWORKING 2018; 21:304-310. [PMID: 29762066 PMCID: PMC5961744 DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0674] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
A growing body of evidence demonstrated that it is feasible to induce ownership over an artificial body to alter bodily experience. However, several uncharted aspects about full-body illusion applications need to be tackled before a complete exploitation of these methods in clinical practice. This work is devoted to explore possible individual age-related differences in shaping changes in body representations induced with a full-body illusion. A total of 40 women were divided into two different age groups according to the median of the variable age. Participants estimated the width of three different body parts (i.e., shoulders, abdomen, and hips) before the entire illusion was induced (baseline), and after the synchronous and the asynchronous conditions. Results revealed that 26-to-55-year-old participants were more resistant to changes induced by the bodily illusion, whereas 19-to-25-year-old participants underestimated their bodies after both conditions. The findings were discussed in terms of the literature exploring age differences in responses to bodily illusion, which could suggest a Bayesian mechanism underlying these individual differences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvia Serino
- 1 Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore , Milan, Italy .,2 Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano , Milan, Italy
| | - Federica Scarpina
- 3 Psychology Research Laboratory, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano , Ospedale San Giuseppe, Piancavallo, Italy
| | - Antonios Dakanalis
- 4 Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia , Pavia, Italy .,5 Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Milano-Bicocca , Monza, Italy
| | - Anouk Keizer
- 6 Department of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Utrecht University , Utrecht, Netherlands
| | - Elisa Pedroli
- 2 Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano , Milan, Italy
| | - Gianluca Castelnuovo
- 1 Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore , Milan, Italy .,3 Psychology Research Laboratory, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano , Ospedale San Giuseppe, Piancavallo, Italy
| | - Alice Chirico
- 1 Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore , Milan, Italy
| | - Valentina Catallo
- 2 Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano , Milan, Italy
| | - Daniele di Lernia
- 1 Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore , Milan, Italy
| | - Giuseppe Riva
- 1 Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore , Milan, Italy .,2 Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Lab, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano , Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|