201
|
Fonteille V, Stoléru S. Les corrélats cérébraux du désir sexuel : approche en neuro-imagerie fonctionnelle. SEXOLOGIES 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/j.sexol.2010.03.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
202
|
|
203
|
Hesslow G. The current status of the simulation theory of cognition. Brain Res 2011; 1428:71-9. [PMID: 21763643 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2010] [Revised: 05/16/2011] [Accepted: 06/10/2011] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
It is proposed that thinking is simulated interaction with the environment. Three assumptions underlie this 'simulation' theory of cognitive function. Firstly, behaviour can be simulated in the sense that we can activate motor structures, as during a normal overt action, but suppress its execution. Secondly, perception can be simulated by internal activation of sensory cortex in a way that resembles its normal activation during perception of external stimuli. The third assumption ('anticipation') is that both overt and simulated actions can elicit perceptual simulation of their most probable consequences. A large body of evidence, mainly from neuroimaging studies, that supports these assumptions, is reviewed briefly. The theory is ontologically parsimonious and does not rely on standard cognitivist constructs such as internal models or representations. It is argued that the simulation approach can explain the relations between motor, sensory and cognitive functions and the appearance of an inner world. It also unifies and explains important features of a wide variety of cognitive phenomena such as memory and cognitive maps. Novel findings from recent developments in memory research on the similarity of imaging and memory and on the role of both prefrontal cortex and sensory cortex in declarative memory and working memory are predicted by the theory and provide striking support for it. This article is part of a Special Issue entitled "The Cognitive Neuroscience".
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Germund Hesslow
- Department of Experimental Medical Science, University of Lund, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
204
|
Poeppl TB, Nitschke J, Dombert B, Santtila P, Greenlee MW, Osterheider M, Mokros A. Functional Cortical and Subcortical Abnormalities in Pedophilia: A Combined Study Using a Choice Reaction Time Task and fMRI. J Sex Med 2011; 8:1660-74. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02248.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
205
|
Activation of the parieto-premotor network is associated with vivid motor imagery--a parametric FMRI study. PLoS One 2011; 6:e20368. [PMID: 21655298 PMCID: PMC3105023 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020368] [Citation(s) in RCA: 74] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2010] [Accepted: 05/01/2011] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study examined the neural basis of vivid motor imagery with parametrical functional magnetic resonance imaging. 22 participants performed motor imagery (MI) of six different right-hand movements that differed in terms of pointing accuracy needs and object involvement, i.e., either none, two big or two small squares had to be pointed at in alternation either with or without an object grasped with the fingers. After each imagery trial, they rated the perceived vividness of motor imagery on a 7-point scale. Results showed that increased perceived imagery vividness was parametrically associated with increasing neural activation within the left putamen, the left premotor cortex (PMC), the posterior parietal cortex of the left hemisphere, the left primary motor cortex, the left somatosensory cortex, and the left cerebellum. Within the right hemisphere, activation was found within the right cerebellum, the right putamen, and the right PMC. It is concluded that the perceived vividness of MI is parametrically associated with neural activity within sensorimotor areas. The results corroborate the hypothesis that MI is an outcome of neural computations based on movement representations located within motor areas.
Collapse
|
206
|
Nather FC, Bueno JLO, Bigand E, Droit-Volet S. Time changes with the embodiment of another's body posture. PLoS One 2011; 6:e19818. [PMID: 21637759 PMCID: PMC3103514 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019818] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2011] [Accepted: 04/13/2011] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The aim of the present study was to investigate whether the perception of presentation durations of pictures of different body postures was distorted as function of the embodied movement that originally produced these postures. Participants were presented with two pictures, one with a low-arousal body posture judged to require no movement and the other with a high-arousal body posture judged to require considerable movement. In a temporal bisection task with two ranges of standard durations (0.4/1.6 s and 2/8 s), the participants had to judge whether the presentation duration of each of the pictures was more similar to the short or to the long standard duration. The results showed that the duration was judged longer for the posture requiring more movement than for the posture requiring less movement. However the magnitude of this overestimation was relatively greater for the range of short durations than for that of longer durations. Further analyses suggest that this lengthening effect was mediated by an arousal effect of limited duration on the speed of the internal clock system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Emmanuel Bigand
- Institut Universitaire de France, Université de Bourgogne, LEAD CNRS, Dijon, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
207
|
Francuz P, Zapała D. The suppression of the μ rhythm during the creation of imagery representation of movement. Neurosci Lett 2011; 495:39-43. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2010] [Revised: 02/28/2011] [Accepted: 03/08/2011] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
208
|
Recovery of motor imagery ability in stroke patients. Rehabil Res Pract 2011; 2011:283840. [PMID: 22110971 PMCID: PMC3195293 DOI: 10.1155/2011/283840] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2010] [Revised: 01/26/2011] [Accepted: 02/08/2011] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective. To investigate whether motor imagery ability recovers in stroke patients and to see what the relationship is between different types of imagery and motor functioning after stroke.
Methods. 12 unilateral stroke patients were measured at 3 and 6 weeks poststroke on 3 mental imagery tasks. Arm-hand function was evaluated using the Utrecht Arm-Hand task and the Brunnström Fugl-Meyer Scale. Age-matched healthy individuals (N = 10) were included as controls.
Results. Implicit motor imagery ability and visual motor imagery ability improved significantly at 6 weeks compared to 3 weeks poststroke.
Conclusion. Our study shows that motor imagery can recover in the first weeks after stroke. This indicates that a group of patients who might not be initially selected for mental practice can, still later in the rehabilitation process, participate in mental practice programs. Moreover, our study shows that mental imagery modalities can be differently affected in individual patients and over time.
Collapse
|
209
|
Ferri F, Frassinetti F, Costantini M, Gallese V. Motor simulation and the bodily self. PLoS One 2011; 6:e17927. [PMID: 21464959 PMCID: PMC3064658 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017927] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2010] [Accepted: 02/16/2011] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies demonstrated the human ability to implicitly recognize their own
body. When submitted to a visual matching task, participants showed the
so-called self-advantage, that is, a better performance with self rather than
others' body or body parts. Here, we investigated whether the body
self-advantage relies upon a motor representation of one's body.
Participants were submitted to a laterality judgment of self and others'
hands (Experiment 1 and 3), which involves a sensory-motor mental simulation.
Moreover, to investigate whether the self-advantage emerges also when an
explicit self processing is required, the same participants were submitted to an
explicit self-body recognition task (Experiment 2). Participants showed the
self-advantage when performing the laterality judgment, but not when
self-recognition was explicitly required. Thus, implicit and explicit
recognition of the bodily self dissociate and only an implicit recognition of
the bodily self, mapped in motor terms, allows the self-advantage to emerge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesca Ferri
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Parma, Parma, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
210
|
Hermes D, Vansteensel MJ, Albers AM, Bleichner MG, Benedictus MR, Mendez Orellana C, Aarnoutse EJ, Ramsey NF. Functional MRI-based identification of brain areas involved in motor imagery for implantable brain–computer interfaces. J Neural Eng 2011; 8:025007. [DOI: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/2/025007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
211
|
Wadsworth HM, Kana RK. Brain mechanisms of perceiving tools and imagining tool use acts: a functional MRI study. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:1863-9. [PMID: 21419144 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2010] [Revised: 02/13/2011] [Accepted: 03/09/2011] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The ability to conceptualize and manipulate tools in a complex manner is a distinguishing characteristic of humans, and forms a promising milestone in human evolution. While using tools is a motor act, proposals for executing such acts may be evoked by the mere perception of a tool. Imagining an action using a tool may invoke mental readjustment of body posture, planning motor movements, and matching such plans with the model action. This fMRI study examined the brain response in 32 healthy adults when they either viewed a tool or imagined using it. While both viewing and imagining tasks recruited similar regions, imagined tool use showed greater activation in motor areas, and in areas around the bilateral temporoparietal junction. Viewing tools, on the other hand, produced robust activation in the inferior frontal, occipital, parietal, and ventral temporal areas. Analysis of gender differences indicated males recruiting medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices and females, left supramarginal gyrus and left anterior insula. While tool viewing seems to generate prehensions about using them, the imagined action using a tool mirrored brain responses underlying functional use of it. The findings of this study may suggest that perception and imagination of tools may form precursors to overt actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heather M Wadsworth
- Department of Psychology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, CIRC 235G, 1719 6th Ave South, Birmingham, AL 35294-0021, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
212
|
Crémers J, Dessoullières A, Garraux G. Hemispheric specialization during mental imagery of brisk walking. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 33:873-82. [PMID: 21425400 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21255] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2010] [Revised: 11/06/2010] [Accepted: 12/16/2010] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Brisk walking, a sensitive test to evaluate gait capacity in normal and pathological aging such as parkinsonism, is used as an alternative to classical fitness program for motor rehabilitation and may help to decrease the risk of cognitive deterioration observed with aging. In this study, we aimed to identify brain areas normally involved in its control. METHODS We conducted a block-design blood oxygen level dependent function magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD fMRI) experiment in 18 young healthy individuals trained to imagine themselves in three main situations: brisk walking in a 25-m-long corridor, standing or lying. Imagined walking time (IWT) was measured as a control of behavioral performance during fMRI. RESULTS The group mean IWT was not significantly different from the actual walking time measured during a training session prior to the fMRI study. Compared with other experimental conditions, mental imagery (MI) of brisk walking was associated with stronger activity in frontal and parietal regions mainly on the right, and cerebellar hemispheres, mainly on the left. Presumed imagined walking speed (2.3 ± 0.4 m/s) was positively correlated with activity levels in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal lobule along with the vermis and the left cerebellar hemisphere. INTERPRETATIONS A new finding in this study is that MI of brisk walking in young healthy individuals strongly involves processes lateralized in right fronto-parietal regions along with left cerebellum. These results show that brisk walking might be a non automatic locomotor activity requiring a high-level supraspinal control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julien Crémers
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital Center of Liège, Belgium
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
213
|
Jardri R, Pins D, Lafargue G, Very E, Ameller A, Delmaire C, Thomas P. Increased overlap between the brain areas involved in self-other distinction in schizophrenia. PLoS One 2011; 6:e17500. [PMID: 21408008 PMCID: PMC3052363 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2010] [Accepted: 02/07/2011] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Self-awareness impairments are frequently mentioned as being responsible for the positive symptoms of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. However, the neural correlates of self-other distinction in this pathology are still poorly understood. In the present study, we developed an fMRI procedure in order to examine self-other distinction during speech exchange situations. Fifteen subjects with schizophrenia were compared to 15 matched controls. The results revealed an increased overlap between the self and non-self cortical maps in schizophrenia, in the medial frontal and medial parietal cortices, as well as in the right middle temporal cortex and the right inferior parietal lobule. Moreover, these neural structures showed less BOLD amplitude differences between the self and non-self conditions in the patients. These activation patterns were judged to be independent of mirror-like properties, familiarity or body-ownership processing. Significantly, the increase in the right IPL signal was found to correlate positively with the severity of first-rank symptoms, and thus could be considered a “state-marker” of schizophrenia, whereas temporal and medial parieto-frontal differences appear to be “trait-markers” of the disease. Such an increased overlap between self and non-self cortical maps might be considered a neuro-physiological signature of the well established self-awareness impairment in people suffering from schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Renaud Jardri
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Fonctionnelles et Pathologies, EA-4559, Université Lille Nord de France, Lille, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
214
|
Abstract
Do smokers simulate smoking when they see someone else smoke? For regular smokers, smoking is such a highly practiced motor skill that it often occurs automatically, without conscious awareness. Research on the brain basis of action observation has delineated a frontoparietal network that is commonly recruited when people observe, plan, or imitate actions. Here, we investigated whether this action observation network would be preferentially recruited in smokers when viewing complex smoking cues, such as those occurring in motion pictures. Seventeen right-handed smokers and 17 nonsmokers watched a popular movie while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging. Using a natural stimulus, such as a movie, allowed us to keep both smoking and nonsmoking participants naive to the goals of the experiment. Brain activity evoked by movie scenes of smoking was contrasted with nonsmoking control scenes that were matched for frequency and duration. Compared with nonsmokers, smokers showed greater activity in left anterior intraparietal sulcus and inferior frontal gyrus, regions involved in the simulation of contralateral hand-based gestures, when viewing smoking versus control scenes. These results demonstrate that smokers spontaneously represent the action of smoking when viewing others smoke, the consequence of which may make it more difficult to abstain from smoking.
Collapse
|
215
|
Raffin E, Giraux P, Reilly KT. The moving phantom: motor execution or motor imagery? Cortex 2011; 48:746-57. [PMID: 21397901 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2010] [Revised: 12/10/2010] [Accepted: 02/01/2011] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Amputees who have a phantom limb often report the ability to move this phantom voluntarily. In the literature, phantom limb movements are generally considered to reflect motor imagery rather than motor execution. The aim of this study was to investigate whether amputees distinguish between executing a movement of the phantom limb and imagining moving the missing limb. We examined the capacity of 19 upper-limb amputees to execute and imagine movements of both their phantom and intact limbs. Their behaviour was compared with that of 18 age-matched normal controls. A global questionnaire-based assessment of imagery ability and timed tests showed that amputees can indeed distinguish between motor execution and motor imagery with the phantom limb, and that the former is associated with activity in stump muscles while the latter is not. Amputation reduced the speed of voluntary movements with the phantom limb but did not change the speed of imagined movements, suggesting that the absence of the limb specifically affects the ability to voluntarily move the phantom but does not change the ability to imagine moving the missing limb. These results suggest that under some conditions, for example amputation, the predicted sensory consequences of a motor command are sufficient to evoke the sensation of voluntary movement. They also suggest that the distinction between imagined and executed movements should be taken into consideration when designing research protocols to investigate the analgesic effects of sensorimotor feedback.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Estelle Raffin
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Faculty of Medicine, University Jean Monnet, Saint-Etienne, France
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
216
|
|
217
|
The relation between self-reported empathy and motor identification with imagined agents. PLoS One 2011; 6:e14595. [PMID: 21298089 PMCID: PMC3027625 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014595] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2010] [Accepted: 12/19/2010] [Indexed: 01/01/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND In a previous study, we found that when required to imagine another person performing an action, participants reported a higher correspondence between their own handedness and the hand used by the imagined person when the agent was seen from the back compared to when the agent was seen from the front. This result was explained as evidence of a greater involvement of motor areas in the back-view perspective, possibly indicating a greater proneness to put oneself in the agent's shoes in such a condition. In turn, the proneness to put oneself in another's shoes could also be considered as a cue of greater identification with the other, that is a form of empathy. If this is the case, the proportion of lateral matches vs mismatches should be different for subjects with high and low self-reported empathy. In the present study, we aimed at testing this hypothesis. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS Participants were required to imagine a person performing a single manual action in a back view and to indicate the hand used by the imagined person during movement execution. Consistent with our hypothesis, the proportion of matching between the handedness of participants and the handedness of agents imagined was higher for participants scoring high in a self-report measure of empathy. Importantly, this relationship was specific for females. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE At least for females, our data seem to corroborate the idea of a link between self-reported empathy and motor identification with imagined agents. This sex-specific result is consistent with neuroimaging studies indicating a stronger involvement of action representations during emotional and empathic processing in females than in males. In sum, our findings underline the possibility of employing behavioral research as a test-bed for theories deriving from functional studies suggesting a link between empathic processing and the activation of motor-related areas.
Collapse
|
218
|
Anderson WS, Weiss N, Lawson HC, Ohara S, Rowland L, Lenz FA. Demonstration of motor imagery movement and phantom movement-related neuronal activity in human thalamus. Neuroreport 2011; 22:88-92. [PMID: 21150804 PMCID: PMC3071423 DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e328342c98a] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Functional imaging studies show that motor imagery activates multiple structures in the human forebrain. We now show that phantom movements in an amputee and imagined movements in intact individuals elicit responses from neurons in several human thalamic nuclei. These include the somatic sensory nucleus receiving input from the periphery (ventral caudal), and the motor nuclei receiving input from the cerebellum [ventral intermediate (Vim)] and the basal ganglia [ventral oral posterior (Vop)]. Seven neurons in the amputee showed phantom movement-related activity (three Vim, two Vop, and two ventral caudal). In addition, seven neurons in a group of three controls showed motor imagery-related activity (four Vim and three Vop). These studies were performed during single neuron recording sessions in patients undergoing therapeutic treatment of phantom pain, tremor, and chronic pain conditions by thalamic stimulation. The activity of neurons in these sensory and motor nuclei, respectively, may encode the expected sensory consequences and the dynamics of planned movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William S. Anderson
- Instructor in Neurosurgery, Harvard Medical School, Department of Neurosurgery, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, 75 Francis Street CA 138B, Boston, MA USA 02115, (o) +1(617)732-6600, (f) +1(617)713-3050
| | - Nirit Weiss
- Assistant Professor, Department of Neurosurgery, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, Annenberg Building, 8-31, One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1136, New York, NY 10029, (o) +1(212)241-6820, (f) +1(212)410-0603
| | - Herman Christopher Lawson
- Baltimore Neurosurgery and Spine Center, 5051 Greenspring Avenue, Suite 101, Baltimore, MD 21209, (o) +1(410)664-3680, (f) +1(410)664-3686
| | - Shinji Ohara
- Division of Neurosurgery, Neuroscience Center, Fukuoka Sanno Hospital, 3-6-45 Momochihama, Sawara-ku, Fukuoka, Japan 814-0001
| | - Lance Rowland
- Sinai Hospital of Baltimore, Department of Neurology, 2401 West Belvedere Avenue, Baltimore, MD 21215, (o) +1(410)601-5709
| | - Frederick A. Lenz
- Professor of Neurosurgery, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Department of Neurosurgery, 600 North Wolfe Street, Meyer 8-181, Baltimore, MD 21287, (o) +1(410)955-2257, (f) +1(443)287-8044
| |
Collapse
|
219
|
Halder S, Agorastos D, Veit R, Hammer EM, Lee S, Varkuti B, Bogdan M, Rosenstiel W, Birbaumer N, Kübler A. Neural mechanisms of brain-computer interface control. Neuroimage 2011; 55:1779-90. [PMID: 21256234 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 117] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/24/2010] [Revised: 12/23/2010] [Accepted: 01/10/2011] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) enable people with paralysis to communicate with their environment. Motor imagery can be used to generate distinct patterns of cortical activation in the electroencephalogram (EEG) and thus control a BCI. To elucidate the cortical correlates of BCI control, users of a sensory motor rhythm (SMR)-BCI were classified according to their BCI control performance. In a second session these participants performed a motor imagery, motor observation and motor execution task in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanner. Group difference analysis between high and low aptitude BCI users revealed significantly higher activation of the supplementary motor areas (SMA) for the motor imagery and the motor observation tasks in high aptitude users. Low aptitude users showed no activation when observing movement. The number of activated voxels during motor observation was significantly correlated with accuracy in the EEG-BCI task (r=0.53). Furthermore, the number of activated voxels in the right middle frontal gyrus, an area responsible for processing of movement observation, correlated (r=0.72) with BCI-performance. This strong correlation highlights the importance of these areas for task monitoring and working memory as task goals have to be activated throughout the BCI session. The ability to regulate behavior and the brain through learning mechanisms involving imagery such as required to control a BCI constitutes the consequence of ideo-motor co-activation of motor brain systems during observation of movements. The results demonstrate that acquisition of a sensorimotor program reflected in SMR-BCI-control is tightly related to the recall of such sensorimotor programs during observation of movements and unrelated to the actual execution of these movement sequences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Halder
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
220
|
|
221
|
Gao Q, Duan X, Chen H. Evaluation of effective connectivity of motor areas during motor imagery and execution using conditional Granger causality. Neuroimage 2011; 54:1280-8. [PMID: 20828626 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 126] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2010] [Revised: 08/30/2010] [Accepted: 08/30/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022] Open
|
222
|
Romero-Romo JI, Bauer CCC, Pasaye EH, Gutiérrez RA, Favila R, Barrios FA. Abnormal functioning of the thalamocortical system underlies the conscious awareness of the phantom limb phenomenon. Neuroradiol J 2010; 23:671-9. [PMID: 24148720 DOI: 10.1177/197140091002300605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2010] [Accepted: 10/07/2010] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Phantom limb (PL), a phenomenon experienced by most patients after amputation, has mostly served as a paradigm to study experiences that appear to be associated with neural plasticity within the CNS. However, the subjective nature of PL experiences has had no definitive means of reliable assessment other than using patients' direct reports, nor was there a way to study the neural mechanisms involved in the conscious awareness of this mental phenomenon. Here we obtained patients' indirect responses to PL experiences for an objective evaluation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Six control subjects and six lower limb (LL) amputees participated in a motor imagery task for both the intact and the particular phantom toes. While all subjects shared neural processing of distinctive regional cerebral activations during motor imagery of the intact toes (prefrontal (PF), supplementary motor area (SMA), primary motor cortex (M1), superior temporal gyrus (STG)), it was only during motor imagery of the amputated toes in amputees that we observed an increased blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the contralateral basal ganglia at the medial globus pallidus (MGP), substantia nigra (SN), and thalamus. This increased BOLD signal in the basal ganglia-thalamus-cortex pathway during imaginary movement of the phantom toes may reflect an abnormal open loop functioning of the thalamocortical system underlying the conscious awareness of the phantom phenomenon. We suggest that the reduction in afferent information contributes to and coalesces with the higher-level reorganization resulting in the subjective conscious awareness of the phantom limb.
Collapse
|
223
|
|
224
|
Sakamoto K, Nakata H, Yumoto M, Kakigi R. Somatosensory processing of the tongue in humans. Front Physiol 2010; 1:136. [PMID: 21423377 PMCID: PMC3059928 DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2010.00136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2010] [Accepted: 09/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We review research on somatosensory (tactile) processing of the tongue based on data obtained using non-invasive neurophysiological and neuroimaging methods. Technical difficulties in stimulating the tongue, due to the noise elicited by the stimulator, the fixation of the stimulator, and the vomiting reflex, have necessitated the development of specialized devices. In this article, we show the brain activity relating to somatosensory processing of the tongue evoked by such devices. More recently, the postero-lateral part of the tongue has been stimulated, and the brain response compared with that on stimulation of the antero-lateral part of the tongue. It is likely that a difference existed in somatosensory processing of the tongue, particularly around primary somatosensory cortex, Brodmann area 40, and the anterior cingulate cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kiwako Sakamoto
- Department of Integrative Physiology, National Institute for Physiological SciencesOkazaki, Japan
- Department of Clinical Laboratory Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo HospitalTokyo, Japan
| | - Hiroki Nakata
- Department of Integrative Physiology, National Institute for Physiological SciencesOkazaki, Japan
- Faculty of Sport Sciences, Waseda UniversityTokorozawa, Saitama, Japan
| | - Masato Yumoto
- Department of Clinical Laboratory Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo HospitalTokyo, Japan
| | - Ryusuke Kakigi
- Department of Integrative Physiology, National Institute for Physiological SciencesOkazaki, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
225
|
The handedness of imagined bodies in action and the role of perspective taking. Brain Cogn 2010; 75:51-9. [PMID: 21035936 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2010] [Revised: 09/26/2010] [Accepted: 10/04/2010] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Past research at the nexus of motor control and perception investigated the role of perspective taking in many behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Some investigators addressed the issue of one's own vs. others' action imagination, but the possible effects of a front or a back view in imagining others' actions have so far been neglected. We report two 'single trial' experiments in which a total of 640 participants were asked to imagine a person performing a manual action - either in a front or in a back view - and then to indicate the hand used by the imagined person during movement execution. In such a task, we assume the existence of two distinct biases: a perceptual-mnemonic bias due to subjects' visual experience of others' actions, encouraging them to imagine right-handed movements, and a motor bias due to subjects' experience of self-made actions, encouraging them to imagine movements performed with the same hand as their dominant hand. We hypothesized that a greater involvement of motor representations in the back view compared to the front view could result in an increased correspondence between one's own manual preference and the hand used by the imagined agent in the former condition. The results of both experiments were consistent with this hypothesis, suggesting that while imagining others' actions we employ motor simulations in different degrees according to the perspective adopted.
Collapse
|
226
|
Tian X, Poeppel D. Mental imagery of speech and movement implicates the dynamics of internal forward models. Front Psychol 2010; 1:166. [PMID: 21897822 PMCID: PMC3158430 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00166] [Citation(s) in RCA: 132] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2010] [Accepted: 09/20/2010] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The classical concept of efference copies in the context of internal forward models has stimulated productive research in cognitive science and neuroscience. There are compelling reasons to argue for such a mechanism, but finding direct evidence in the human brain remains difficult. Here we investigate the dynamics of internal forward models from an unconventional angle: mental imagery, assessed while recording high temporal resolution neuronal activity using magnetoencephalography. We compare two overt and covert tasks; our covert, mental imagery tasks are unconfounded by overt input/output demands – but in turn necessitate the development of appropriate multi-dimensional topographic analyses. Finger tapping (studies 1 and 2) and speech experiments (studies 3–5) provide temporally constrained results that implicate the estimation of an efference copy. We suggest that one internal forward model over parietal cortex subserves the kinesthetic feeling in motor imagery. Secondly, observed auditory neural activity ~170 ms after motor estimation in speech experiments (studies 3–5) demonstrates the anticipated auditory consequences of planned motor commands in a second internal forward model in imagery of speech production. Our results provide neurophysiological evidence from the human brain in favor of internal forward models deploying efference copies in somatosensory and auditory cortex, in finger tapping and speech production tasks, respectively, and also suggest the dynamics and sequential updating structure of internal forward models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xing Tian
- Department of Psychology, New York University New York, NY, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
227
|
Kilintari M, Raos V, Savaki HE. Grasping in the dark activates early visual cortices. Cereb Cortex 2010; 21:949-63. [PMID: 20833697 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhq175] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
We have previously demonstrated that the primary motor and somatosensory cortices of monkeys are somatotopically activated for action-observation as are for action-generation, indicating that the recruitment of learned somatosensory-motor representations underlies the perception of others' actions. Here we examined the effects of seen and unseen actions on the early visual cortices, to determine whether stored visual representations are employed in addition to the somatosensory-motor ones. We used the quantitative (14)C-deoxyglucose method to map the activity throughout the cortex of the occipital operculum, lunate, and inferior occipital sulci of "rhesus monkeys" who reached to grasp a 3D object either in the light or in the dark or who observed the same action executed by another subject. In all cases, the extrastriate areas V3d and V3A displayed marked activation. We suggest that these activations reflect processing of visuospatial information useful for the reaching component of action, and 3D object-related information useful for the grasping part. We suggest that a memorized visual representation of the action supports action-recognition, as well as action-execution in complete darkness when the object and its environment are invisible. Accordingly, the internal representation that serves action-cognition is not purely somatosensory-motor but also includes a visual component.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marina Kilintari
- Department of Basic Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, School of Health Sciences, University of Crete, Crete, 71003 Greece
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
228
|
Gueugneau N, Papaxanthis C. Time-of-day effects on the internal simulation of motor actions: psychophysical evidence from pointing movements with the dominant and non-dominant arm. Chronobiol Int 2010; 27:620-39. [PMID: 20524805 DOI: 10.3109/07420521003664205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
It is well known that circadian rhythms modulate human physiology and behavior at various levels. However, chronobiological data concerning mental and sensorimotor states of motor actions are still lacking in the literature. In the present study, we examined the effects of time-of-day on two important aspects of the human motor behavior: prediction and laterality. Motor prediction was experimentally investigated by means of imagined movements and laterality by comparing the difference in temporal performance between right and left arm movements. Ten healthy participants had to actually perform or to imagine performing arm-pointing movements between two targets at different hours of the day (i.e., 08:00, 11:00, 14:00, 17:00, 20:00, and 23:00 h). Executed and imagined movements were accomplished with both the right and left arm. We found that both imagined and executed arm pointing movements significantly fluctuated through the day. Furthermore, the accuracy of motor prediction, investigated by the temporal discrepancy between executed and imagined movements, was significantly better in the afternoon (i.e., 14:00, 17:00, and 20:00 h) than morning (08:00 and 11:00 h) and evening (23:00 h). Our results also revealed that laterality was not stable throughout the day. Indeed, the smallest temporal differences between the two arms appeared at 08:00 and 23:00 h, whereas the largest ones occurred at the end of the morning (11:00 h). The daily variation of motor imagery may suggest that internal predictive models are flexible entities that are continuously updated throughout the day. Likewise, the variations in temporal performance between the right and the left arm during the day may indicate a relative independence of the two body sides in terms of circadian rhythms. In general, our findings suggest that cognitive (i.e., mental imagery) and motor (i.e., laterality) states of human behavior are modulated by circadian rhythms.
Collapse
|
229
|
Reed CL. EVIDENCE FOR MOVEMENT PREPROGRAMMING AND ON LINE CONTROL IN DIFFERENTIALLY IMPAIRED PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON'S DISEASE. Cogn Neuropsychol 2010; 15:723-45. [DOI: 10.1080/026432998381078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
|
230
|
Mahon BZ, Caramazza A. CONSTRAINING QUESTIONS ABOUT THE ORGANISATION AND REPRESENTATION OF CONCEPTUAL KNOWLEDGE. Cogn Neuropsychol 2010; 20:433-50. [DOI: 10.1080/02643290342000014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
231
|
Different motor imagery modes following brain damage. Cortex 2010; 46:1016-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2008] [Revised: 07/20/2009] [Accepted: 08/03/2009] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
232
|
Bufalari I, Sforza A, Cesari P, Aglioti SM, Fourkas AD. Motor imagery beyond the joint limits: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Biol Psychol 2010; 85:283-90. [PMID: 20688131 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.07.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/02/2010] [Revised: 07/22/2010] [Accepted: 07/23/2010] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The processes and neural bases used for motor imagery are also used for the actual execution of correspondent movements. Humans, however, can imagine movements they cannot perform. Here we explored whether plausibility of movements is mapped on the corticospinal motor system and whether the process is influenced by visuomotor vs. kinesthetic-motor first person imagery strategy. Healthy subjects imagined performing possible or biomechanically impossible right index finger movements during single pulse TMS of the left motor cortex. We found an increase of corticospinal excitability during motor imagery which was higher for impossible than possible movements and specific for the muscle involved in the actual execution of the imagined movement. We expand our previous action observation studies, suggesting that the plausibility of a movement is computed in regions upstream the primary motor cortex, and that motor imagery is a higher-order process not fully constrained by the rules that govern motor execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I Bufalari
- Department of Psychology, University of Rome La Sapienza, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Rome, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
233
|
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Das Mentale Training im Sinne der planmäßig wiederholten Vorstellung einer motorischen Fertigkeit ist ein zentraler Gegenstand der sportwissenschaftlichen Forschung und eine wichtige Trainingsergänzung in der Sportpraxis. Für das bessere Verständnis der Trainingswirkung wird in diesem Beitrag vorgeschlagen, Mentales Training als Simulation zu verstehen. Der Begriff der Simulation bezieht sich, erstens, auf die neuronale Ebene, also der Aktivierung von motorischen Arealen während der Bewegungsvorstellung. Zweitens, auf die motorische Kontrollebene und der Umsetzung durch interne Modelle, wobei das Vorwärtsmodell als Emulator gesehen wird. Drittens, auf die Wirkungsweise des Mentalen Trainings mit einer Fokussierung auf die Korrektur der zentralen Prozesse anhand der simulierten Rückmeldungen des Emulators. Der Ansatz der Simulation lässt sich auf andere mentale Zustände, die motorische Handlungen abbilden, übertragen und bietet somit einen generellen Erklärungsansatz für motorische Leistungsveränderungen, die durch kognitive Prozesse verursacht werden. Zudem deckt der Simulationsbegriff die flexible und phänomenale Sicht des mental Trainierenden ab. Der Ansatz der Simulation bietet auf verschiedenen Ebenen Vorhersagen, die zukünftige Untersuchungen in diesem Bereich fruchtbar anregen sollten.
Collapse
|
234
|
Welfringer A, Leifert-Fiebach G, Babinsky R, Brandt T. Visuomotorische Imaginationstherapie in der Neglektrehabilitation – Grundlagen, Vorgehen und Falldarstellungen. ZEITSCHRIFT FUR NEUROPSYCHOLOGIE 2010. [DOI: 10.1024/1016-264x/a000009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Mentales Training findet vermehrt in der neurologischen Rehabilitation Einsatz. Neglekpatienten mit repräsentationalen Störungen, u. a. des eigenen Körpers, könnten von einer visuomotorischen Imaginationstherapie profitieren. Das therapeutische Vorgehen für die praktische Anwendung in der Neglektrehabilitation wird vorgestellt bezüglich (1) Patientenauswahl, (2) Testdiagnostik, (3) Messung der Imaginationsfähigkeit, (4) Psychoedukation, (5) Setting, (6) Therapiemanual, (7) Frequenz und Komplexitätsgrade, (8) Therapieprotokoll, (9) Elektromyographie sowie (10) Eigentraining. Zwei Fallbeispiele verdeutlichen wie eine visuomotorische Imaginationstherapie selbst im chronischen Krankheitsstadium zu Funktionsverbesserungen führen und bei subakutem personalem Neglekt die Körperwahrnehmung beeinflusst werden kann.
Collapse
|
235
|
Wei G, Luo J. Sport expert's motor imagery: Functional imaging of professional motor skills and simple motor skills. Brain Res 2010; 1341:52-62. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 136] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2008] [Revised: 07/26/2009] [Accepted: 08/05/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
236
|
Vieira AB. Grammatical equivalents of Palaeolithic tools: a hypothesis. Theory Biosci 2010; 129:203-10. [DOI: 10.1007/s12064-010-0094-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2009] [Accepted: 11/30/2009] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
237
|
Tomasino B, Weiss PH, Fink GR. To move or not to move: imperatives modulate action-related verb processing in the motor system. Neuroscience 2010; 169:246-58. [PMID: 20420884 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2010] [Revised: 04/12/2010] [Accepted: 04/18/2010] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
It has been suggested that the processing of action-related words involves activation of the motor circuitry. Using fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging), the current study further explored the interaction between action and language by investigating whether the linguistic context, in which an action word occurs, modulates motor circuitry activity related to the processing of action words. To this end, we examined whether the presentation of hand action-related verbs as positive or negative imperatives, for example, "Do grasp" or "Don't write," modulates neural activity in the hand area of primary motor cortex (M1) or premotor cortex (Pm). Subjects (n = 19) were asked to read silently the imperative phrases, in which both meaningful action verbs and meaningless pseudo-verbs were presented, and to decide whether they made sense (lexical decision task). At the behavioral level, response times in the lexical decision task were significantly longer for negative, compared to positive, imperatives. At the neural level, activity was differentially decreased by action verbs presented as negative imperatives for the premotor and the primary motor cortex of both hemispheres. The data suggest that context (here: positive vs. negative imperatives), in which an action verb is encountered, modulates the neural activity within key areas of the motor system. The finding implies that motor simulation (or motor planning) rather than semantic processing per se may underlie previously observed motor system activation related to action verb processing. Furthermore, the current data suggest that negative imperatives may inhibit motor simulation or motor planning processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- B Tomasino
- Cognitive Neurology Section, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Juelich, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
238
|
Wang Y, Chen H, Gong Q, Shen S, Gao Q. Analysis of functional networks involved in motor execution and motor imagery using combined hierarchical clustering analysis and independent component analysis. Magn Reson Imaging 2010; 28:653-60. [PMID: 20378292 DOI: 10.1016/j.mri.2010.02.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2009] [Revised: 12/07/2009] [Accepted: 02/08/2010] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive experiments involving motor execution (ME) and motor imagery (MI) have been intensively studied using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). However, the functional networks of a multitask paradigm which include ME and MI were not widely explored. In this article, we aimed to investigate the functional networks involved in MI and ME using a method combining the hierarchical clustering analysis (HCA) and the independent component analysis (ICA). Ten right-handed subjects were recruited to participate a multitask experiment with conditions such as visual cue, MI, ME and rest. The results showed that four activation clusters were found including parts of the visual network, ME network, the MI network and parts of the resting state network. Furthermore, the integration among these functional networks was also revealed. The findings further demonstrated that the combined HCA with ICA approach was an effective method to analyze the fMRI data of multitasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuqing Wang
- Key Laboratory for Neuroinformation of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, PR China
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
239
|
Yee E, Drucker DM, Thompson-Schill SL. fMRI-adaptation evidence of overlapping neural representations for objects related in function or manipulation. Neuroimage 2010; 50:753-63. [PMID: 20034582 PMCID: PMC2836190 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2009] [Revised: 12/01/2009] [Accepted: 12/08/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Sensorimotor-based theories of semantic memory contend that semantic information about an object is represented in the neural substrate invoked when we perceive or interact with it. We used fMRI adaptation to test this prediction, measuring brain activation as participants read pairs of words. Pairs shared function (flashlight-lantern), shape (marble-grape), both (pencil-pen), were unrelated (saucer-needle), or were identical (drill-drill). We observed adaptation for pairs with both function and shape similarity in left premotor cortex. Further, degree of function similarity was correlated with adaptation in three regions: two in the left temporal lobe (left medial temporal lobe, left middle temporal gyrus), which has been hypothesized to play a role in mutimodal integration, and one in left superior frontal gyrus. We also found that degree of manipulation (i.e., action) and function similarity were both correlated with adaptation in two regions: left premotor cortex and left intraparietal sulcus (involved in guiding actions). Additional considerations suggest that the adaptation in these two regions was driven by manipulation similarity alone; thus, these results imply that manipulation information about objects is encoded in brain regions involved in performing or guiding actions. Unexpectedly, these same two regions showed increased activation (rather than adaptation) for objects similar in shape. Overall, we found evidence (in the form of adaptation) that objects that share semantic features have overlapping representations. Further, the particular regions of overlap provide support for the existence of both sensorimotor and amodal/multimodal representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eiling Yee
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6241, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
240
|
Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, Ferlazzo F, Caltagirone C, Petrosini L. Changes in cerebello-motor connectivity during procedural learning by actual execution and observation. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 23:338-48. [PMID: 20350172 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21471] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
The cerebellum is involved in motor learning of new procedures both during actual execution of a motor task and during observational training. These processes are thought to depend on the activity of a neural network that involves the lateral cerebellum and primary motor cortex (M1). In this study, we used a twin-coil TMS technique to investigate whether execution and observation of a visuomotor procedural learning task is related to modulation of cerebello-motor connectivity. We observed that, at rest, a magnetic conditioning pulse applied over the lateral cerebellum reduced the motor-evoked potentials obtained by stimulating the contralateral M1, indicating activation of a cerebello-motor connection. Furthermore, during procedural learning, cerebellar stimulation resulted in selective facilitation, not inhibition, of contralateral M1 excitability. The effects were evident when motor learning was obtained by actual execution of the task or by observation, but they disappeared if procedural learning had already been acquired by previous observational training. These results indicate that changes in cerebello-motor connectivity occur in relation to specific phases of procedural learning, demonstrating a complex pattern of excitatory and inhibitory drives modulated across time.
Collapse
|
241
|
Frak V, Nazir T, Goyette M, Cohen H, Jeannerod M. Grip force is part of the semantic representation of manual action verbs. PLoS One 2010; 5:e9728. [PMID: 20300535 PMCID: PMC2838801 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009728] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2009] [Accepted: 02/25/2010] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor actions and action verbs activate similar cortical brain regions. A functional interference can be taken as evidence that there is a parallel treatment of these two types of information and would argue for the biological grounding of language in action. A novel approach examining the relationship between language and grip force is presented. With eyes closed and arm extended, subjects listened to words relating (verbs) or not relating (nouns) to a manual action while holding a cylinder with an integrated force sensor. There was a change in grip force when subjects heard verbs that related to manual action. Grip force increased from about 100 ms following the verb presentation, peaked at 380 ms and fell abruptly after 400 ms, signalling a possible inhibition of the motor simulation evoked by these words. These observations reveal the intimate relationship that exists between language and grasp and show that it is possible to elucidate online new aspects of sensorimotor interaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victor Frak
- Département de kinanthropologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
242
|
Mental representations of action: the neural correlates of the verbal and motor components. Brain Res 2010; 1328:89-103. [PMID: 20226773 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.082] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2009] [Revised: 02/23/2010] [Accepted: 02/27/2010] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Recent theories have hypothesized that semantic representations of action verbs and mental representations of action may be supported by partially overlapping, distributed brain networks. An fMRI experiment in healthy participants was designed to identify the common and specific regions in three different tasks from a common set of object drawings (manipulable man-made objects (MMO) and biological objects (MBO)): the generation of action words (GenA), the mental simulation of action (MSoA) and the mime of an action with the right hand (MimA). A fourth task, object naming (ON), was used as control for input/output effects. A null conjunction identified a common neural network consisting of nine regions distributed over premotor, parietal and occipital cortices. Within this common network, GenA elicited significantly more activation than either ON or MSoA in the left inferior frontal region, while MSoA elicited significantly more activation than either ON or GenA in the left superior parietal lobule. Both MSoA and GenA activated the left inferior parietal lobule more than ON. Furthermore, the left superior parietal cortex was activated to a greater extent by MMO than by MBO regardless of the tasks. These results suggest that action-denoting verbs and motor representations of the same actions activate a common frontal-parietal network. The left inferior parietal cortex and the left superior parietal cortex are likely to be involved in the retrieval of spatial-temporal features of object manipulation; the former might relate to the grasping and manipulation of any object while the latter might be linked to specific object-related gestures.
Collapse
|
243
|
Boord P, Craig A, Tran Y, Nguyen H. Discrimination of left and right leg motor imagery for brain–computer interfaces. Med Biol Eng Comput 2010; 48:343-50. [DOI: 10.1007/s11517-010-0579-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2009] [Accepted: 01/07/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
244
|
Abstract
AbstractTwo issues have been raised. The first concerns whether consciousness is attached to a given type of action. It is argued that purposive actions are represented before being executed and that motor representations can either remain implicit or become explicit according to the task. The second issue concerns whether or not mental imagery of action is independent from action. Recent evidence showing the commonality of neural mechanisms for motor imagery and action, respectively, goes against the idea of independent processes.
Collapse
|
245
|
Response intention and imagery processes: Locus, interaction, and contribution to motor learning. Behav Brain Sci 2010. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x00043880] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
AbstractBy way of commentary on Jeannerod (1994), we propose that (1) intention, response imagery, and actual response processes carry equal weight in inferring from one process to another, (2) memory networks control intention, which interacts with imagery-based processing to control response imagery, and (3) response imagery will demonstrate learning effects better when imagery reconstruction and elaboration are emphasized and neutral retention tests are utilized.
Collapse
|
246
|
Alternative origins of motor images. Behav Brain Sci 2010. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x00043879] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
AbstractJeannerod (1994) advocates studying motor images to understand the representation of action. We identify an unsettled issue that complicates the use of motor images to study the representation of action and present some evidence for a clear absence of equivalence between motor imagery and movement preparation. We then elaborate and emphasize the relevance of the mental practice literature, which was introduced in the first round of commentary. Finally, we suggest two methods that will allow motor imagery theorists to address these points.
Collapse
|
247
|
Abstract
AbstractJeannerod's target article and a number of commentators stressed the necessity of distinguishing between motor representation and perceptual images. A patient with a lesion of the central somatosensory pathways allowed us to test for this distinction. The outcome suggests that motor images may not be visual and that the “pragmatic” framework proposed by Jeannerod for vision can apply to other sensory modalities.
Collapse
|
248
|
Guillot A, Tolleron C, Collet C. Does motor imagery enhance stretching and flexibility? J Sports Sci 2010; 28:291-8. [DOI: 10.1080/02640410903473828] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
249
|
|
250
|
Lorey B, Pilgramm S, Walter B, Stark R, Munzert J, Zentgraf K. Your mind's hand: motor imagery of pointing movements with different accuracy. Neuroimage 2009; 49:3239-47. [PMID: 19948224 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2009] [Revised: 11/04/2009] [Accepted: 11/15/2009] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Jeannerod (2001) postulated that motor control and motor simulation states are functionally equivalent. If this is the case, the specifically relevant task parameters in online motor control should also be represented in motor imagery. We tested whether the different spatial accuracy demands of manual pointing movements are reflected on a neural level in motor imagery. During functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning, 23 participants imagined hand movements that differed systematically in terms of pointing accuracy needs (i.e., none, low, high). In a low-accuracy condition, two big squares were presented visually prior to the imagery phase. These squares had to be pointed at alternately on a mental level. In the high-accuracy condition, two little squares had to be hit. As expected on the basis of speed-accuracy trade-off principles, results showed that participants required more time when accuracy of the imagined movements increased. The fMRI results showed a stepwise increase in activation in the anterior cerebellum and the anterior part of the superior parietal lobe (SPL) with rising accuracy needs. Moreover, we found increased activation of the anterior part of the SPL and of the dorsal premotor cortex (dPMC) when imagery included a square (i.e., in the low- and high-accuracy conditions) compared to the no-square condition. These areas have also been discussed in relation to online motor control, suggesting that specific task parameters relevant in the domain of motor control are also coded in motor imagery. We suggest that the functional equivalence of action states is due mostly to internal estimations of the expected sensory feedback in both motor control and motor imagery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Britta Lorey
- University of Giessen, Germany, Institute for Sports Science, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|