301
|
Transfer after Dual n-Back Training Depends on Striatal Activation Change. J Neurosci 2016; 36:10198-213. [PMID: 27683914 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2305-15.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2015] [Accepted: 08/02/2016] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
UNLABELLED The dual n-back working memory (WM) training paradigm (comprising auditory and visual stimuli) has gained much attention since studies have shown widespread transfer effects. By including a multimodal dual-task component, the task is demanding to the human cognitive system. We investigated whether dual n-back training improves general cognitive resources or a task-specific WM updating process in participants. We expected: (1) widespread transfer effects and the recruitment of a common neuronal network by the training and the transfer tasks and (2) narrower transfer results and that a common activation network alone would not produce transfer, but instead an activation focus on the striatum, which is associated with WM updating processes. The training group showed transfer to an untrained dual-modality WM updating task, but not to single-task versions of the training or the transfer task. They also showed diminished neuronal overlap between the training and the transfer task from pretest to posttest and an increase in striatal activation in both tasks. Furthermore, we found an association between the striatal activation increase and behavioral improvement. The control groups showed no transfer and no change in the amount of activation overlap or in striatal activation from pretest to posttest. We conclude that, instead of improving general cognitive resources (which would have required a transfer effect to all transfer tasks and that a frontal activation overlap between the tasks produced transfer), dual n-back training improved a task-specific process: WM updating of stimuli from two modalities. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The current study allows for a better understanding of the cognitive and neural effects of working memory (WM) training and transfer. It shows that dual n-back training mainly improves specific processes of WM updating, and this improvement leads to narrow transfer effects to tasks involving the same processes. On a neuronal level this is accompanied by increased neural activation in the striatum that is related to WM updating. The current findings challenge the view that dual n-back training provokes a general boosting of the WM system and of its neural underpinnings located in frontoparietal brain regions. Instead, the findings imply the relevance of task-specific brain regions which are involved in important cognitive processes during training and transfer tasks.
Collapse
|
302
|
Reevaluating the effectiveness of n-back training on transfer through the Bayesian lens: Support for the null. Psychon Bull Rev 2016; 23:306-16. [PMID: 26082280 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0865-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
A recent meta-analysis by Au et al. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 366-377, (2015) reviewed the n-back training paradigm for working memory (WM) and evaluated whether (when aggregating across existing studies) there was evidence that gains obtained for training tasks transferred to gains in fluid intelligence (Gf). Their results revealed an overall effect size of g = 0.24 for the effect of n-back training on Gf. We reexamine the data through a Bayesian lens, to evaluate the relative strength of the evidence for the alternative versus null hypotheses, contingent on the type of control condition used. We find that studies using a noncontact (passive) control group strongly favor the alternative hypothesis that training leads to transfer but that studies using active-control groups show modest evidence in favor of the null. We discuss these findings in the context of placebo effects.
Collapse
|
303
|
Gathercole SE, Dunning DL, Holmes J, Norris D. Working memory training involves learning new skills. JOURNAL OF MEMORY AND LANGUAGE 2016; 105:19-42. [PMID: 31235992 PMCID: PMC6591133 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2018.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 123] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
We present a new framework characterizing training-induced changes in WM as the acquisition of novel cognitive routines akin to learning a new skill. Predictions were tested in three studies analyzing the transfer between WM tasks following WM training. Study 1 reports a meta-analysis establishing substantial transfer when trained and untrained tasks shared either a serial recall, complex span or backward span paradigm. Transfer was weaker for serial recall of verbal than visuo-spatial material, suggesting that this paradigm is served by an existing verbal STM system and does not require a new routine. Re-analysis of published WM training data in Study 2 showed that transfer was restricted to tasks sharing properties proposed to require new routines. In a re-analysis of data from four studies, Study 3 demonstrated that transfer was greatest for children with higher fluid cognitive abilities. These findings suggest that development of new routines depends on general cognitive resources and that they can only be applied to other similarly-structured tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan E. Gathercole
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 7EF, England, United Kingdom
| | - Darren L. Dunning
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 7EF, England, United Kingdom
| | - Joni Holmes
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 7EF, England, United Kingdom
| | - Dennis Norris
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 7EF, England, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
304
|
Au J, Katz B, Buschkuehl M, Bunarjo K, Senger T, Zabel C, Jaeggi SM, Jonides J. Enhancing Working Memory Training with Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci 2016; 28:1419-32. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00979] [Citation(s) in RCA: 94] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
Abstract
Working memory (WM) is a fundamental cognitive ability that supports complex thought but is limited in capacity. Thus, WM training interventions have become very popular as a means of potentially improving WM-related skills. Another promising intervention that has gained increasing traction in recent years is transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a noninvasive form of brain stimulation that can modulate cortical excitability and temporarily increase brain plasticity. As such, it has the potential to boost learning and enhance performance on cognitive tasks. This study assessed the efficacy of tDCS to supplement WM training. Sixty-two participants were randomized to receive either right prefrontal, left prefrontal, or sham stimulation with concurrent visuospatial WM training over the course of seven training sessions. Results showed that tDCS enhanced training performance, which was strikingly preserved several months after training completion. Furthermore, we observed stronger effects when tDCS was spaced over a weekend break relative to consecutive daily training, and we also demonstrated selective transfer in the right prefrontal group to nontrained tasks of visual and spatial WM. These findings shed light on how tDCS may be leveraged as a tool to enhance performance on WM-intensive learning tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacky Au
- 1University of California, Irvine
- 2MIND Research Institute, Irvine, CA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
305
|
Dynamics of the Human Structural Connectome Underlying Working Memory Training. J Neurosci 2016; 36:4056-66. [PMID: 27053212 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1973-15.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2015] [Accepted: 02/04/2016] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
UNLABELLED Brain region-specific changes have been demonstrated with a variety of cognitive training interventions. The effect of cognitive training on brain subnetworks in humans, however, remains largely unknown, with studies limited to functional networks. Here, we used a well-established working memory training program and state-of-the art neuroimaging methods in 40 healthy adults (21 females, mean age 26.5 years). Near and far-transfer training effects were assessed using computerized working memory and executive function tasks. Adaptive working memory training led to improvement on (non)trained working memory tasks and generalization to tasks of reasoning and inhibition. Graph theoretical analysis of the structural (white matter) network connectivity ("connectome") revealed increased global integration within a frontoparietal attention network following adaptive working memory training compared with the nonadaptive group. Furthermore, the impact on the outcome of graph theoretical analyses of different white matter metrics to infer "connection strength" was evaluated. Increased efficiency of the frontoparietal network was best captured when using connection strengths derived from MR metrics that are thought to be more sensitive to differences in myelination (putatively indexed by the [quantitative] longitudinal relaxation rate, R1) than previously used diffusion MRI metrics (fractional anisotropy or fiber-tracking recovered streamlines). Our findings emphasize the critical role of specific microstructural markers in providing important hints toward the mechanisms underpinning training-induced plasticity that may drive working memory improvement in clinical populations. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT This is the first study to explore training-induced changes in the structural connectome using a well-controlled design to examine cognitive training with up-to-date neuroimaging methods. We found changes in global integration based on white matter connectivity within a frontoparietal attention network following adaptive working memory training compared with a nonadaptive comparison group. Furthermore, the impact of different diffusion MR metrics and more specific markers of white matter on the graph theoretical findings was evaluated. An increase in network global efficiency following working memory training was best captured when connection strengths were weighted by MR relaxation rates (influenced by myelination). These results are important for the optimization of cognitive training programs for healthy individuals and people with brain disease.
Collapse
|
306
|
|
307
|
Otto MW, Eastman A, Lo S, Hearon BA, Bickel WK, Zvolensky M, Smits JAJ, Doan SN. Anxiety sensitivity and working memory capacity: Risk factors and targets for health behavior promotion. Clin Psychol Rev 2016; 49:67-78. [PMID: 27611632 DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 88] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2015] [Revised: 06/03/2016] [Accepted: 07/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Understanding the nature and influence of specific risk profiles is increasingly important for health behavior promotion. The purpose of this article is to document the value of two factors-anxiety sensitivity (AS) and working memory capacity (WMC)-for enhancing risk for the initiation and/or maintenance of a range of negative health behaviors. AS is a distress-related risk factor that potentiates avoidance/coping motivations for negative health behaviors. Stress provides the conditions for negative somatic and affective states, and AS amplifies the aversiveness of these experiences and correspondingly hinders adaptive functioning. In contrast, low WMC is hypothesized to exert its effect by decreasing the capacity to filter out current temptations, attenuating a focus on longer-term goals and impairing the application of relevant coping skills at times of stress. In this review, we provide conceptual models for the separate roles of high AS and low WMC in negative health behaviors, review the influence of these factors on specific health behavior exemplars (eating behaviors/obesity, physical activity, smoking, alcohol use, and sleep promotion), provide preliminary evidence for their value as independent treatment targets for health-behavior promotion, and encourage specific research directions in relation to these variables.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael W Otto
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, USA.
| | - Abraham Eastman
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, USA
| | - Stephen Lo
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, USA
| | | | - Warren K Bickel
- Addiction Recovery Research Center, Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, USA
| | - Michael Zvolensky
- University of Houston, USA; The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, USA
| | - Jasper A J Smits
- Department of Psychology and Institute for Mental Health Research, The University of Texas at Austin, USA
| | - Stacey N Doan
- Department of Psychology, Claremont McKenna College, USA
| |
Collapse
|
308
|
Foroughi CK, Monfort SS, Paczynski M, McKnight PE, Greenwood PM. Placebo effects in cognitive training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113:7470-4. [PMID: 27325761 PMCID: PMC4941515 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1601243113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 154] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Although a large body of research shows that general cognitive ability is heritable and stable in young adults, there is recent evidence that fluid intelligence can be heightened with cognitive training. Many researchers, however, have questioned the methodology of the cognitive-training studies reporting improvements in fluid intelligence: specifically, the role of placebo effects. We designed a procedure to intentionally induce a placebo effect via overt recruitment in an effort to evaluate the role of placebo effects in fluid intelligence gains from cognitive training. Individuals who self-selected into the placebo group by responding to a suggestive flyer showed improvements after a single, 1-h session of cognitive training that equates to a 5- to 10-point increase on a standard IQ test. Controls responding to a nonsuggestive flyer showed no improvement. These findings provide an alternative explanation for effects observed in the cognitive-training literature and the brain-training industry, revealing the need to account for confounds in future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cyrus K Foroughi
- Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030
| | - Samuel S Monfort
- Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030
| | - Martin Paczynski
- Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030
| | | | - P M Greenwood
- Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030
| |
Collapse
|
309
|
Melby-Lervåg M, Redick TS, Hulme C. Working Memory Training Does Not Improve Performance on Measures of Intelligence or Other Measures of "Far Transfer": Evidence From a Meta-Analytic Review. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2016; 11:512-34. [PMID: 27474138 PMCID: PMC4968033 DOI: 10.1177/1745691616635612] [Citation(s) in RCA: 495] [Impact Index Per Article: 55.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
Abstract
It has been claimed that working memory training programs produce diverse beneficial effects. This article presents a meta-analysis of working memory training studies (with a pretest-posttest design and a control group) that have examined transfer to other measures (nonverbal ability, verbal ability, word decoding, reading comprehension, or arithmetic; 87 publications with 145 experimental comparisons). Immediately following training there were reliable improvements on measures of intermediate transfer (verbal and visuospatial working memory). For measures of far transfer (nonverbal ability, verbal ability, word decoding, reading comprehension, arithmetic) there was no convincing evidence of any reliable improvements when working memory training was compared with a treated control condition. Furthermore, mediation analyses indicated that across studies, the degree of improvement on working memory measures was not related to the magnitude of far-transfer effects found. Finally, analysis of publication bias shows that there is no evidential value from the studies of working memory training using treated controls. The authors conclude that working memory training programs appear to produce short-term, specific training effects that do not generalize to measures of "real-world" cognitive skills. These results seriously question the practical and theoretical importance of current computerized working memory programs as methods of training working memory skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Charles Hulme
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, and Department of Special Needs Education, University of Oslo
| |
Collapse
|
310
|
Elmasry J, Loo C, Martin D. A systematic review of transcranial electrical stimulation combined with cognitive training. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2016; 33:263-78. [PMID: 25624425 DOI: 10.3233/rnn-140473] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Transcranial Electrical Stimulation (tES) methods have been shown to enhance performance across a range of cognitive tasks. It is thought that tES can be used to enhance the treatment-effects of cognitive training (CT), leading to lasting improvements in neurocognitive function. Recently, a small number of studies have investigated the effects of tES combined with CT in healthy and cognitively impaired subjects. OBJECTIVE To evaluate the effects of tES + CT on both CT task performance and on non-trained cognitive outcomes. METHODS A systematic review was conducted in accordance with PRISMA guidelines. Databases (PsycINFO, EMBASE, PubMed and Medline) were searched for all randomized, controlled and naturalistic prospective studies up until June 2014, combining tES and CT. RESULTS 13 studies comprising 465 participants met the inclusion criteria. Findings indicated that tES + CT enhanced performance on the majority of CT tasks. The effects on non-trained tasks were mixed, with some evidence for improvements in working memory, cognitive control, approximate number sense and arithmetic processing. CONCLUSIONS tES + CT enhances performance on CT tasks across a range of cognitive functions. Preliminary evidence suggests that tES may also increase transfer effects to non-trained tasks in some domains. Recommendations for future studies are provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Elmasry
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.,The Black Dog Institute, Sydney, Australia
| | - Colleen Loo
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.,The Black Dog Institute, Sydney, Australia.,St George Hospital, Sydney, Australia
| | - Donel Martin
- School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.,The Black Dog Institute, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
311
|
Stephens JA, Berryhill ME. Older Adults Improve on Everyday Tasks after Working Memory Training and Neurostimulation. Brain Stimul 2016; 9:553-9. [PMID: 27178247 PMCID: PMC4957521 DOI: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 96] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2015] [Revised: 03/18/2016] [Accepted: 04/01/2016] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Aging is associated with decline in executive function (EF), upper-level cognitive abilities such as planning, problem solving, and working memory (WM). This decline is associated with age-related volume loss and reduced functional connectivity in the frontal lobes. Cognitive training interventions aim to counter these losses, but often fail to elicit benefits beyond improvements on trained tasks. Recent interventions pairing WM training with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) have improved WM and elicited transfer to untrained EF tasks. Limitations in previous work include exclusive use of laboratory-based computer training and testing and poor characterization of the mechanism(s) of durable tDCS-linked change. OBJECTIVE/HYPOTHESIS To determine if tDCS-linked WM training improves performance on ecologically valid transfer measures administered in participants' homes. To explore intervention-based changes using neuroimaging (fNIRS) and genotyping (COMT val158met). METHODS 90 healthy older adult participants completed 5 sessions of WM training paired with tDCS (Sham, 1 mA tDCS, 2 mA tDCS; 15 min). At follow-up, we assessed performance change on laboratory-based and ecologically valid tasks. RESULTS All participants showed improvement on trained tasks. Importantly, 2 mA of tDCS induced significantly greater far transfer gains after 1 month without contact. Gains were observed on standard far transfer tasks along with ecologically valid far transfer tasks, and stimulation was well tolerated by all participants. FNIRS and genotyping results were less conclusive, but provide promising avenues for future research initiatives. CONCLUSION These findings highlight the translational value for tDCS-based interventions in healthy older adults interested in maintaining cognitive function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaclyn A Stephens
- Department of Psychology, Program in Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Nevada, Reno, NV 89557.
| | - Marian E Berryhill
- Department of Psychology, Program in Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Nevada, Reno, NV 89557
| |
Collapse
|
312
|
Wang P, Liu HH, Zhu XT, Meng T, Li HJ, Zuo XN. Action Video Game Training for Healthy Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study. Front Psychol 2016; 7:907. [PMID: 27378996 PMCID: PMC4911405 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00907] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2016] [Accepted: 06/01/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Action video game (AVG) has attracted increasing attention from both the public and from researchers. More and more studies found video game training improved a variety of cognitive functions. However, it remains controversial whether healthy adults can benefit from AVG training, and whether young and older adults benefit similarly from AVG training. In the present study, we aimed to quantitatively assess the AVG training effect on the cognitive ability of adults and to compare the training effects on young and older adults by conducting a meta-analysis on previous findings. We systematically searched video game training studies published between January 1986 and July 2015. Twenty studies were included in the present meta-analysis, for a total of 313 participants included in the training group and 323 participants in the control group. The results demonstrate that healthy adults achieve moderate benefit from AVG training in overall cognitive ability and moderate to small benefit in specific cognitive domains. In contrast, young adults gain more benefits from AVG training than older adults in both overall cognition and specific cognitive domains. Age, education, and some methodological factors, such as the session duration, session number, total training duration, and control group type, modulated the training effects. These meta-analytic findings provide evidence that AVG training may serve as an efficient way to improve the cognitive performance of healthy adults. We also discussed several directions for future AVG training studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ping Wang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China; University of Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| | - Han-Hui Liu
- Youth Work Department, Chinese Youth University for Political Studies Beijing, China
| | - Xing-Ting Zhu
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Beijing, China
| | - Tian Meng
- Youth Work Department, Chinese Youth University for Political Studies Beijing, China
| | - Hui-Jie Li
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Beijing, China
| | - Xi-Nian Zuo
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
313
|
Heppe H, Kohler A, Fleddermann MT, Zentgraf K. The Relationship between Expertise in Sports, Visuospatial, and Basic Cognitive Skills. Front Psychol 2016; 7:904. [PMID: 27378994 PMCID: PMC4911412 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2016] [Accepted: 06/01/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Team sports place high demands on visuospatial and other cognitive skills. However, there is a lack of research on visuospatial skills of elite athletes and there are heterogeneous results on basic cognitive skills of this population. Therefore, this series of studies tested different cognitive skills in elite team sports athletes. In Experiment 1, elite athletes were compared to recreational athletes, but no differences were observed between the groups in choice response time (CRT) and mental rotation (MR). To see if differences could be observed when the tested groups had a greater difference in expertise and more representative stimuli, in Experiment 2, we tested CRT and MR of elite athletes who had higher level of expertise, and we also used three-dimensional human stimuli. Overall, we still found no differences in MR; however, elite athletes did have shorter CRTs. In Experiment 3, instead of testing MR, we compared elite athletes' and recreational athletes' basic cognitive skills, such as processing speed, letter readout speed, memory span, and sustained attention. We found that elite athletes only performed better in sustained attention. Building on this data, in a supplementary analysis (Experiment 4) we tested whether MR and CRTs are correlated with basic cognitive skills. Results show that processing speed is the best predictor for MR, whereas letter readout speed explains most of the variance in CRTs. Finally, we discuss these findings against the backdrop of expertise and offer implications for future studies on mental rotation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Holger Heppe
- Department of Human Performance and Training in Sports, Institute of Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of MünsterMünster, Germany; Otto Creutzfeldt Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of MünsterMünster, Germany
| | - Axel Kohler
- Otto Creutzfeldt Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of MünsterMünster, Germany; Institute of Psychology, University of MünsterMünster, Germany
| | - Marie-Therese Fleddermann
- Department of Human Performance and Training in Sports, Institute of Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Münster Münster, Germany
| | - Karen Zentgraf
- Department of Human Performance and Training in Sports, Institute of Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of MünsterMünster, Germany; Otto Creutzfeldt Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of MünsterMünster, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
314
|
Li B, Zhu X, Hou J, Chen T, Wang P, Li J. Combined Cognitive Training vs. Memory Strategy Training in Healthy Older Adults. Front Psychol 2016; 7:834. [PMID: 27375521 PMCID: PMC4896109 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00834] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2016] [Accepted: 05/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
UNLABELLED As mnemonic utilization deficit in older adults associates with age-related decline in executive function, we hypothesized that memory strategy training combined with executive function training might induce larger training effect in memory and broader training effects in non-memory outcomes than pure memory training. The present study compared the effects of combined cognitive training (executive function training plus memory strategy training) to pure memory strategy training. Forty healthy older adults were randomly assigned to a combined cognitive training group or a memory strategy training group. A control group receiving no training was also included. Combined cognitive training group received 16 sessions of training (eight sessions of executive function training followed by eight sessions of memory strategy training). Memory training group received 16 sessions of memory strategy training. The results partly supported our hypothesis in that indeed improved performance on executive function was only found in combined training group, whereas memory performance increased less in combined training compared to memory strategy group. Results suggest that combined cognitive training may be less efficient than pure memory training in memory outcomes, though the influences from insufficient training time and less closeness between trained executive function and working memory could not be excluded; however it has broader training effects in non-memory outcomes. CLINICAL TRIAL REGISTRATION www.chictr.org.cn, identifier ChiCTR-OON-16007793.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bing Li
- Center on Aging Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China; English Department, Faculty of Humanities and Educational Sciences, Technische Universität BraunschweigBraunschweig, Germany
| | - Xinyi Zhu
- Center on Aging Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China; University of Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| | - Jianhua Hou
- Center on Aging Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China; University of Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| | - Tingji Chen
- Center on Aging Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China; Human Information Processing Laboratory, School of Social Science and Humanities, University of TampereTampere, Finland
| | - Pengyun Wang
- Center on Aging Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Beijing, China
| | - Juan Li
- Center on Aging Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China; State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
315
|
There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: A reply to Au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychon Bull Rev 2016; 23:324-30. [PMID: 26082279 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0862-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 117] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The possible cognitive benefits of working memory training programs have been the subject of intense interest and controversy. Recently two meta-analyses have claimed that working memory training can be effective in enhancing cognitive skills in adulthood (Au et al. Behavioural Brain Research 228:(1) 107-115, 2014) and stemming cognitive decline in old age (Karbach & Verhaeghen Psychological Science 25:2027-2037, 2014). The current article critically evaluates these claims. We argue that these meta-analyses produce misleading results because of (1) biases in the studies included, (2) a failure to take account of baseline differences when calculating effect sizes, and (3) a failure to emphasize the difference between studies with treated versus untreated control groups. We present new meta-analyses and conclude that there is no convincing evidence that working memory training produces general cognitive benefits.
Collapse
|
316
|
There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is NOT effective: A reply to Melby-Lervåg and Hulme (2015). Psychon Bull Rev 2016; 23:331-7. [PMID: 26518308 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0967-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Our recent meta-analysis concluded that training on working memory can improve performance on tests of fluid intelligence (Au et al., Psychon Bull Rev, 22(2), 366-377, 2015). Melby-Lervåg and Hulme (Psychon Bull Rev, doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0862-z ) challenge this conclusion on the grounds that it did not take into consideration baseline differences on a by-study level and that the effects were primarily driven by purportedly less rigorous studies that did not include active control groups. Their re-analysis shows that accounting for baseline differences produces a statistically significant, but considerably smaller, overall effect size (g = 0.13 vs g = 0.24 in Au et al.), which loses significance after excluding studies without active controls. The present report demonstrates that evidence of impact variation by the active/passive nature of control groups is ambiguous and also reveals important discrepancies between Melby-Lervåg and Hulme's analysis and our original meta-analysis in terms of the coding and organization of data that account for the discrepant effect sizes. We demonstrate that there is in fact no evidence that the type of control group per se moderates the effects of working memory training on measures of fluid intelligence and reaffirm the original conclusions in Au et al., which are robust to multiple methods of calculating effect size, including the one proposed by Melby-Lervåg and Hulme.
Collapse
|
317
|
Toril P, Reales JM, Mayas J, Ballesteros S. Video Game Training Enhances Visuospatial Working Memory and Episodic Memory in Older Adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:206. [PMID: 27199723 PMCID: PMC4859063 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00206] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2016] [Accepted: 04/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In this longitudinal intervention study with experimental and control groups, we investigated the effects of video game training on the visuospatial working memory (WM) and episodic memory of healthy older adults. Participants were 19 volunteer older adults, who received 15 1-h video game training sessions with a series of video games selected from a commercial package (Lumosity), and a control group of 20 healthy older adults. The results showed that the performance of the trainees improved significantly in all the practiced video games. Most importantly, we found significant enhancements after training in the trained group and no change in the control group in two computerized tasks designed to assess visuospatial WM, namely the Corsi blocks task and the Jigsaw puzzle task. The episodic memory and short-term memory of the trainees also improved. Gains in some WM and episodic memory tasks were maintained during a 3-month follow-up period. These results suggest that the aging brain still retains some degree of plasticity, and that video game training might be an effective intervention tool to improve WM and other cognitive functions in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pilar Toril
- Studies of Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology II, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain
| | - José M Reales
- Studies of Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain; Department of Methodology of the Behavioral Sciences, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain
| | - Julia Mayas
- Studies of Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology II, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain
| | - Soledad Ballesteros
- Studies of Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology II, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED)Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
318
|
A simultaneous examination of two forms of working memory training: Evidence for near transfer only. Mem Cognit 2016; 44:1014-37. [DOI: 10.3758/s13421-016-0616-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
319
|
Bak TH, Long MR, Vega-Mendoza M, Sorace A. Novelty, Challenge, and Practice: The Impact of Intensive Language Learning on Attentional Functions. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0153485. [PMID: 27120179 PMCID: PMC4847793 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2015] [Accepted: 03/30/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
We investigated the impact of a short intensive language course on attentional functions. We examined 33 participants of a one-week Scottish Gaelic course and compared them to 34 controls: 16 active controls who participated in courses of comparable duration and intensity but not involving foreign language learning and 18 passive controls who followed their usual routines. Participants completed auditory tests of attentional inhibition and switching. There was no difference between the groups in any measures at the beginning of the course. At the end of the course, a significant improvement in attention switching was observed in the language group (p < .001) but not the control group (p = .127), independent of the age of participants (18–78 years). Half of the language participants (n = 17) were retested nine months after their course. All those who practiced Gaelic 5 hours or more per week improved from their baseline performance. In contrast, those who practiced 4 hours or fewer showed an inconsistent pattern: some improved while others stayed the same or deteriorated. Our results suggest that even a short period of intensive language learning can modulate attentional functions and that all age groups can benefit from this effect. Moreover, these short-term effects can be maintained through continuous practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas H. Bak
- Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
- Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology (CCACE), Edinburgh, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Madeleine R. Long
- Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
- Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology (CCACE), Edinburgh, United Kingdom
- Department of Linguistics, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Mariana Vega-Mendoza
- Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Antonella Sorace
- Department of Linguistics, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
320
|
Moreau D, Kirk IJ, Waldie KE. Seven Pervasive Statistical Flaws in Cognitive Training Interventions. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:153. [PMID: 27148010 PMCID: PMC4830817 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00153] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2016] [Accepted: 03/28/2016] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
The prospect of enhancing cognition is undoubtedly among the most exciting research questions currently bridging psychology, neuroscience, and evidence-based medicine. Yet, convincing claims in this line of work stem from designs that are prone to several shortcomings, thus threatening the credibility of training-induced cognitive enhancement. Here, we present seven pervasive statistical flaws in intervention designs: (i) lack of power; (ii) sampling error; (iii) continuous variable splits; (iv) erroneous interpretations of correlated gain scores; (v) single transfer assessments; (vi) multiple comparisons; and (vii) publication bias. Each flaw is illustrated with a Monte Carlo simulation to present its underlying mechanisms, gauge its magnitude, and discuss potential remedies. Although not restricted to training studies, these flaws are typically exacerbated in such designs, due to ubiquitous practices in data collection or data analysis. The article reviews these practices, so as to avoid common pitfalls when designing or analyzing an intervention. More generally, it is also intended as a reference for anyone interested in evaluating claims of cognitive enhancement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Moreau
- Centre for Brain Research and School of Psychology, University of AucklandAuckland, New Zealand
| | - Ian J. Kirk
- Centre for Brain Research and School of Psychology, University of AucklandAuckland, New Zealand
| | - Karen E. Waldie
- Centre for Brain Research and School of Psychology, University of AucklandAuckland, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
321
|
Diamond A, Ling DS. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2016; 18:34-48. [PMID: 26749076 PMCID: PMC5108631 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 489] [Impact Index Per Article: 54.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2015] [Revised: 10/26/2015] [Accepted: 11/23/2015] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
The 'Executive Functions' (EFs) of inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility enable us to think before we act, resist temptations or impulsive reactions, stay focused, reason, problem-solve, flexibly adjust to changed demands or priorities, and see things from new and different perspectives. These skills are critical for success in all life's aspects and are sometimes more predictive than even IQ or socioeconomic status. Understandably, there is great interest in improving EFs. It's now clear they can be improved at any age through training and practice, much as physical exercise hones physical fitness. However, despite claims to the contrary, wide transfer does not seem to occur and 'mindless' aerobic exercise does little to improve EFs. Important questions remain: How much can EFs be improved (are benefits only superficial) and how long can benefits be sustained? What are the best methods for improving EFs? What about an approach accounts for its success? Do the answers to these differ by individual characteristics such as age or gender? Since stress, sadness, loneliness, or poor health impair EFs, and the reverse enhances EFs, we predict that besides directly train EFs, the most successful approaches for improving EFs will also address emotional, social, and physical needs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adele Diamond
- Program in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, UBC, 2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 2A1.
| | - Daphne S Ling
- Program in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, UBC, 2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 2A1
| |
Collapse
|
322
|
Wayne RV, Hamilton C, Jones Huyck J, Johnsrude IS. Working Memory Training and Speech in Noise Comprehension in Older Adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 8:49. [PMID: 27047370 PMCID: PMC4801856 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00049] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2015] [Accepted: 02/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding speech in the presence of background sound can be challenging for older adults. Speech comprehension in noise appears to depend on working memory and executive-control processes (e.g., Heald and Nusbaum, 2014), and their augmentation through training may have rehabilitative potential for age-related hearing loss. We examined the efficacy of adaptive working-memory training (Cogmed; Klingberg et al., 2002) in 24 older adults, assessing generalization to other working-memory tasks (near-transfer) and to other cognitive domains (far-transfer) using a cognitive test battery, including the Reading Span test, sensitive to working memory (e.g., Daneman and Carpenter, 1980). We also assessed far transfer to speech-in-noise performance, including a closed-set sentence task (Kidd et al., 2008). To examine the effect of cognitive training on benefit obtained from semantic context, we also assessed transfer to open-set sentences; half were semantically coherent (high-context) and half were semantically anomalous (low-context). Subjects completed 25 sessions (0.5–1 h each; 5 sessions/week) of both adaptive working memory training and placebo training over 10 weeks in a crossover design. Subjects' scores on the adaptive working-memory training tasks improved as a result of training. However, training did not transfer to other working memory tasks, nor to tasks recruiting other cognitive domains. We did not observe any training-related improvement in speech-in-noise performance. Measures of working memory correlated with the intelligibility of low-context, but not high-context, sentences, suggesting that sentence context may reduce the load on working memory. The Reading Span test significantly correlated only with a test of visual episodic memory, suggesting that the Reading Span test is not a pure-test of working memory, as is commonly assumed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel V Wayne
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Cheryl Hamilton
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University Kingston, ON, Canada
| | | | - Ingrid S Johnsrude
- Department of Psychology, Queen's UniversityKingston, ON, Canada; Department of Psychology, School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western OntarioLondon, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
323
|
Basak C, O'Connell MA. To Switch or Not to Switch: Role of Cognitive Control in Working Memory Training in Older Adults. Front Psychol 2016; 7:230. [PMID: 26973554 PMCID: PMC4774648 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00230] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2015] [Accepted: 02/04/2016] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
It is currently not known what are the best working memory training strategies to offset the age-related declines in fluid cognitive abilities. In this randomized clinical double-blind trial, older adults were randomly assigned to one of two types of working memory training – one group was trained on a predictable memory updating task (PT) and another group was trained on a novel, unpredictable memory updating task (UT). Unpredictable memory updating, compared to predictable, requires greater demands on cognitive control (Basak and Verhaeghen, 2011a). Therefore, the current study allowed us to evaluate the role of cognitive control in working memory training. All participants were assessed on a set of near and far transfer tasks at three different testing sessions – before training, immediately after the training, and 1.5 months after completing the training. Additionally, individual learning rates for a comparison working memory task (performed by both groups) and the trained task were computed. Training on unpredictable memory updating, compared to predictable, significantly enhanced performance on a measure of episodic memory, immediately after the training. Moreover, individuals with faster learning rates showed greater gains in this episodic memory task and another new working memory task; this effect was specific to UT. We propose that the unpredictable memory updating training, compared to predictable memory updating training, may a better strategy to improve selective cognitive abilities in older adults, and future studies could further investigate the role of cognitive control in working memory training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chandramallika Basak
- Center for Vital Longevity, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson TX, USA
| | - Margaret A O'Connell
- Center for Vital Longevity, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
324
|
Akimoto Y, Nozawa T, Kanno A, Kambara T, Ihara M, Ogawa T, Goto T, Taki Y, Yokoyama R, Kotozaki Y, Nouchi R, Sekiguchi A, Takeuchi H, Miyauchi CM, Sugiura M, Okumura E, Sunda T, Shimizu T, Tozuka E, Hirose S, Nanbu T, Kawashima R. High-gamma power changes after cognitive intervention: preliminary results from twenty-one senior adult subjects. Brain Behav 2016; 6:e00427. [PMID: 26855826 PMCID: PMC4733105 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.427] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2015] [Revised: 12/08/2015] [Accepted: 12/11/2015] [Indexed: 01/03/2023] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Brain-imaging techniques have begun to be popular in evaluating the effectiveness of cognitive intervention training. Although gamma activities are rarely used as an index of training effects, they have several characteristics that suggest their potential suitability for this purpose. This pilot study examined whether cognitive training in elderly people affected the high-gamma activity associated with attentional processing and whether high-gamma power changes were related to changes in behavioral performance. METHODS We analyzed (MEG) magnetoencephalography data obtained from 35 healthy elderly subjects (60-75 years old) who had participated in our previous intervention study in which the subjects were randomly assigned to one of the three types of intervention groups: Group V trained in a vehicle with a newly developed onboard cognitive training program, Group P trained with a similar program but on a personal computer, and Group C was trained to solve a crossword puzzle as an active control group. High-gamma (52-100 Hz) activity during a three-stimulus visual oddball task was measured before and after training. As a result of exclusion in the MEG data analysis stage, the final sample consisted of five subjects in Group V, nine subjects in Group P, and seven subjects in Group C. RESULTS Results showed that high-gamma activities were differently altered between groups after cognitive intervention. In particular, members of Group V, who showed significant improvements in cognitive function after training, exhibited increased high-gamma power in the left middle frontal gyrus during top-down anticipatory target processing. High-gamma power changes in this region were also associated with changes in behavioral performance. CONCLUSIONS Our preliminary results suggest the usefulness of high-gamma activities as an index of the effectiveness of cognitive training in elderly subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoritaka Akimoto
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Takayuki Nozawa
- Smart Ageing International Research Center Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Akitake Kanno
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Toshimune Kambara
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Tokyo 102-8472 Japan
| | - Mizuki Ihara
- Smart Ageing International Research Center Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Takeshi Ogawa
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Takakuni Goto
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Yasuyuki Taki
- Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Ryoichi Yokoyama
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Yuka Kotozaki
- Smart Ageing International Research Center Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Rui Nouchi
- Smart Ageing International Research Center Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Human and Social Response Research Division International Research Institute of Disaster Science Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Atsushi Sekiguchi
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Division of Medical Neuroimage Analysis Department of Community Medical Supports Tohoku Medical Megabank Organization Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Department of Adult Mental Health National Institute of Mental Health National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry Kodaira 187-8553 Japan
| | - Hikaru Takeuchi
- Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Carlos Makoto Miyauchi
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Motoaki Sugiura
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Human and Social Response Research Division International Research Institute of Disaster Science Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Eiichi Okumura
- Department of Epileptology Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| | - Takashi Sunda
- Mobility Services Laboratory Research Division 2 Nissan Motor Co., Ltd. Kanagawa 243-0123 Japan
| | - Toshiyuki Shimizu
- Mobility Services Laboratory Research Division 2 Nissan Motor Co., Ltd. Kanagawa 243-0123 Japan
| | - Eiji Tozuka
- Vehicle Test and Measurement Technology Development Department CAE and Testing Division 1 Nissan Motor Co., Ltd. Kanagawa 243-0192 Japan
| | - Satoru Hirose
- Mobility Services Laboratory Research Division 2 Nissan Motor Co., Ltd. Kanagawa 243-0123 Japan
| | - Tatsuyoshi Nanbu
- Prototype and Test Department Research Division 2 Nissan Motor Co., Ltd. Kanagawa 243-0123 Japan
| | - Ryuta Kawashima
- Department of Functional Brain Imaging Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Smart Ageing International Research Center Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan; Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer Tohoku University Sendai 980-8575 Japan
| |
Collapse
|
325
|
Hadwin JA, Richards HJ. Working Memory Training and CBT Reduces Anxiety Symptoms and Attentional Biases to Threat: A Preliminary Study. Front Psychol 2016; 7:47. [PMID: 26869956 PMCID: PMC4735443 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2015] [Accepted: 01/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Research indicates that cognitive processes linked to the detection of threat stimuli are associated with poor attentional control, placing children and adolescents at increased risk for the development of anxious affect. The current study aimed to provide preliminary data to assess whether an intervention designed to improve attentional control (via working memory; WM) would lead to better performance in tests of WM and would be associated with positive changes in symptoms of trait and test anxiety, increased inhibitory control and reduced attention to threat. Forty adolescents aged 11-14 years who reported elevated anxiety and low attentional control were randomly allocated to a WM training or an active cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) control group. Post intervention, WM training was associated with greater improvements (versus. CBT) in trained WM tasks. Both groups, however, reported fewer anxiety symptoms, demonstrated increased inhibitory control and a reduction in attentional biases to threat post intervention and these results were maintained at follow up. The study provides indicative evidence which suggests that WM training has similar benefits to a more traditional CBT intervention on reduced anxiety and attentional biases for threat. Future research should aim to replicate the findings in a large sample size and explore the broader impact of training on day-to-day functioning. In addition, further research is needed to identify which participants benefit most from different interventions (using baseline characteristics) on treatment compliance and outcome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie A Hadwin
- Developmental Brain-Behaviour Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Southampton Southampton, UK
| | - Helen J Richards
- Developmental Brain-Behaviour Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Southampton Southampton, UK
| |
Collapse
|
326
|
Liu ZX, Glizer D, Tannock R, Woltering S. EEG alpha power during maintenance of information in working memory in adults with ADHD and its plasticity due to working memory training: A randomized controlled trial. Clin Neurophysiol 2016; 127:1307-1320. [DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.10.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2015] [Revised: 09/15/2015] [Accepted: 10/07/2015] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
|
327
|
Inhibition Plasticity in Older Adults: Practice and Transfer Effects Using a Multiple Task Approach. Neural Plast 2016; 2016:9696402. [PMID: 26885407 PMCID: PMC4738960 DOI: 10.1155/2016/9696402] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2015] [Revised: 10/24/2015] [Accepted: 10/27/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE. To examine plasticity of inhibition, as indexed by practice effects of inhibition tasks and the associated transfer effects, using a multiple task approach in healthy older adults. METHOD. Forty-eight healthy older adults were evenly assigned to either a practice group or a no-contact control group. All participants completed pretest (2.5 hours) and posttest (2 hours) sessions, with a 2-week interval in between. During the 2-week interval, only the practice group completed six 30-minute practice sessions (three sessions per week for two consecutive weeks) of three lab-based inhibition tasks. RESULTS. All three inhibition tasks demonstrated significant improvement across practice sessions, suggesting practice-induced plasticity. The benefit, however, only transferred to near-near tasks. The results are inconclusive with regard to the near-far and far-far transfer effects. DISCUSSION. This study further extends literature on practice effects of inhibition in older adults by using a multiple task approach. Together with previous work, the current study suggests that older adults are able to improve inhibition performance through practice and transfer the practice gains to tasks that overlap in both target cognitive ability and task structure (i.e., near-near tasks).
Collapse
|
328
|
Sandry J. Working memory and memory loss in neurodegenerative disease. Neurodegener Dis Manag 2016; 5:1-4. [PMID: 25711447 DOI: 10.2217/nmt.14.51] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Joshua Sandry
- Neuropsychology & Neuroscience Research, Kessler Foundation, 300 Executive Drive, Suite 70, West Orange, NJ 07052, USA
| |
Collapse
|
329
|
Abstract
PURPOSE OF REVIEW The article discusses the two most significant modifiable risk factors for dementia, namely, physical inactivity and lack of stimulating cognitive activity, and their effects on developing cognitive reserve. RECENT FINDINGS Both of these leisure-time activities were associated with significant reductions in the risk of dementia in longitudinal studies. In addition, physical activity, particularly aerobic exercise, is associated with less age-related gray and white matter loss and with less neurotoxic factors. On the other hand, cognitive training studies suggest that training for executive functions (e.g., working memory) improves prefrontal network efficiency, which provides support to brain functioning in the face of cognitive decline. While physical activity preserves neuronal structural integrity and brain volume (hardware), cognitive activity strengthens the functioning and plasticity of neural circuits (software), thus supporting cognitive reserve in different ways. Future research should examine whether lifestyle interventions incorporating these two domains can reduce incident dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sheung-Tak Cheng
- Department of Health and Physical Education, The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, N.T., Hong Kong. .,Department of Clinical Psychology, Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norfolk, NR4 7TJ, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
330
|
Effects of working memory and attentional control training and their transfer onto fluid intelligence in early and late adulthood. HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REPORT 2016. [DOI: 10.5114/hpr.2016.56846] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
|
331
|
Del Missier F, Hansson P, Parker AM, Bruine de Bruin W, Nilsson LG, Mäntylä T. Unraveling the Aging Skein: Disentangling Sensory and Cognitive Predictors of Age-related Differences in Decision Making. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1926] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Fabio Del Missier
- Department of Psychology; Stockholm University; Stockholm Sweden
- Department of Life Sciences; University of Trieste; Trieste Italy
| | | | | | - Wändi Bruine de Bruin
- Leeds University Business School; Leeds UK
- Department of Engineering & Public Policy; Carnegie Mellon University; Pittsburgh PA USA
| | - Lars-Göran Nilsson
- Department of Psychology; Stockholm University; Stockholm Sweden
- ARC Karolinska Institutet Stockholm; Stockholm Sweden
| | - Timo Mäntylä
- Department of Psychology; Stockholm University; Stockholm Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
332
|
Küper K, Karbach J. Increased training complexity reduces the effectiveness of brief working memory training: evidence from short-term single and dualn-back training interventions. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1118106] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
333
|
Nagle A, Riener R, Wolf P. High User Control in Game Design Elements Increases Compliance and In-game Performance in a Memory Training Game. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1774. [PMID: 26635681 PMCID: PMC4653717 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01774] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2015] [Accepted: 11/04/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Computer games are increasingly being used for training cognitive functions like working memory and attention among the growing population of older adults. While cognitive training games often include elements like difficulty adaptation, rewards, and visual themes to make the games more enjoyable and effective, the effect of different degrees of afforded user control in manipulating these elements has not been systematically studied. To address this issue, two distinct implementations of the three aforementioned game elements were tested among healthy older adults (N = 21, 69.9 ± 6.4 years old) playing a game-like version of the n-back task on a tablet at home for 3 weeks. Two modes were considered, differentiated by the afforded degree of user control of the three elements: user control of difficulty vs. automatic difficulty adaptation, difficulty-dependent rewards vs. automatic feedback messages, and user choice of visual theme vs. no choice. The two modes ("USER-CONTROL" and "AUTO") were compared for frequency of play, duration of play, and in-game performance. Participants were free to play the game whenever and for however long they wished. Participants in USER-CONTROL exhibited significantly higher frequency of playing, total play duration, and in-game performance than participants in AUTO. The results of the present study demonstrate the efficacy of providing user control in the three game elements, while validating a home-based study design in which participants were not bound by any training regimen, and could play the game whenever they wished. The results have implications for designing cognitive training games that elicit higher compliance and better in-game performance, with an emphasis on home-based training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aniket Nagle
- Sensory-Motor Systems Lab, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, ETH ZurichZurich, Switzerland
| | - Robert Riener
- Sensory-Motor Systems Lab, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, ETH ZurichZurich, Switzerland
- Spinal Cord Injury Center, Balgrist University HospitalZurich, Switzerland
| | - Peter Wolf
- Sensory-Motor Systems Lab, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, ETH ZurichZurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
334
|
Baniqued PL, Allen CM, Kranz MB, Johnson K, Sipolins A, Dickens C, Ward N, Geyer A, Kramer AF. Working Memory, Reasoning, and Task Switching Training: Transfer Effects, Limitations, and Great Expectations? PLoS One 2015; 10:e0142169. [PMID: 26555341 PMCID: PMC4640538 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2015] [Accepted: 10/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Although some studies have shown that cognitive training can produce improvements to untrained cognitive domains (far transfer), many others fail to show these effects, especially when it comes to improving fluid intelligence. The current study was designed to overcome several limitations of previous training studies by incorporating training expectancy assessments, an active control group, and "Mind Frontiers," a video game-based mobile program comprised of six adaptive, cognitively demanding training tasks that have been found to lead to increased scores in fluid intelligence (Gf) tests. We hypothesize that such integrated training may lead to broad improvements in cognitive abilities by targeting aspects of working memory, executive function, reasoning, and problem solving. Ninety participants completed 20 hour-and-a-half long training sessions over four to five weeks, 45 of whom played Mind Frontiers and 45 of whom completed visual search and change detection tasks (active control). After training, the Mind Frontiers group improved in working memory n-back tests, a composite measure of perceptual speed, and a composite measure of reaction time in reasoning tests. No training-related improvements were found in reasoning accuracy or other working memory tests, nor in composite measures of episodic memory, selective attention, divided attention, and multi-tasking. Perceived self-improvement in the tested abilities did not differ between groups. A general expectancy difference in problem-solving was observed between groups, but this perceived benefit did not correlate with training-related improvement. In summary, although these findings provide modest evidence regarding the efficacy of an integrated cognitive training program, more research is needed to determine the utility of Mind Frontiers as a cognitive training tool.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline L. Baniqued
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Courtney M. Allen
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Michael B. Kranz
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Kathryn Johnson
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Aldis Sipolins
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Charles Dickens
- Aptima, Inc., Woburn, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Nathan Ward
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Alexandra Geyer
- Aptima, Inc., Woburn, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Arthur F. Kramer
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
335
|
Chan JS, Wu Q, Liang D, Yan JH. Visuospatial working memory training facilitates visually-aided explicit sequence learning. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 161:145-53. [PMID: 26398484 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.09.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2014] [Revised: 09/08/2015] [Accepted: 09/10/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Finger sequence learning requires visuospatial working memory (WM). However, the dynamics between age, WM training, and motor skill acquisition are unclear. Therefore, we examined how visuospatial WM training improves finger movement sequential accuracy in younger (n=26, 21.1±1.37years) and older adults (n=22, 70.6±4.01years). After performing a finger sequence learning exercise and numerical and spatial WM tasks, participants in each age group were randomly assigned to either the experimental (EX) or control (CO) groups. For one hour daily over a 10-day period, the EX group practiced an adaptive n-back spatial task while those in the CO group practiced a non-adaptive version. As a result of WM practice, the EX participants increased their accuracy in the spatial n-back tasks, while accuracy remained unimproved in the numerical n-back tasks. In all groups, reaction times (RT) became shorter in most numerical and spatial n-back tasks. The learners in the EX group - but not in the CO group - showed improvements in their retention of finger sequences. The findings support our hypothesis that computerized visuospatial WM training improves finger sequence learning both in younger and in older adults. We discuss the theoretical implications and clinical relevance of this research for motor learning and functional rehabilitation.
Collapse
|
336
|
Chiou KS, Sandry J, Chiaravalloti ND. Cognitive contributions to differences in learning after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2015; 37:1074-85. [DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1078293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
337
|
Bamidis PD, Fissler P, Papageorgiou SG, Zilidou V, Konstantinidis EI, Billis AS, Romanopoulou E, Karagianni M, Beratis I, Tsapanou A, Tsilikopoulou G, Grigoriadou E, Ladas A, Kyrillidou A, Tsolaki A, Frantzidis C, Sidiropoulos E, Siountas A, Matsi S, Papatriantafyllou J, Margioti E, Nika A, Schlee W, Elbert T, Tsolaki M, Vivas AB, Kolassa IT. Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical training: the role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive disorder. Front Aging Neurosci 2015; 7:152. [PMID: 26300772 PMCID: PMC4528297 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00152] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2015] [Accepted: 07/24/2015] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Physical as well as cognitive training interventions improve specific cognitive functions but effects barely generalize on global cognition. Combined physical and cognitive training may overcome this shortcoming as physical training may facilitate the neuroplastic potential which, in turn, may be guided by cognitive training. This study aimed at investigating the benefits of combined training on global cognition while assessing the effect of training dosage and exploring the role of several potential effect modifiers. In this multi-center study, 322 older adults with or without neurocognitive disorders (NCDs) were allocated to a computerized, game-based, combined physical and cognitive training group (n = 237) or a passive control group (n = 85). Training group participants were allocated to different training dosages ranging from 24 to 110 potential sessions. In a pre-post-test design, global cognition was assessed by averaging standardized performance in working memory, episodic memory and executive function tests. The intervention group increased in global cognition compared to the control group, p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.31. Exploratory analysis revealed a trend for less benefits in participants with more severe NCD, p = 0.08 (cognitively healthy: d = 0.54; mild cognitive impairment: d = 0.19; dementia: d = 0.04). In participants without dementia, we found a dose-response effect of the potential number and of the completed number of training sessions on global cognition, p = 0.008 and p = 0.04, respectively. The results indicate that combined physical and cognitive training improves global cognition in a dose-responsive manner but these benefits may be less pronounced in older adults with more severe NCD. The long-lasting impact of combined training on the incidence and trajectory of NCDs in relation to its severity should be assessed in future long-term trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Panagiotis D Bamidis
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Patrick Fissler
- Institute of Psychology and Pedagogy, Clinical and Biological Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm Germany
| | - Sokratis G Papageorgiou
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Vasiliki Zilidou
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Evdokimos I Konstantinidis
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Antonis S Billis
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Evangelia Romanopoulou
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Maria Karagianni
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece ; Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki Greece
| | - Ion Beratis
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Angeliki Tsapanou
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Georgia Tsilikopoulou
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Eirini Grigoriadou
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece ; Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki Greece
| | - Aristea Ladas
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece ; Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology Lab, Department of Psychology, City College, The University of Sheffield International Faculty, Thessaloniki Greece
| | - Athina Kyrillidou
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece ; Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki Greece
| | - Anthoula Tsolaki
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece ; Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki Greece
| | - Christos Frantzidis
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Efstathios Sidiropoulos
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Anastasios Siountas
- Laboratory of Medical Physics, Faculty of Health Sciences, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Stavroula Matsi
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - John Papatriantafyllou
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Eleni Margioti
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Aspasia Nika
- Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Unit, 1st and 2nd Department of Neurology, Medical School, National Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens, Greece
| | - Winfried Schlee
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| | - Thomas Elbert
- Clinical Psychology and Clinical Neuropsychology, University of Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| | - Magda Tsolaki
- Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki Greece ; 3rd Department of Neurology, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Ana B Vivas
- Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology Lab, Department of Psychology, City College, The University of Sheffield International Faculty, Thessaloniki Greece
| | - Iris-Tatjana Kolassa
- Institute of Psychology and Pedagogy, Clinical and Biological Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm Germany
| |
Collapse
|
338
|
Investigating the effectiveness of working memory training in the context of Personality Systems Interaction theory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2015. [PMID: 26208631 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0687-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Previous research has shown mixed results for the ability of working memory training to improve fluid intelligence. The aims of this study were first to replicate these improvements, and then to explore the moderating role of Personality Systems Interaction (PSI) personality factors. By using three different training methods and an active-contact control group, we examined the effects of 25 days of cognitive training on 142 participants. After examining our results in context of PSI theory, we found that different training methods yielded different IQ gains in participants, depending on their personality styles. In addition, these correlations suggested a meaningful pattern, indicating that PSI theory may be able to account for the different outcomes of cognitive training studies. Our findings may facilitate tailor-made cognitive training interventions in the future, and can contribute to explaining the mechanisms underlying the far transfer of working memory training to fluid intelligence.
Collapse
|
339
|
Binder JC, Zöllig J, Eschen A, Mérillat S, Röcke C, Schoch SF, Jäncke L, Martin M. Multi-domain training in healthy old age: Hotel Plastisse as an iPad-based serious game to systematically compare multi-domain and single-domain training. Front Aging Neurosci 2015; 7:137. [PMID: 26257643 PMCID: PMC4511844 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00137] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2015] [Accepted: 07/06/2015] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Finding effective training interventions for declining cognitive abilities in healthy aging is of great relevance, especially in view of the demographic development. Since it is assumed that transfer from the trained to untrained domains is more likely to occur when training conditions and transfer measures share a common underlying process, multi-domain training of several cognitive functions should increase the likelihood of such an overlap. In the first part, we give an overview of the literature showing that cognitive training using complex tasks, such as video games, leisure activities, or practicing a series of cognitive tasks, has shown promising results regarding transfer to a number of cognitive functions. These studies, however, do not allow direct inference about the underlying functions targeted by these training regimes. Custom-designed serious games allow to design training regimes according to specific cognitive functions and a target population's need. In the second part, we introduce the serious game Hotel Plastisse as an iPad-based training tool for older adults that allows the comparison of the simultaneous training of spatial navigation, visuomotor function, and inhibition to the training of each of these functions separately. Hotel Plastisse not only defines the cognitive functions of the multi-domain training clearly, but also implements training in an interesting learning environment including adaptive difficulty and feedback. We propose this novel training tool with the goal of furthering our understanding of how training regimes should be designed in order to affect cognitive functioning of older adults most broadly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia C Binder
- Division of Gerontopsychology and Gerontology, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Jacqueline Zöllig
- Division of Gerontopsychology and Gerontology, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Anne Eschen
- International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Susan Mérillat
- International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Christina Röcke
- International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Sarah F Schoch
- International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Lutz Jäncke
- International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Mike Martin
- Division of Gerontopsychology and Gerontology, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland ; University Research Priority Program "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
340
|
Könen T, Karbach J. The benefits of looking at intraindividual dynamics in cognitive training data. Front Psychol 2015; 6:615. [PMID: 26029148 PMCID: PMC4428056 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2015] [Accepted: 04/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Tanja Könen
- Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Germany ; Center for Research on Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA) Frankfurt, Germany
| | - Julia Karbach
- Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Germany ; Center for Research on Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA) Frankfurt, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
341
|
Moreau D, Morrison AB, Conway ARA. An ecological approach to cognitive enhancement: complex motor training. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 157:44-55. [PMID: 25725192 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2014] [Revised: 02/03/2015] [Accepted: 02/11/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive training has received a lot of attention recently, yielding findings that can be conflicting and controversial. In this paper, we present a novel approach to cognitive training based on complex motor activities. In a randomized controlled design, participants were assigned to one of three conditions: aerobic exercise, working memory training or designed sport--an intervention specifically tailored to include both physical and cognitive demands. After training for eight weeks, the designed sport group showed the largest gains in all cognitive measures, illustrating the efficacy of complex motor activities to enhance cognition. Designed sport training also revealed impressive health benefits, namely decreased heart rate and blood pressure. In this period of skepticism over the efficacy of computerized cognitive training, we discuss the potential of ecological interventions targeting both cognition and physical fitness, and propose some possible applications.
Collapse
|
342
|
|
343
|
Ballesteros S, Mayas J, Prieto A, Toril P, Pita C, Laura PDL, Reales JM, Waterworth JA. A randomized controlled trial of brain training with non-action video games in older adults: results of the 3-month follow-up. Front Aging Neurosci 2015; 7:45. [PMID: 25926790 PMCID: PMC4396447 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2014] [Accepted: 03/23/2015] [Indexed: 01/17/2023] Open
Abstract
This randomized controlled study (ClinicalTrials.gov NCT02007616) investigated the maintenance of training effects of 20 1-hr non-action video game training sessions with selected games from a commercial package on several age-declining cognitive functions and subjective wellbeing after a 3-month no-contact period. Two groups of cognitively normal older adults participated in both the post-training (posttest) and the present follow-up study, the experimental group who received training and the control group who attended several meetings with the research team during the study but did not receive training. Groups were similar at baseline on demographics, vocabulary, global cognition, and depression status. Significant improvements in the trained group, and no variation in the control group had been previously found at posttest, in processing speed, attention and visual recognition memory, as well as in two dimensions of subjective wellbeing. In the current study, improvement from baseline to 3 months follow-up was found only in wellbeing (Affection and Assertivity dimensions) in the trained group whereas there was no change in the control group. Previous significant improvements in processing speed, attention and spatial memory become non-significant after the 3-month interval. Training older adults with non-action video games enhanced aspects of cognition just after training but this effect disappeared after a 3-month no-contact follow-up period. Cognitive plasticity can be induced in older adults by training, but to maintain the benefits periodic boosting sessions would be necessary.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Soledad Ballesteros
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | - Julia Mayas
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | - Antonio Prieto
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | - Pilar Toril
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | - Carmen Pita
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | - Ponce de León Laura
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | - José M Reales
- Studies on Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Group, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia Madrid, Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|
344
|
Salminen T, Frensch P, Strobach T, Schubert T. Age-specific differences of dual n-back training. AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2015; 23:18-39. [PMID: 25867501 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2015.1031723] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Age-related decline in executive functions can be decisive in performing everyday tasks autonomously. Working memory (WM) is closely related to executive functions, and training of WM has yielded evidence toward cognitive plasticity in older adults. The training effects often transfer to untrained tasks and functions. These effects have mostly been shown in processes such as WM and attention, whereas studies investigating transfer to executive functions have been scarce. We trained older adults aged 57-73 years in a WM training task that was reported to be effective in producing transfer in young adults. The training intervention consisted of a dual n-back task including independently processed auditory and visual n-back tasks. We investigated transfer to tasks engaging executive functions, and compared the effects in older adults to those reported in young adults. We found that both training groups improved in the training task. Although the training effect in older adults was smaller than the training effect in young adults, the older adults still showed a notable improvement so that after training they performed on the same level as young adults without training. The older adults also showed transfer to an untrained WM updating task, a result that was in accordance with the findings in young adults; other transfer effects in older adults were lacking. We conclude that although transfer effects were scarce, the present study provides encouraging evidence toward the possibilities to compensate for age-related decline in executive functions by a WM training intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tiina Salminen
- a Department of Psychology , Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin , Germany
| | - Peter Frensch
- a Department of Psychology , Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin , Germany
| | - Tilo Strobach
- a Department of Psychology , Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin , Germany
| | - Torsten Schubert
- a Department of Psychology , Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin , Germany
| |
Collapse
|
345
|
Jones KT, Stephens JA, Alam M, Bikson M, Berryhill ME. Longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0121904. [PMID: 25849358 PMCID: PMC4388845 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2014] [Accepted: 02/05/2015] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
An increasing concern affecting a growing aging population is working memory (WM) decline. Consequently, there is great interest in improving or stabilizing WM, which drives expanded use of brain training exercises. Such regimens generally result in temporary WM benefits to the trained tasks but minimal transfer of benefit to untrained tasks. Pairing training with neurostimulation may stabilize or improve WM performance by enhancing plasticity and strengthening WM-related cortical networks. We tested this possibility in healthy older adults. Participants received 10 sessions of sham (control) or active (anodal, 1.5 mA) tDCS to the right prefrontal, parietal, or prefrontal/parietal (alternating) cortices. After ten minutes of sham or active tDCS, participants performed verbal and visual WM training tasks. On the first, tenth, and follow-up sessions, participants performed transfer WM tasks including the spatial 2-back, Stroop, and digit span tasks. The results demonstrated that all groups benefited from WM training, as expected. However, at follow-up 1-month after training ended, only the participants in the active tDCS groups maintained significant improvement. Importantly, this pattern was observed for both trained and transfer tasks. These results demonstrate that tDCS-linked WM training can provide long-term benefits in maintaining cognitive training benefits and extending them to untrained tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kevin T. Jones
- Memory and Brain Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Nevada, United States of America
- Cognitive Neuropsychology Lab, Department of Neurology, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
| | - Jaclyn A. Stephens
- Memory and Brain Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Nevada, United States of America
| | - Mahtab Alam
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, The City College of New York, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Marom Bikson
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, The City College of New York, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Marian E. Berryhill
- Memory and Brain Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, Nevada, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
346
|
Ritchie SJ, Bates TC, Deary IJ. Is education associated with improvements in general cognitive ability, or in specific skills? Dev Psychol 2015; 51:573-82. [PMID: 25775112 PMCID: PMC4445388 DOI: 10.1037/a0038981] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has indicated that education influences cognitive development, but it is unclear what, precisely, is being improved. Here, we tested whether education is associated with cognitive test score improvements via domain-general effects on general cognitive ability (g), or via domain-specific effects on particular cognitive skills. We conducted structural equation modeling on data from a large (n = 1,091), longitudinal sample, with a measure of intelligence at age 11 years and 10 tests covering a diverse range of cognitive abilities taken at age 70. Results indicated that the association of education with improved cognitive test scores is not mediated by g, but consists of direct effects on specific cognitive skills. These results suggest a decoupling of educational gains from increases in general intellectual capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stuart J Ritchie
- Department of Psychology and Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, The University of Edinburgh
| | - Timothy C Bates
- Department of Psychology and Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, The University of Edinburgh
| | - Ian J Deary
- Department of Psychology and Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, The University of Edinburgh
| |
Collapse
|
347
|
Does working memory training have to be adaptive? PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2015; 80:181-94. [PMID: 25716189 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0655-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2014] [Accepted: 02/17/2015] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
This study tested the common assumption that, to be most effective, working memory (WM) training should be adaptive (i.e., task difficulty is adjusted to individual performance). Indirect evidence for this assumption stems from studies comparing adaptive training to a condition in which tasks are practiced on the easiest level of difficulty only [cf. Klingberg (Trends Cogn Sci 14:317-324, 2010)], thereby, however, confounding adaptivity and exposure to varying task difficulty. For a more direct test of this hypothesis, we randomly assigned 130 young adults to one of the three WM training procedures (adaptive, randomized, or self-selected change in training task difficulty) or to an active control group. Despite large performance increases in the trained WM tasks, we observed neither transfer to untrained structurally dissimilar WM tasks nor far transfer to reasoning. Surprisingly, neither training nor transfer effects were modulated by training procedure, indicating that exposure to varying levels of task difficulty is sufficient for inducing training gains.
Collapse
|
348
|
Deveau J, Jaeggi SM, Zordan V, Phung C, Seitz AR. How to build better memory training games. Front Syst Neurosci 2015; 8:243. [PMID: 25620916 PMCID: PMC4288240 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2014] [Accepted: 12/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Can we create engaging training programs that improve working memory (WM) skills? While there are numerous procedures that attempt to do so, there is a great deal of controversy regarding their efficacy. Nonetheless, recent meta-analytic evidence shows consistent improvements across studies on lab-based tasks generalizing beyond the specific training effects (Au et al., 2014; Karbach and Verhaeghen, 2014), however, there is little research into how WM training aids participants in their daily life. Here we propose that incorporating design principles from the fields of Perceptual Learning (PL) and Computer Science might augment the efficacy of WM training, and ultimately lead to greater learning and transfer. In particular, the field of PL has identified numerous mechanisms (including attention, reinforcement, multisensory facilitation and multi-stimulus training) that promote brain plasticity. Also, computer science has made great progress in the scientific approach to game design that can be used to create engaging environments for learning. We suggest that approaches integrating knowledge across these fields may lead to a more effective WM interventions and better reflect real world conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenni Deveau
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside Riverside, CA, USA
| | - Susanne M Jaeggi
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine Irvine, CA, USA ; Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine Irvine, CA, USA
| | - Victor Zordan
- Department of Computer Science, University of California, Riverside Riverside, CA, USA
| | - Calvin Phung
- Department of Computer Science, University of California, Riverside Riverside, CA, USA
| | - Aaron R Seitz
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside Riverside, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
349
|
New directions in cognitive training: on methods, transfer, and application. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2014; 78:749-55. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-014-0619-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2014] [Accepted: 09/24/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|