1
|
Bao Y, Zhou B, Yu X, Mao L, Gutyrchik E, Paolini M, Logothetis N, Pöppel E. Conscious vision in blindness: A new perceptual phenomenon implemented on the "wrong" side of the brain. Psych J 2024. [PMID: 39019467 DOI: 10.1002/pchj.787] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2023] [Accepted: 06/04/2024] [Indexed: 07/19/2024]
Abstract
Patients with lesions in the visual cortex are blind in corresponding regions of the visual field, but they still may process visual information, a phenomenon referred to as residual vision or "blindsight". Here we report behavioral and fMRI observations with a patient who reports conscious vision across an extended area of blindness for moving, but not for stationary stimuli. This completion effect is shown to be of perceptual and not of conceptual origin, most likely mediated by spared representations of the visual field in the striate cortex. The neural output to extra-striate areas from regions of the deafferented striate cortex is apparently still intact; this is, for instance, indicated by preserved size constancy of visually completed stimuli. Neural responses as measured with fMRI reveal an activation only for moving stimuli, but importantly on the ipsilateral side of the brain. In a conceptual model this shift of activation to the "wrong" hemisphere is explained on the basis of an imbalance of excitatory and inhibitory interactions within and between the striate cortices due to the brain injury. The observed neuroplasticity indicated by this shift together with the behavioral observations provide important new insights into the functional architecture of the human visual system and provide new insight into the concept of consciousness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yan Bao
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Bin Zhou
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Xinchi Yu
- Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, USA
- Department of Linguistics, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, USA
| | - Lihua Mao
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Evgeny Gutyrchik
- Institute of Medical Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - Marco Paolini
- Department of Radiology, University Hospital, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
| | - Nikos Logothetis
- International Center for Primate Brain Research, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, China
| | - Ernst Pöppel
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China
- Institute of Medical Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Geuzebroek AC, van den Berg AV. Impaired visual competition in patients with homonymous visual field defects. Neuropsychologia 2017; 97:152-162. [PMID: 28209521 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2016] [Revised: 02/06/2017] [Accepted: 02/12/2017] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Intense visual training can lead to partial recovery of visual field defects caused by lesions of the primary visual cortex. However, the standard visual detection and discrimination tasks, used to assess this recovery process tend to ignore the complexity of the natural visual environment, where multiple stimuli continuously interact. Visual competition is an essential component for natural search tasks and detecting unexpected events. Our study focused on visual decision-making and to what extent the recovered visual field can compete for attention with the 'intact' visual field. Nine patients with visual field defects who had previously received visual discrimination training, were compared to healthy age-matched controls using a saccade target-selection paradigm, in which participants actively make a saccade towards the brighter of two flashed targets. To further investigate the nature of competition (feed-forward or feedback inhibition), we presented two flashes that reversed their intensity difference during the flash. Both competition between recovered visual field and intact visual field, as well as competition within the intact visual field, were assessed. Healthy controls showed the expected primacy effect; they preferred the initially brighter target. Surprisingly, choice behaviour, even in the patients' supposedly 'intact' visual field, was significantly different from the control group for all but one. In the latter patient, competition was comparable to the controls. All other patients showed a significantly reduced preference to the brighter target, but still showed a small hint of primacy in the reversal conditions. The present results indicate that patients and controls have similar decision-making mechanisms but patients' choices are affected by a strong tendency to guess, even in the intact visual field. This tendency likely reveals slower integration of information, paired with a lower threshold. Current rehabilitation should therefore also include training focused on improving visual decision-making of the defective and the intact visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A C Geuzebroek
- Department Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Postbus 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen (route 205), The Netherlands.
| | - A V van den Berg
- Department Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Postbus 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen (route 205), The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Zhou B, Pöppel E, Wang L, Yang T, Zaytseva Y, Bao Y. Seeing without knowing: Operational principles along the early visual pathway. Psych J 2016; 5:145-60. [PMID: 27678480 DOI: 10.1002/pchj.141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2016] [Revised: 07/20/2016] [Accepted: 07/22/2016] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Bin Zhou
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology; Chinese Academy of Sciences; Beijing China
| | - Ernst Pöppel
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Key Laboratory of Machine Perception (Ministry of Education), and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health; Peking University; Beijing China
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Human Science Center; Ludwig-Maximilians-University; Munich Germany
| | - Lingyan Wang
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Key Laboratory of Machine Perception (Ministry of Education), and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health; Peking University; Beijing China
| | - Taoxi Yang
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Human Science Center; Ludwig-Maximilians-University; Munich Germany
| | - Yuliya Zaytseva
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Human Science Center; Ludwig-Maximilians-University; Munich Germany
- Department of National IT System of Mental Health and Brain Monitoring; National Institute of Mental Health; Klecany Czech Republic
- Department of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology, 3rd Faculty of Medicine; Charles University; Prague Czech Republic
| | - Yan Bao
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Key Laboratory of Machine Perception (Ministry of Education), and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health; Peking University; Beijing China
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Human Science Center; Ludwig-Maximilians-University; Munich Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Skvortsov A, Romashchuk A. Single case studies as a means for developing psychological theories. Psych J 2015; 4:231-42. [PMID: 26663629 DOI: 10.1002/pchj.117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2015] [Accepted: 09/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
The Socratic function of single case studies (SCSs) is described in its relation to the problem of scientific theory development. Contrary to the traditional point of view, the single case study is not a demonstration or verification of theoretical concepts, but a method of their generation and opportunity for analysis of their interrelations. Considering the case study from the perspective of the Socratic function brings to light important conclusions about the ecological validity of theory development. The essential features of the Socratic function are illustrated using the example of the famous Romantic Essays of Alexandr Luria.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anatoliy Skvortsov
- Faculty of Social Sciences/School of Psychology, Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russian Federation.,Center for Speech Pathology and Neurorehabilitation, Moscow, Russian Federation
| | - Alexander Romashchuk
- Faculty of Psychology/Department of Psychology, Moscow State University, Moscow, Russian Federation
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Lewald J, Tegenthoff M, Peters S, Hausmann M. Passive auditory stimulation improves vision in hemianopia. PLoS One 2012; 7:e31603. [PMID: 22666311 PMCID: PMC3362608 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031603] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2011] [Accepted: 01/10/2012] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Techniques employed in rehabilitation of visual field disorders such as hemianopia are usually based on either visual or audio-visual stimulation and patients have to perform a training task. Here we present results from a completely different, novel approach that was based on passive unimodal auditory stimulation. Ten patients with either left or right-sided pure hemianopia (without neglect) received one hour of unilateral passive auditory stimulation on either their anopic or their intact side by application of repetitive trains of sound pulses emitted simultaneously via two loudspeakers. Immediately before and after passive auditory stimulation as well as after a period of recovery, patients completed a simple visual task requiring detection of light flashes presented along the horizontal plane in total darkness. The results showed that one-time passive auditory stimulation on the side of the blind, but not of the intact, hemifield of patients with hemianopia induced an improvement in visual detections by almost 100% within 30 min after passive auditory stimulation. This enhancement in performance was reversible and was reduced to baseline 1.5 h later. A non-significant trend of a shift of the visual field border toward the blind hemifield was obtained after passive auditory stimulation. These results are compatible with the view that passive auditory stimulation elicited some activation of the residual visual pathways, which are known to be multisensory and may also be sensitive to unimodal auditory stimuli as were used here.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jörg Lewald
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Kasten E, Bunzenthal U, Sabel BA. Visual field recovery after vision restoration therapy (VRT) is independent of eye movements: an eye tracker study. Behav Brain Res 2006; 175:18-26. [PMID: 16970999 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.07.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2006] [Revised: 07/19/2006] [Accepted: 07/24/2006] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
AIM It has been argued that patients with visual field defects compensate for their deficit by making more frequent eye movements toward the hemianopic field and that visual field enlargements found after vision restoration therapy (VRT) may be an artefact of such eye movements. In order to determine if this was correct, we recorded eye movements in hemianopic subjects before and after VRT. METHODS Visual fields were measured in subjects with homonymous visual field defects (n=15) caused by trauma, cerebral ischemia or haemorrhage (lesion age >6 months). Visual field charts were plotted using both high-resolution perimetry (HRP) and conventional perimetry before and after a 3-month period of VRT, with eye movements being recorded with a 2D-eye tracker. This permitted quantification of eye positions and measurements of deviation from fixation. RESULTS VRT lead to significant visual field enlargements as indicated by an increase of stimulus detection of 3.8% when tested using HRP and about 2.2% (OD) and 3.5% (OS) fewer misses with conventional perimetry. Eye movements were expressed as the standard deviations (S.D.) of the eye position recordings from fixation. Before VRT, the S.D. was +/-0.82 degrees horizontally and +/-1.16 degrees vertically; after VRT, it was +/-0.68 degrees and +/-1.39 degrees , respectively. A cluster analysis of the horizontal eye movements before VRT showed three types of subjects with (i) small (n=7), (ii) medium (n=7) or (iii) large fixation instability (n=1). Saccades were directed equally to the right or the left side; i.e., with no preference toward the blind hemifield. After VRT, many subjects showed a smaller variability of horizontal eye movements. Before VRT, 81.6% of the recorded eye positions were found within a range of 1 degrees horizontally from fixation, whereas after VRT, 88.3% were within that range. In the 2 degrees range, we found 94.8% before and 98.9% after VRT. Subjects moved their eyes 5 degrees or more 0.3% of the time before VRT versus 0.1% after VRT. Thus, in this study, subjects with homonymous visual field defects who were attempting to fixate a central target while their fields were being plotted, typically showed brief horizontal shifts with no preference toward or away from the blind hemifield. These eye movements were usually less than 1 degrees from fixation. Large saccades toward the blind field after VRT were very rare. CONCLUSION VRT has no effect on either the direction or the amplitude of horizontal eye movements during visual field testing. These results argue against the theory that the visual field enlargements are artefacts induced by eye movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erich Kasten
- Institute of Medical Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke-University of Magdeburg, Leipziger Str. 44, D-39120 Magdeburg, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
Over the past 20 years, researchers have discovered over 30 separate visual areas in the cortex of the macaque monkey that exhibit specific responses to visual and environmental stimuli. Many of these areas are homologous to regions of the human visual cortex, and numerous syndromes involving these areas are described in the neurologic and ophthalmic literature. The focus of this review is the anatomy and physiology of these higher cortical visual areas, with special emphasis on their relevance to syndromes in humans. The early visual system processes information primarily by way of two separate systems: parvocellular and magnocellular. Thus, even at this early stage, visual information is functionally segregated. We will trace this segregation to downstream areas involved in increasingly complex visual processing and discuss the results of lesions in these areas in humans. An understanding of these areas is important, as many of these patients will first seek the attention of the ophthalmologist, often with vague, poorly defined complaints that may be difficult to specifically define.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C A Girkin
- Department of Ophthalmology, University of Alabama-Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
Brain plasticity refers to its ability to recover after damage. Visual field plasticity is not well recognized. We report a 12-year-old female who first presented with recurrent seizures and was subsequently found to have a large, right occipital cortical dysplasia on magnetic resonance imaging. Her visual field by Goldmann perimetry was totally normal. Visual-evoked potential studies revealed the left hemifield P100 response was detected maximally at the right temporal and parietal regions. A weak but reproducible right hemifield P100 response was located at the right medial skull base. Functional magnetic resonance imaging with flashlight stimulation revealed cerebral activity mainly at the right posterior temporal and parietal lobes and left occipital lobe. These studies suggested that the left hemifield function was located at the right posterior temporal and parietal lobes. The left occipital lobe may also have been reorganized, with a P100 vector pointing out from its inferiomedial base. We reviewed other related reported cases. We believe that visual-evoked potential studies and visual functional magnetic resonance imaging should be performed more liberally for recognition of visual field plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C K Kong
- Department of Paediatrics, Caritas Medical Centre, Kowloon, Hong Kong, People's Republic of China
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Abstract
On the basis of a taxonomy of functions an outline is given on potential deficits after brain lesions. The taxonomy distinguishes between "what"- and "how"-functions. Whereas for what-functions the localizing principle may apply, the how-functions being responsible for the logistics of the brain are non-locally represented. After brain injury the what- and how-functions may be differentially effected. On this basis 4 domains of functional rehabilitation can be distinguished, namely restitution and substitution of function after partly or complete losses of what-functions, activation and integration of function after alterations of activation or a disruption of functional coordination. Different strategies of functional rehabilitation are related to basic neurobiological principles which have been made responsible for restitution of function. Neuropsychological rehabilitation remains a challenge for neurobiology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N von Steinbüchel
- Institut für medizinische Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, München, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation from a Theoretical Point of View. Neuropsychol Rehabil 1992. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-77067-8_1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/15/2022]
|
11
|
Grant S, Berman NE. Mechanism of anomalous retinal correspondence: maintenance of binocularity with alteration of receptive-field position in the lateral suprasylvian (LS) visual area of strabismic cats. Vis Neurosci 1991; 7:259-81. [PMID: 1751416 DOI: 10.1017/s0952523800004077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
We have examined the effects of rearing kittens with a unilateral convergent strabismus, induced surgically at 3 weeks of age, on the binocularity (ocular dominance) and receptive-field position of neurons in the motion-sensitive lateral suprasylvian (LS) area of cat extrastriate cortex. Data were compared to those obtained from area 17 in the same animals, and from the two areas of cortex in normal adult cats. Interocular alignment of the operated cats was assessed in alert adults using corneal reflex photography and during recording from the positions of retinal landmarks under paralysis. The strabismus magnitude in each operated cat was calculated by comparison with equivalent data from the normal animals. Strabismus always caused a major loss of binocularity in area 17. The remaining binocular neurons had receptive-field (RF) pairs arising from positions of normal correspondence in the two retinae and would thus have been responsive to different regions of visual space through the misaligned eyes in the alert animal. In area LS, the effects were dependent on the strabismus magnitude. In the group of four cats with pronounced strabismus (18-30 deg crossed), a loss of binocularity occurred in area LS equivalent in severity to that in area 17. The majority of the remaining binocular LS neurons possessed RF pairs in normal retinal correspondence and would thus, in the alert animal, have received spatially disparate visual input through the two eyes. This also occurred in three other cats with more moderate strabismus (11-15 deg crossed), although only a small breakdown in the binocularity of area LS was apparent. The group of cats with mild strabismus (less than or equal to 10 deg crossed) had normal proportions of binocular neurons in area LS. In three of these cats, the maintenance of binocularity was accompanied by shifts in RF position, with visual inputs arising from anomalous retinal locations. These shifts compensated, in part, for the strabismus angle present in each cat, so that most of the binocular LS neurons would have received inputs from regions of visual correspondence through the misaligned eyes when the animal was alert. Similar mechanisms could afford a basis for the binocular visual compensations that occur in humans with small-angle strabismus of early onset. If so, anomalous retinal correspondence in such individuals would have as a locus areas of extrastriate cortex with a role in motion perception, and would involve alterations to the neural substrate underlying normal binocular vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Grant
- Department of Anatomy and Developmental Biology, University College London, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|