1
|
Willis GL, Armstrong SM. Orphan neurones and amine excess: the functional neuropathology of Parkinsonism and neuropsychiatric disease. BRAIN RESEARCH. BRAIN RESEARCH REVIEWS 1998; 27:177-242. [PMID: 9729369 DOI: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00013-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/08/2023]
Abstract
The aetiology and treatment of Parkinsonism is currently conceptualised within a dopamine (DA) deficiency-repletion framework. Loss of striatal DA is thought to cause motor impairment of which tremor, bradykinaesia and rigidity are prominent features. Repletion of deficient DA should at least minimise parkinsonian signs and symptoms. In Section 2, based on extensive pre-clinical and clinical findings, the instability of this approach to Parkinsonism is scrutinised as the existing negative findings challenging the DA deficiency hypothesis are reviewed and reinterpreted. In Section 3 it is suggested that Parkinsonism is due to a DA excess far from the striatum in the area of the posterior lateral hypothalamus (PLH) and the substantia nigra (SN). This unique area, around the diencephalon/mesencephalon border (DCMCB), is packed with many ascending and descending fibres which undergo functional transformation during degeneration, collectively labelled 'orphan neurones'. These malformed cells remain functional resulting in pathological release of transmitter and perpetual neurotoxicity. Orphan neurone formation is commonly observed in the PLH of animals and in man exhibiting Parkinsonism. The mechanism by which orphan neurones impair motor function is analogous to that seen in the diseased human heart. From this perspective, to conceptualise orphan neurones at the DCMCB as 'Time bombs in the brain' is neither fanciful nor unrealistic [E.M. Stricker, M.J. Zigmond, Comments on effects of nigro-striatal dopamine lesions, Appetite 5 (1984) 266-267] as the DA excess phenomenon demands a different therapeutic approach for the management of Parkinsonism. In Section 4 the focus is on this novel concept of treatment strategies by concentrating on non-invasive, pharmacological and surgical modification of functional orphan neurones as they affect adjacent systems. The Orphan neurone/DA excess hypothesis permits a more comprehensive and defendable interpretation of the interrelationship between Parkinsonism and schizophrenia and other related disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G L Willis
- The Bronowski Institute of Behavioural Neuroscience, Coliban Medical Centre, Kyneton, Victoria 3444, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Miller R. Striatal dopamine in reward and attention: a system for understanding the symptomatology of acute schizophrenia and mania. INTERNATIONAL REVIEW OF NEUROBIOLOGY 1993; 35:161-278. [PMID: 8463061 DOI: 10.1016/s0074-7742(08)60571-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- R Miller
- Department of Anatomy and Structural Biology, University of Otago Medical School, Dunedin, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
|
4
|
Ichihara K, Nabeshima T, Kameyama T. Effects of dopamine receptor agonists on passive avoidance learning in mice: interaction of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors. Eur J Pharmacol 1992; 213:243-9. [PMID: 1355736 DOI: 10.1016/0014-2999(92)90688-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined the effects of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor agonists on the acquisition stage of passive avoidance learning and on locomotor activity in mice. The D2 agonist, RU 24213 (1-10 mg/kg s.c.), and the non-selective agonist, apomorphine (0.3-3 mg/kg s.c.), but not the D1 agonist, SKF 38393 (1-10 mg/kg s.c.), impaired learning and activated locomotion. RU 24213 (1 mg/kg s.c.) was more effective in impairing learning than in activating locomotion. The concurrent administration of SKF 38393 (10 mg/kg i.p.) and RU 24213 (1 and 3 mg/kg s.c.) produced a synergistic effect in both behavioral situations. The D1 antagonist, SCH 23390 (0.025 mg/kg i.p.), slightly inhibited the effects of apomorphine and of the combination of SKF 38393 and RU 24213 on learning but not on locomotion. The D2 antagonist, (-)-sulpiride (40 mg/kg i.p.), completely blocked these effects in both situations. These results suggest that dopamine receptor agonists impair passive avoidance learning through the D2 receptor, and that D1 and D2 receptors act synergistically in this impairment, as they do in their effects on locomotion. The involvement of D1 and D2 receptors is qualitatively similar in each of these behaviors, although some small differences may exist.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Ichihara
- Department of Chemical Pharmacology, Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Meijo University, Nagoya, Japan
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Brock JW, Prasad C. Motor, but not sensory, cortical potentials are amplified by high-protein diet. Physiol Behav 1991; 50:887-93. [PMID: 1805278 DOI: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90410-p] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
Animals fed a high-protein diet (50% casein) are hyperactive and more responsive to nociceptive stimuli than those fed either a normal- or low-protein diet. The mechanisms mediating dietary protein-induced behavior are unknown and may include both central and peripheral neural effects. Adult, Sprague-Dawley rats were fed 50% casein (treatment group) and 24% casein (control group) ad lib for 36-40 weeks. The animals were anesthetized with alpha-chloralose and urethane (50 mg/kg and 1.5 mg/kg, IP). EEG recordings were averaged while the anesthetized animal was conditioned using an alerting stimulus-imperative stimulus (AS-IS) paradigm. AS consisted of a 1.5 kHz, 90 dB tone cue. This was followed 2 seconds later by IS, an electrical tail stimulation (11 V, 1.4 s duration). Two negative deflections (N1 and N2) were generated by the frontal cortex during the AS-IS interstimulus interval. N1, an alerting response, was not different between the two groups. N2 amplitude and peak latency were significantly increased in the high-protein group (205% and 117% of control, respectively; p less than 0.05). N2 represents the activation of cells in the motor cortex. Brainstem auditory-evoked responses and somatosensory-evoked potentials also were recorded, but no differences were observed between the two diet groups. These data suggest that consumption of a high-protein diet results in an increase in central arousal mechanisms (measured by cortical negativity response), specifically involving increased excitability of the motor cortex, that is not associated with a disorder of information processing in the cerebral cortex (measured by brainstem auditory-evoked responses and somatosensory-evoked potentials).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J W Brock
- Neuroscience Laboratory, Pennington Biomedical Research Center, Baton Rouge, LA 70808
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Cheal M, Lyon DR, Hubbard DC. Does attention have different effects on line orientation and line arrangement discrimination? THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. A, HUMAN EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 1991; 43:825-57. [PMID: 1775666 DOI: 10.1080/14640749108400959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
Visual search and texture segregation studies have led to the inference that stimuli differing in the orientation of their component line segments can be distinguished without focal attention, whereas stimuli that differ only in the arrangement of line segments cannot. In most of this research, the locus of attention has not been explicitly manipulated. In the first experiment presented here, attention was directed to a relevant peripheral target by a cue presented near the target location or at the fovea. Effects of attention on orientation discrimination were assessed in a two-alternative forced-choice task with targets that were either: (1) lines that slanted obliquely to the right or left, or were horizontal or vertical, or (2) Y-like targets that had a short arm leading obliquely right or left of a vertical line. In some groups, a four-alternative forced-choice test with lines at 0 degree, 45 degrees, 90 degrees, and 135 degrees orientations was used. Discrimination of these targets (i.e. targets that differ in the orientation of component line segments) was only minimally facilitated as the time between the onset of the valid cue and the onset of the target (cue-target stimulus onset asynchrony, SOA) was increased from 0 or 17 msec to 267 msec. In contrast, discrimination of targets that did not differ in the orientation of component line segments but differed in line arrangement (T-like characters), was greatly facilitated by longer cue-target SOAs. In Experiment 2, a cue misdirected attention on 20% of the trials. A decrement occurred on incorrectly cued trials in comparison to correctly cued trials for both types of stimuli used (lines and Ts). The results from these experiments suggest that discrimination of line orientation benefits less from focal attention than does discrimination of line arrangement, but that both discriminations suffer when attention must be disengaged from an irrelevant spatial location.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Cheal
- University of Dayton Research Institute, Higley, AZ 85236-2020
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Ichihara K, Nabeshima T, Kameyama T. Differential effects of pimozide and SCH 23390 on acquisition of learning in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 1989; 164:189-95. [PMID: 2569407 DOI: 10.1016/0014-2999(89)90458-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Our main purpose was to clarify the differences between the effects of dopamine D-1 (SCH 23390) and D-2 (pimozide) antagonists on memory acquisition in a water-finding and a one-trial passive avoidance task with ddY mice. In the water-finding task, pimozide (0.1 and 0.2 mg/kg i.p.) enhanced the acquisition of latent learning of mice although it suppressed exploratory behavior. In contrast, SCH 23390 (0.1 mg/kg i.p.) attenuated the acquisition of latent learning and suppressed exploratory behavior. In the passive avoidance task, pimozide (0.1 and 0.2 mg/kg i.p.) enhanced the acquisition of the passive avoidance response of mice. SCH 23390 (0.05 and 0.1 mg/kg i.p.) failed to enhance the acquisition of the passive avoidance response. These results could suggest that a moderate block of D-2 receptors enhances memory acquisition but blocking D-1 receptors affects it in an opposite way.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Ichihara
- Department of Chemical Pharmacology, Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Meijo University, Nagoya, Japan
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Piazza PV, Ferdico M, Russo D, Crescimanno G, Benigno A, Amato G. Facilitatory effect of ventral tegmental area A10 region on the attack behaviour in the cat: possible dopaminergic role in selective attention. Exp Brain Res 1988; 72:109-16. [PMID: 3169178 DOI: 10.1007/bf00248505] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
Lateral hypothalamus (LH) stimulation in cats which do not spontaneously attack rats, produces an attack pattern which may be divided into 3 main stages: the first, defined as exploratory time (ET), begins with an environmental search and culminates in orienting towards the prey; in the second, defined as attack time (AT), the cat stalks the rat; the last is the biting stage in which the cat seizes and kills the prey by biting its head and neck. The effects of ventral tegmental area (VTA) stimulation on the latency of the whole sequence and on the different stages of the attack pattern were studied. VTA activation resulted in a significant decrease of biting latency, due to the reduction of exploratory time. Moreover, a significant period of prey fixation, seldom present during LH stimulation alone, was observed after VTA-LH co-stimulation. Sulpiride injection caused the disappearance of VTA effects on the predatory pattern. The results indicate that VTA activation induces a decrease in behaviour related to exploration of the environment, and an increase in the focusing of attention on the prey, which seems an important component in the regulation of the predatory pattern. Pharmacological evidence indicates that the VTA effect is mediated by the mesolimbic-mesocortical dopaminergic (DA) system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P V Piazza
- Istituto di Fisiologia umana, Palermo, Italy
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Abstract
In four experiments using habituation procedures, it was shown that selective investigation of stimuli that are novel gradually developed between 13 and 28 days of age in gerbil pups. The youngest pups investigated the stimuli, but were highly variable in their responses to an object or to odors of conspecifics. Habituation could, but did not reliably, occur. By 21 days of age, investigation of a cup attenuated in a series of 5 one-minute trials and responses increased after the cup was moved to a new location. By 28 days of age, reliable habituation was shown 24 hours after a one-minute exposure to a cup. The data were used to infer how an immature process of selective attention may be responsible for variability in expression of habituation during development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Cheal
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe 85287
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Abstract
The Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguicultatus) is suggested as a model for aging research because of its unique physiological attributes, ease of handling, and because of data previously collected. Factors that demonstrate the gerbils' suitability in fulfulling practical and scientific considerations important in determining a model for aging research are listed. Additionally, several unique physiological attributes of gerbils are described. Based on these attributes and on review of research in gerbils, it is suggested that gerbils can serve as animal models for behavioral and biological processes, and for normative and pathological aspects for aging.
Collapse
|
11
|
Kelley AE, Winnock M, Stinus L. Amphetamine, apomorphine and investigatory behavior in the rat: analysis of the structure and pattern of responses. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1986; 88:66-74. [PMID: 3080777 DOI: 10.1007/bf00310515] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
In the present experiments, the effects of a wide range of doses of d-amphetamine and apomorphine were studied on investigatory behavior in an automated eight-hole box. Amphetamine (0.125, 0.25, 0.5, 1.0, 3.0, 5.0 mg/kg) increased frequency and total duration of responses, and decreased mean duration in a dose-dependent manner. The strategy and organization of responses, as measured by the order of hole-visits and hole-switching, were unchanged at lower doses of amphetamine but were altered at higher doses. Perseverative hole-poking was observed at the highest dose (5.0) as indicated by increased number of hole-pokes per hole-visit. Apomorphine (0.05, 0.1, 0.2, 0.4, 0.8, 1.6, 3.2 mg/kg) decreased mean duration of responses, but in contrast to amphetamine markedly diminished frequency. Locomotor activity was also measured at all doses of both drugs. Our observations indicate that these two stimulant drugs both of which increase motor activity, have markedly different effects on investigatory responses. It is likely that amphetamine increases prepotent response tendencies (i.e., hole-poking), although this does not necessarily reflect enhanced exploration. Further, the results obtained with amphetamine support predictions made by the Lyon-Robbins behavioral theory of amphetamine effects.
Collapse
|
12
|
Margulies DM. Selective attention and the brain: a hypothesis concerning the hippocampal--ventral striatal axis, the mediation of selective attention, and the pathogenesis of attentional disorders. Med Hypotheses 1985; 18:221-64. [PMID: 3853081 DOI: 10.1016/0306-9877(85)90027-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Abstract
The mechanisms mediating selective attention are not currently known. Dysfunctional selective attention is a common and prominent finding in a variety of medical and psychiatric conditions. A hypothesis is developed that efferents from the hippocampal formation are the final common pathway of processes which determine the noteworthiness of both exteroceptive and interoceptive stimuli, and that dysfunction of these efferents is a common pathway for a variety of anatomical, electrophysiological, and neurochemical lesions. This hypothesis suggests that clinical syndromes of disordered attention may be caused by various lesions of efferent connections from hippocampal formation to nucleus accumbens. The hypothesis further addresses the possibility that the threshold of hippocampus to various classes of stimuli may change on a diurnal and phasic basis. Experimental evidence that bears on the hypothesis is reviewed and experimental implications of the hypothesis are explored.
Collapse
|
13
|
Cheal ML, Johnson MR, Ellingboe J, Skupny AS. Perseveration of attention to conspecific odors and novel objects in castrated gerbils. Physiol Behav 1984; 33:563-70. [PMID: 6522476 DOI: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90372-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
Castrated male and female gerbils were tested for odor preference and for attention to conspecific odors and a novel object. Castrated gerbils housed with sham-operates preferred home odors, discriminated between two groups of male gerbils by olfactory cues, and perseverated in attention to odors of male gerbils and to a novel object. Similar perseveration to male conspecific odors was shown in gerbils given injections of L-DOPA (30 mg/kg). Combined treatment (castration and L-DOPA) resulted in additive effects on perseveration. This research challenges two general hypotheses of gonadal hormone function. The first, that changes in odor preference after castration are due to a loss in testicular androgen, is insufficient, because (1) female as well as male gerbils showed similar perseveration to odors, (2) there was a significant correlation between LH and duration of investigation of male conspecific odors, and (3) L-DOPA, the dopamine precurser, also caused perseveration to conspecific odors. The second, that gonadal hormones are responsible for persistence of attention, cannot be broadly generalized, because castration with resultant elevation of LH and regression of ventral glands resulted in perseveration of attention in male and female gerbils.
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
Haloperidol (0, 0.1, 0.3, or 1.0 mg/kg), the typical butyrophenone neuroleptic, decreased investigation of novel objects by gerbils following systemic injections. When given prior to apomorphine (1 or 3 mg/kg), haloperidol blocked apomorphine-induced disruption of selective attention in a dose-dependent manner. Thus, haloperidol acts like pimozide in this paradigm. In contrast, clozapine (0, 0.03, 0.1, 0.3, 1.0, or 3.0 mg/kg), the atypical dibenzodiazepine neuroleptic, increased frequency of investigation both soon after injection and 24h later, suggesting interference with maintenance of attention. When given with apomorphine (1.0 mg/kg), clozapine did not block apomorphine effects on selective attention. The results are related to differential effects of these neuroleptics on other behaviors and to their individual pharmacological profiles.
Collapse
|