1
|
McCrary T, Hughes T, Brook AH, Paul KS. Mirror, mirror? An evaluation of identical twin mirroring in tooth crown morphology. Anat Rec (Hoboken) 2024; 307:3102-3119. [PMID: 38372073 DOI: 10.1002/ar.25408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2023] [Revised: 01/30/2024] [Accepted: 01/31/2024] [Indexed: 02/20/2024]
Abstract
It has been estimated that 25% of monozygotic ("identical") twin pairs exhibit reverse asymmetry (RA) or "mirroring" of minor anatomical features as a result of delayed zygote division. Here, we examine whether identical twin mirroring accounts for patterns of dental asymmetry in a sample of monozygotic and dizygotic ("fraternal") twins. We focus on crown morphology to approach the following question: is there an association between dental RA frequency and twin type suggestive of the presence of mirror image twins in our sample? Data were collected from 208 deciduous and 196 permanent dentitions of participants of the University of Adelaide Twin Study using Arizona State University Dental Anthropology System standards. RA frequencies were compared across morphological complexes (deciduous, permanent), twin types (monozygotic, dizygotic), and traits. Fisher's exact tests were performed to formally evaluate the association between twin type and dental RA. Across the entire dataset, RA rates failed to exceed 8% for any twin type. In monozygotic twins, deciduous mirroring totaled 5.3% of observed cases, while permanent mirroring totaled 7.8% of observed cases. We found no statistically significant association between RA and twin type for any morphological character (p-value range: 0.07-1.00). Our results suggest the timing of monozygotic twin division does not explain the structure of asymmetry for our morphology dataset and that published estimates of identical twin mirroring rates may be inflated or contingent upon phenotype. Instead, rates reported for this sample more closely align with the proposed etiology of this condition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tess McCrary
- Department of Anthropology, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, Arkansas, USA
- University of Tennessee Health Science Center, College of Dentistry, Memphis, Tennessee, USA
| | - Toby Hughes
- Adelaide Dental School, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, SA, Australia
| | - Alan H Brook
- Adelaide Dental School, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, SA, Australia
- Barts and the London School of Medicine and Dentistry, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Kathleen S Paul
- Department of Anthropology, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, Arkansas, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Pfeifer LS, Schmitz J, Papadatou-Pastou M, Peterburs J, Paracchini S, Ocklenburg S. Handedness in twins: meta-analyses. BMC Psychol 2022; 10:11. [PMID: 35033205 PMCID: PMC8760823 DOI: 10.1186/s40359-021-00695-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2021] [Accepted: 11/24/2021] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND In the general population, 10.6% of people favor their left hand over the right for motor tasks. Previous research suggests higher prevalence of atypical (left-, mixed-, or non-right-) handedness in (i) twins compared to singletons, and in (ii) monozygotic compared to dizygotic twins. Moreover, (iii) studies have shown a higher rate of handedness concordance in monozygotic compared to dizygotic twins, in line with genetic factors playing a role for handedness. METHODS By means of a systematic review, we identified 59 studies from previous literature and performed three sets of random effects meta-analyses on (i) twin-to-singleton Odds Ratios (21 studies, n = 189,422 individuals) and (ii) monozygotic-to-dizygotic twin Odds Ratios (48 studies, n = 63,295 individuals), both times for prevalence of left-, mixed-, and non-right-handedness. For monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs we compared (iii) handedness concordance Odds Ratios (44 studies, n = 36,217 twin pairs). We also tested for potential effects of moderating variables, such as sex, age, the method used to assess handedness, and the twins' zygosity. RESULTS We found (i) evidence for higher prevalence of left- (Odds Ratio = 1.40, 95% Confidence Interval = [1.26, 1.57]) and non-right- (Odds Ratio = 1.36, 95% Confidence Interval = [1.22, 1.52]), but not mixed-handedness (Odds Ratio = 1.08, 95% Confidence Interval = [0.52, 2.27]) among twins compared to singletons. We further showed a decrease in Odds Ratios in more recent studies (post-1975: Odds Ratio = 1.30, 95% Confidence Interval = [1.17, 1.45]) compared to earlier studies (pre-1975: Odds Ratio = 1.90, 95% Confidence Interval = [1.59-2.27]). While there was (ii) no difference between monozygotic and dizygotic twins regarding prevalence of left- (Odds Ratio = 0.98, 95% Confidence Interval = [0.89, 1.07]), mixed- (Odds Ratio = 0.96, 95% Confidence Interval = [0.46, 1.99]), or non-right-handedness (Odds Ratio = 1.01, 95% Confidence Interval = [0.91, 1.12]), we found that (iii) handedness concordance was elevated among monozygotic compared to dizygotic twin pairs (Odds Ratio = 1.11, 95% Confidence Interval = [1.06, 1.18]). By means of moderator analyses, we did not find evidence for effects of potentially confounding variables. CONCLUSION We provide the largest and most comprehensive meta-analysis on handedness in twins. Although a raw, unadjusted analysis found a higher prevalence of left- and non-right-, but not mixed-handedness among twins compared to singletons, left-handedness was substantially more prevalent in earlier than in more recent studies. The single large, recent study which included birth weight, Apgar score and gestational age as covariates found no twin-singleton difference in handedness rate, but these covariates could not be included in the present meta-analysis. Together, the secular shift and the influence of covariates probably make it unsafe to conclude that twinning has a genuine relationship to handedness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lena Sophie Pfeifer
- Cognitive Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr University Bochum, Universitätsstraße 150, 44780, Bochum, Germany.
| | - Judith Schmitz
- School of Medicine, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland
| | - Marietta Papadatou-Pastou
- School of Education, Department of Primary Education, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece
- Biomedical Research Foundation of the Academy of Athens, Athens, Greece
| | - Jutta Peterburs
- Institute of Systems Medicine and Department of Human Medicine, MSH Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Marceau K, McMaster MTB, Smith TF, Daams JG, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI, Knopik VS. The Prenatal Environment in Twin Studies: A Review on Chorionicity. Behav Genet 2016; 46:286-303. [PMID: 26944881 PMCID: PMC4858569 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-016-9782-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2015] [Accepted: 01/01/2016] [Indexed: 03/26/2024]
Abstract
A literature search was conducted to identify articles examining the association of chorionicity (e.g., whether twins share a single chorion and thus placenta or have separate chorions/placentas) and genetics, psychiatry/behavior, and neurological manifestations in humans twins and higher-order multiples. The main aim was to assess how frequently chorionicity has been examined in relation to heritability estimates, and to assess which phenotypes may be most sensitive to, or affected by, bias in heritability estimates because of chorionicity. Consistent with the theory that some chorionicity effects could lead to overestimation and others to underestimation of heritability, there were instances of each across the many phenotypes reviewed. However, firm conclusions should not be drawn since some of the outcomes were only examined in one or few studies and often sample sizes were small. While the evidence for bias due to chorionicity was mixed or null for many outcomes, results do, however, consistently suggest that heritability estimates are underestimated for measures of birth weight and early growth when chorionicity is not taken into account.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristine Marceau
- />Division of Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Rhode Island Hospital, Providence, RI USA
- />Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Brown University, Providence, RI USA
- />Division of Behavioral Genetics, Coro West Suite 204, 1 Hoppin St, Providence, RI 02903 USA
| | - Minni T. B. McMaster
- />EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Taylor F. Smith
- />Division of Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Rhode Island Hospital, Providence, RI USA
- />Department of Psychology and Child Development, California Polytechnic State University, San Luis Obispo, CA USA
| | - Joost G. Daams
- />Academic Medical Center, Medical Library, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | - Dorret I. Boomsma
- />EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Valerie S. Knopik
- />Division of Behavioral Genetics, Department of Psychiatry, Rhode Island Hospital, Providence, RI USA
- />Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University, Providence, RI USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Left in the Genes. Laterality 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-801239-0.00002-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
5
|
Ooki S. Nongenetic factors associated with human handedness and footedness in Japanese twin children. Environ Health Prev Med 2012; 11:304-12. [PMID: 21432360 DOI: 10.1007/bf02898021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2006] [Accepted: 09/06/2006] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The aim of this study is to clarify the factors related to the handedness and footedness of twins using two of the largest databases on Japanese twins available. METHODS The first group consisted of 1,131 twin pairs, all school children either 11 or 12 years old (S group), and the second group consisted of 951 twin pairs of different ages (1-15 years) in several maternal associations (M group). All data were gathered using a questionnaire. Factors associated with the handedness or footedness of twin individuals were analyzed by univariate and multivariate logistic analyses. RESULTS Multivariate logistic analysis showed that for handedness, birth year (OR=1.02) and neonatal asphyxia (OR=1.62) were selected in the S group, and sex (OR=1.34), the age of twins (OR=1.56), parity (OR=1.31), gestational age (OR=1.58), and family history (OR=1.82) were selected in the M group. for footedness, birth complications (OR=1.37) were selected in the S group, and sex (OR=1.33), the age of twins (OR=1.69), gestational age (OR=1.83), and family history (OR=2.49) were selected in the M group. Factors associated with handedness and footedness specific to twins, such as zygosity, placentation, birth order within twin pairs and the sex of the cotwin, were not found, although being a twin might have some effects. CONCLUSION It was concluded that factors that affect handedness or footedness in general, such as sex, birth year, age, parity, neonatal asphyxia, gestational age, birth complications, and family history, seem to have stronger effects on handedness and footedness than being a twin.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Syuichi Ooki
- Department of Health Science, Ishikawa Prefectural Nursing University, Tsu 7-1 Nakanuma, 929-1212, Kahoku, Ishikawa, Japan,
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Gutknecht L, Spitz E, Carlier M. Long-term effect of placental type on anthropometrical and psychological traits among monozygotic twins: a follow up study. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2012. [DOI: 10.1375/twin.2.3.212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
AbstractThe long-term effect of differences in placentation in MZ twins is a controversial subject. An effect has been clearly established for birth weight but data on psychological traits are still under debate. We studied 20 pairs of monochorionic MZ (MCMZ) and 24 pairs of dichorionic MZ (DCMZ) twins. A chorion effect was observed for Block Design (WISC-R) confirming a previous report: MC MZ co-twins were more similar that DC MZ co-twins. For anthropometrical measures, an expected effect in the opposite direction was found. No chorion effect was significant for the other variables. A follow up was undertaken 3 years later using cognitive, national academic evaluations, and personality variables. The sample included 16 pairs of MC MZ and 22 pairs of DC MZ twins. Again a chorion effect was observed on anthropometrical variables but results on the Block Design were not replicated. However, the MC MZ co-twins were more similar than the DC MZ co-twins for two other cognitive variables: Perceptual Organization Index from the WISC-R and Global Visualisation from a Belgian reasoning test. Among the personality variables only one was sensitive to a chorion effect. The discussion focuses on the need for larger samples to achieve adequate power in statistical comparisons.
Collapse
|
7
|
Medland SE, Duffy DL, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Martin NG. Handedness in Twins: Joint Analysis of Data From 35 Samples. Twin Res Hum Genet 2012. [DOI: 10.1375/twin.9.1.46] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
AbstractSimultaneous analysis of handedness data from 35 samples of twins (with a combined sample size of 21,127 twin pairs) found a small but significant additive genetic effect accounting for 25.47% of the variance (95% confidence interval [CI] 15.69–29.51%). No common environmental influences were detected (C = 0.00; 95% CI 0.00–7.67%), with the majority of the variance, 74.53%, explained by factors unique to the individual (95% CI 70.49–78.67%). No significant heterogeneity was observed within studies that used similar methods to assess handedness, or across studies that used different methods. At an individual level the majority of studies had insufficient power to reject a purely unique environmental model due to insufficient power to detect familial aggregation. This lack of power is seldom mentioned within studies, and has contributed to the misconception that twin studies of handedness are not informative.
Collapse
|
8
|
Prescott CA, Johnson RC, McArdle JJ. Chorion type as a possible influence on the results and interpretation of twin study data. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2012. [DOI: 10.1375/twin.2.4.244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
9
|
Abstract
It is well established that the distribution of the combinations of handedness (LL, LR, and RR) in twin pairs is roughly binomial regardless of zygosity or sex. This fact has challenged current genetic models of handedness. We show here that: (a) there must be at least one environmental determinant of handedness; and (b) the binomial distribution does not in itself invalidate genetic models of handedness. In singletons, left-handedness is strongly associated with low birth weight. In twins, this association is weaker (apparently as a consequence of a stronger association with birth order--i.e., with being first-born--within the pair). We speculate that the hazards associated with being first-born in twin pairs (e.g., trauma) are more closely associated with left-handedness than are the hazards associated with being second-born (e.g., hypoxia).
Collapse
|
10
|
Cerebral asymmetries in monozygotic twins: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:3086-93. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2010] [Revised: 06/08/2010] [Accepted: 06/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
11
|
Driven to be inactive? The genetics of physical activity. PROGRESS IN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY AND TRANSLATIONAL SCIENCE 2010; 94:271-90. [PMID: 21036329 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-375003-7.00010-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
Abstract
The health implications of physical inactivity, including its integral role in promoting obesity, are well known and have been well documented. Physical activity is a multifactorial behavior with various factors playing a role in determining individual physical activity levels. Research using both human and animal models in the past several years has clearly indicated that genetics is associated with physical activity. Furthermore, researchers have identified several significant and suggestive genomic quantitative trait loci associated with physical activity. To date, the identities of the causal genes underlying physical activity regulation are unclear, with few strong candidate genes. The current research provides a foundation from which future confirmatory research can be launched as well as determination of the mechanisms through which the genetic factors act. The application of this knowledge could significantly augment the information available for physical activity behavior change interventions resulting in more efficient programs for those predisposed to be inactive.
Collapse
|
12
|
Suzuki K, Ando J, Satou N. Genetic effects on infant handedness under spatial constraint conditions. Dev Psychobiol 2009; 51:605-15. [DOI: 10.1002/dev.20395] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
13
|
Machin G. Non-identical monozygotic twins, intermediate twin types, zygosity testing, and the non-random nature of monozygotic twinning: A review. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C-SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 2009; 151C:110-27. [DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.c.30212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 106] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
|
14
|
Chen WJ, Su CH. Handedness and schizotypy in non-clinical populations: Influence of handedness measures and age on the relationship. Laterality 2007; 11:331-49. [PMID: 16754234 DOI: 10.1080/13576500600572693] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
This study aimed to evaluate the relation of handedness to schizotypal personality, with the influence of different handedness measures and age on the relationship investigated as well among representative samples of school students and community adults in Taiwan, where social pressure against certain left-handed actions was strong. In a total of 175 primary school students, 1020 junior high-school students, and 342 adult participants aged 20 to 65 years, all the participants completed the 12-item Annett handedness questionnaire, and the latter two groups further completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) and the Perceptual Aberration Scale (PAS). There was a trend of increasing non-right handedness with younger age groups. Two classification methods in handedness, Annett's or Briggs-Nebes' three-category classification, led to very different frequency distribution and relation to schizotypy. For the adolescents, either Annett's classification or quantitative measures (Hand Preference Index and either-hand use scores) in handedness exhibited an association with schizotypy, whereas for the adults the either-hand use score was the only measure that did so. In contrast, no such difference was found using the Briggs-Nebes' classification method. The association of the non-right or mixed handedness was more consistent and of greater magnitude with the positive aspect of schizotypy, especially the PAS and the Cognitive-perceptual Dysfunction of the SPQ. The results highlight the potential of adolescents for future investigation on certain common causes underlying atypical cerebral lateralisation and positive schizotypy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wei J Chen
- National Taiwan University College of Public Health, Taiwan.
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
OOKI S. Nongenetic Factors Associated with Human Handedness and Footedness in Japanese Twin Children. Environ Health Prev Med 2006. [DOI: 10.1265/ehpm.11.304] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
|
16
|
Su CH, Kuo PH, Lin CCH, Chen WJ. A School-based Twin Study of Handedness among Adolescents in Taiwan. Behav Genet 2005; 35:723-33. [PMID: 16273317 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-005-6189-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2004] [Accepted: 06/01/2005] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
This study aimed to evaluate whether twinning might influence handedness and the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to handedness in a total of 321 pairs of twins, 36 same-sex sib-pairs, and 1020 singletons, aged 12-16 and systematically recruited from the junior high schools in Taipei. Twins' zygosity was determined by a combination of DNA typing and physical similarity. The direction and consistency of handedness in twins did not differ from that seen in singletons. Compared with the full model containing additive genes (A), shared (C), and non-shared (E) environment, both AE and CE models had equivalently acceptable fit. The contribution from additive genes in the AE model was estimated to be 16% (directional) to 13% (consistent) for the continuous handedness and 34-10% for the categorical one, whereas the corresponding contribution from shared environment in the CE model was 14-14% and 32-11%, respectively. Handedness in adolescents appears to be not influenced by twinning and not substantially heritable, whereas environmental factors, especially those not shared between siblings, are the most important ones for explaining individual variations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiu-Hsia Su
- Institute of Epidemiology, College of Public Health, National Taiwan University, 1 Jen-Ai Road, Sec. 1, Taipei 100, Taiwan
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Abstract
This paper reviews three designs that compare co-twins in tests of prenatal programming. The first design uses co-twin comparisons to attempt within-family replications of between-family associations, reported in non-twins, between reduced fetal growth and risk for later disease or behavior disorder. These replication efforts are mixed, perhaps due to small samples of weight-discordant twins, perhaps confounded by differences in the meaning of fetal growth in twins. The second design compares monozygotic co-twins from pairs differing in their placentation, with some evidence, again constrained by sample limitations that twins sharing a single placenta are more alike in dimensions of cognitive ability and personality. A third design employs a dermatoglyphic index of bilateral asymmetry as a marker of reduced buffering from environmental noise, with positive results from MZ twins that await necessary replication in longitudinal data from non-twins. Twin comparisons, especially the comparisons of monozygotic co-twins, offer an informative and incisive approach for research on prenatal programming in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Richard J Rose
- Department of Psychology, Indiana University, Room 132, Psychology Bldg, 1101 East Tenth Street, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abstract
In the largest meta-analysis of twins and singletons conducted to date we have found a higher incidence of left-handedness in twins compared to singletons. Our analysis revealed no difference in the frequency of left-handedness among monozygotic versus dizygotic twins. However, identical twins were more likely to be concordant for hand preference than non-identical twins, which is consistent with a genetic model of handedness. Prior analyses have not revealed these findings consistently, and this has led to a number of conflicting models of handedness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N L Sicotte
- Department of Neurology, UCLA School of Medicine, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Valera EM, Heller W, Berenbaum H. A twin study of individual differences in perceptual asymmetry. Laterality 2005; 4:299-311. [PMID: 15513119 DOI: 10.1080/713754340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Individual differences in perceptual asymmetry have been associated with individual differences in cognitive abilities, personality characteristics, and psychiatric symptoms, for which between-person variation appears to be genetically influenced. Perceptual asymmetry scores are also associated with direction of handedness, for which between-person variation does not appear to be genetically influenced. To assess whether between-person variation of perceptual asymmetry scores is genetically influenced, we examined asymmetry on a freevision task of face processing, the Chimeric Faces Task (CFT), in a sample of 31 monozygotic (MZ) and 20 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs. MZ and DZ within twin-pair resemblances were compared to assess genetic and familial influences on asymmetric hemispheric function. We found that twins within a pair were no more likely to resemble each other than were unrelated individuals. The results suggest that the between-person variation in CFT perceptual asymmetry is not influenced by genes or shared environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E M Valera
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign 61820, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Doyen AL, Carlier M. Measuring handedness: a validation study of Bishop's reaching card test. Laterality 2005; 7:115-30. [PMID: 15513192 DOI: 10.1080/13576500143000140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
Abstract
The aim of the study was the validation of Bishop's reaching card test (Bishop et al., 1996) in a French population. Annett's peg-moving test (1985) and the Purdue pegboard (Tiffin, 1948) were also used to assess manual performance. Subgroups of left- and right-handers were established according to manual preference as measured by Annett's questionnaire (1985). A sample of 260 adults was tested, including 158 self-professed right-handers, 77 self-professed left-handers, and 25 self-professed ambidextrous people. The three manual performance tests and the questionnaire presented good metric qualities (homogeneity, test-retest reliability). However the constitution of subgroups of laterality (in particular left-handers) as a function of manual preference could not be validated by classic tasks, or by Bishop's test, which is supposed to combine features of both preference and performance. Moreover the link between performance and preference was weak. Both evaluations should be used to produce a complete pattern of laterality for each participant.
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
Handedness and language skills were assessed in 196 same-sex twin pairs (101 MZ and 95 DZ), who were selected from an epidemiological study of twins, so that children with risk of language impairment were over-represented. When assessed at 6 years of age, 83 children met criteria for specific language impairment (SLI), 32 had general developmental (GD) delay, and the remaining 277 were typically-developing (TD). Hand preference (HP) assessed by inventory did not distinguish SLI, TD, or GD groups. The quantification of hand preference (QHP) measure, which measures persistence of a HP when reaching across the midline, did show weaker HP in those with SLI compared to the other two groups. It is suggested that the QHP measure assesses developmental aspects of manual lateralization, and is sensitive to neurodevelopmental immaturity in SLI. Furthermore, genetic analysis showed that the QHP measure, unlike the handedness inventory, was significantly heritable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D V M Bishop
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3UD, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
Hand, eye and foot preferences in Tunisia were examined in relation to age, gender and geographic location. We analyzed 1291 questionnaires from 653 men and 638 women, aged 8 to 74 years. Despite the cultural pressure against the use of the left hand for food-related activities, the overall frequency of left-hand writers (10.9%) was comparable to that found in the Western world. The frequency of left-hand writers was higher for subjects with one left-handed parent, and even higher for subjects with two left-handed parents than for subjects whose parents were right handed. The frequency of left-hand writers dropped to 5.9% in the older age-group; it was found to be higher in Tunis, the largest and most Occidental city, than in other cities, while left-hand eating was lower in the South than in the Center or in the North. The frequency of left-footers also dropped in the older age groups and was higher in Tunis than in other cities. Eye preference, consistency of preferred-hand use, crossed hand-eye laterality, crossed hand-foot laterality and gender-related differences in lateral preferences were all comparable to Western results. These data suggest that lateral preferences are partly influenced by a genetic factor, but that handedness (and to a lesser degree footedness) emerges from the intricate interaction of several factors including genetic and cultural influences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Riadh Dahmen
- Laboratoire Cognition et Dévelopment, CNRS, Boulogne, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Roubertoux PL, Le Roy I, Tordjman S, Cherfou A, Migliore-Samour D. Analysis of quantitative trait loci for behavioral laterality in mice. Genetics 2003; 163:1023-30. [PMID: 12663540 PMCID: PMC1462476 DOI: 10.1093/genetics/163.3.1023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Laterality is believed to have genetic components, as has been deduced from family studies in humans and responses to artificial selection in mice, but these genetic components are unknown and the underlying physiological mechanisms are still a subject of dispute. We measured direction of laterality (preferential use of left or right paws) and degree of laterality (absolute difference between the use of left and right paws) in C57BL/6ByJ (B) and NZB/BlNJ (N) mice and in their F(1) and F(2) intercrosses. Measurements were taken of both forepaws and hind paws. Quantitative trait loci (QTL) did not emerge for direction but did for degree of laterality. One QTL for forepaw (LOD score = 5.6) and the second QTL for hind paw (LOD score = 7.2) were both located on chromosome 4 and their peaks were within the same confidence interval. A QTL for plasma luteinizing hormone concentration was also found in the confidence interval of these two QTL. These results suggest that the physiological mechanisms underlying degree of laterality react to gonadal steroids.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pierre L Roubertoux
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Institut de Neurosciences Physiologiques et Cognitives, INPC.CNRS, 13402 Marseille Cedex 20, France.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Frederiksen H, Christensen K. The influence of genetic factors on physical functioning and exercise in second half of life. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2003; 13:9-18. [PMID: 12535312 DOI: 10.1034/j.1600-0838.2003.20219.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
During the past decades, a number of studies of families and twins in particular have assessed the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to traits reflecting various aspects of physical functioning: maximal O2 uptake, muscular endurance, muscular strength, muscle cross sectional area, flexibility, and trainability. Although the estimate of the size of the genetic component differs between the various studies, they point towards a moderate to substantial genetic influence on these phenotypes. Most of the studies have used only young and healthy study subjects, although in recent years phenotypes of particular importance to the elderly and the oldest-old (e.g., activities-of-daily living abilities) have also been shown to have substantial genetic component. Furthermore, behavioural studies have also revealed a genetic contribution to the disposition to level of leisure time physical activity. At present, there is still a few association studies on specific genetic variants, and the results have either been inconsistent or failed to show an association with physical functioning. Therefore, the mechanisms through which the genetic influence is expressed, is still an enigma. Here, we summarise the evidence currently available for a genetic influence on physical functioning and disposition to leisure time physical activity with a focus on recent Danish twin data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Frederiksen
- The Danish Twin Registry, Epidemiology, Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, DK-5000 Odense, Denmark
| | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
|
26
|
Basso O, Olsen J, Holm NV, Skytthe A, Vaupel JW, Christensen K. Handedness and mortality: a follow-up study of Danish twins born between 1900 and 1910. Epidemiology 2000; 11:576-80. [PMID: 10955411 DOI: 10.1097/00001648-200009000-00014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The declining prevalence of left-handed individuals with increasing age has led to two main avenues of hypotheses; the association is due either (1) to a birth cohort effect and/or an age effect caused by a switch to right-handedness with advancing age or (2) to mortality selection that reduces survival in left-handed individuals, or both. It is uncertain whether a cohort or age effect can explain the decline in age-related prevalence, and conflicting evidence exists in favor of the mortality hypothesis. We compared mortality in a subgroup of 118 opposite-handed twin pairs by counting in how many instances the right-handed twin died first. There was no evidence of differential survival between right-handed and non-right-handed individuals in the entire 1900-1910 cohort. With respect to the number of right-handed twins who died first, there was no material disadvantage among those who were not right-handed. In 60% (95% confidence interval = 49.0-71.5%) of dizygotic pairs, the right-handed twins died first. In 50% of monozygotic pairs, right-handed twins died first. The prevalence of not being right-handed was higher among males (9.2%) than females (6.5%); there was a similar frequency of non-right-handedness in monozygotic (8.0%) and dizygotic (7.8%) twins. We did not find evidence of excess mortality among non-right-handed adult twins in this follow-up study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- O Basso
- The Danish Epidemiology Science Centre at the Department of Epidemiology and Social Medicine, Aarhus University
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Derom C, Thiery E, Vlietinck R, Loos R, Derom R. Handedness in twins according to zygosity and chorion type: a preliminary report. Behav Genet 1996; 26:407-8. [PMID: 8771900 DOI: 10.1007/bf02359484] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
In the course of the East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey (EFPTS), handedness was assessed as part of a genealogical study (Meulemans et al., 1995) in 1616 twins (808 twin pairs) aged 6 to 28. Our findings are that, in this large population-based study with known placentation and zygosity, the often observed higher frequency of left-handedness in twins is confirmed, that it appears to be independent of zygosity and chorion type, and that the belief that discordant handedness in monozygotic twins represents mirror-imaging is mythical.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Derom
- Centre for Human Genetics, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|