1
|
In mixed company: two macaws are self-regarding in a symbolic prosocial choice task. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-021-03123-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
|
2
|
Schnell AK, Boeckle M, Rivera M, Clayton NS, Hanlon RT. Cuttlefish exert self-control in a delay of gratification task. Proc Biol Sci 2021; 288:20203161. [PMID: 33653135 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2020.3161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
The ability to exert self-control varies within and across taxa. Some species can exert self-control for several seconds whereas others, such as large-brained vertebrates, can tolerate delays of up to several minutes. Advanced self-control has been linked to better performance in cognitive tasks and has been hypothesized to evolve in response to specific socio-ecological pressures. These pressures are difficult to uncouple because previously studied species face similar socio-ecological challenges. Here, we investigate self-control and learning performance in cuttlefish, an invertebrate that is thought to have evolved under partially different pressures to previously studied vertebrates. To test self-control, cuttlefish were presented with a delay maintenance task, which measures an individual's ability to forgo immediate gratification and sustain a delay for a better but delayed reward. Cuttlefish maintained delay durations for up to 50-130 s. To test learning performance, we used a reversal-learning task, whereby cuttlefish were required to learn to associate the reward with one of two stimuli and then subsequently learn to associate the reward with the alternative stimulus. Cuttlefish that delayed gratification for longer had better learning performance. Our results demonstrate that cuttlefish can tolerate delays to obtain food of higher quality comparable to that of some large-brained vertebrates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Markus Boeckle
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.,Karl Landsteiner University of Health Science, Krems, Austria
| | - Micaela Rivera
- Department of Psychology, Ripon College, Ripon, WI 54971, USA
| | - Nicola S Clayton
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Roger T Hanlon
- Eugene Bell Center, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Association between social factors and gastrointestinal parasite product excretion in a group of non-cooperatively breeding carrion crows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-021-02967-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
4
|
Proximate mechanisms and relational history: the interdependence of food transfers in socially housed orang-utans (Pongo abelii). Anim Behav 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.07.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
5
|
Wascher CAF, Feider B, Bugnyar T, Dufour V. Crows and common ravens do not reciprocally exchange tokens with a conspecific to gain food rewards. Ethology 2019. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.12970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Thomas Bugnyar
- Department of Cognitive Biology University of Vienna Vienna Austria
| | - Valérie Dufour
- Team of Cognitive and Social Ethology UMR 7247 PRC CNRS Inra IFCE University of Tours Tours France
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Liévin‐Bazin A, Pineaux M, Le Covec M, Gahr M, Bovet D, Bayern AMP. Food sharing and affiliation: An experimental and longitudinal study in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus). Ethology 2019. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.12850] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Agatha Liévin‐Bazin
- Laboratoire Éthologie Cognition Développement, EA 3456 Université Paris Nanterre Nanterre France
| | - Maxime Pineaux
- Laboratoire Évolution et Diversité Biologique, UMR 5174 Université de Toulouse Midi‐Pyrénées, CNRS, IRD, UPS Toulouse France
| | - Mathilde Le Covec
- Laboratoire Éthologie Cognition Développement, EA 3456 Université Paris Nanterre Nanterre France
| | - Manfred Gahr
- Department of Behavioural Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics Max Planck Institute for Ornithology Seewiesen Germany
| | - Dalila Bovet
- Laboratoire Éthologie Cognition Développement, EA 3456 Université Paris Nanterre Nanterre France
| | - Auguste M. P. Bayern
- Department of Behavioural Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics Max Planck Institute for Ornithology Seewiesen Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Boucherie PH, Poulin N, Dufour V. Not much ado about something: behavioural mechanisms of pair bond separation and formation in long-term pairing rooks. ECOSCIENCE 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/11956860.2017.1414671] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Palmyre H. Boucherie
- Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien, University of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Strasbourg, France
| | - Nicolas Poulin
- Centre Statistique de Strasbourg, Institut de Recherche Mathématique Avancée, Strasbourg, France
| | - Valérie Dufour
- Cognitive and Social Ethology Team, Délégation Alsace, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Strasbourg, France
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
|
9
|
Kopp KS, Liebal K. Here you are!—Selective and active food sharing within and between groups in captive Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2016. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-016-2130-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
10
|
Boucherie PH, Mariette MM, Bret C, Dufour V. Bonding beyond the pair in a monogamous bird: impact on social structure in adult rooks (Corvus frugilegus). BEHAVIOUR 2016. [DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-00003372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
The formation of social bonds outside the mated pair is not frequently reported in monogamous birds, although it may be expected in some species like rooks, living in groups all year round. Here we explore the social structure of captive adult rooks over three breeding seasons. We recorded proximities and affiliations (i.e., allofeeding, allopreening, contact-sit) to classify relationships according to their strength. Three categories of relationships emerged: primary (i.e., pairs), secondary and weak relationships. Affiliations and sexual behaviours were not restricted to pairs, and secondary relationships were clearly recognizable. Mixed-sex secondary relationships were qualitatively equivalent to pairs (i.e., same behaviours in the same proportions), although they were quantitatively less intense. Same sex pairs occurred, and were qualitatively equivalent to mixed-sex pairs. Overall we found that rooks social structure is more than just an aggregation of pairs, which highlights the importance of considering extra-pair relationships in socially monogamous birds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Palmyre H. Boucherie
- Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien, University of Strasbourg, 23 rue Becquerel, 67087 Strasbourg, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UMR 7178, 23 rue Becquerel, 67087 Strasbourg, France
| | - Mylène M. Mariette
- Centre for Integrative Ecology, School of Life & Environmental Sciences, Deakin University, Melbourne, VIC 3216, Australia
| | - Céline Bret
- German Primate Centre, Leibniz-Institut für Primatenforschung, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
| | - Valérie Dufour
- Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien, University of Strasbourg, 23 rue Becquerel, 67087 Strasbourg, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UMR 7178, 23 rue Becquerel, 67087 Strasbourg, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Tranmer M, Marcum CS, Morton FB, Croft DP, de Kort SR. Using the relational event model (REM) to investigate the temporal dynamics of animal social networks. Anim Behav 2015; 101:99-105. [PMID: 26190856 PMCID: PMC4502436 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.12.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Social dynamics are of fundamental importance in animal societies. Studies on nonhuman animal social systems often aggregate social interaction event data into a single network within a particular time frame. Analysis of the resulting network can provide a useful insight into the overall extent of interaction. However, through aggregation, information is lost about the order in which interactions occurred, and hence the sequences of actions over time. Many research hypotheses relate directly to the sequence of actions, such as the recency or rate of action, rather than to their overall volume or presence. Here, we demonstrate how the temporal structure of social interaction sequences can be quantified from disaggregated event data using the relational event model (REM). We first outline the REM, explaining why it is different from other models for longitudinal data, and how it can be used to model sequences of events unfolding in a network. We then discuss a case study on the European jackdaw, Corvus monedula, in which temporal patterns of persistence and reciprocity of action are of interest, and present and discuss the results of a REM analysis of these data. One of the strengths of a REM analysis is its ability to take into account different ways in which data are collected. Having explained how to take into account the way in which the data were collected for the jackdaw study, we briefly discuss the application of the model to other studies. We provide details of how the models may be fitted in the R statistical software environment and outline some recent extensions to the REM framework.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Tranmer
- Social Statistics, The University of Manchester, Manchester, U.K
| | | | | | | | - Selvino R. de Kort
- School of Science and the Environment, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, U.K
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Salwiczek LH, Emery NJ, Schlinger B, Clayton NS. The development of caching and object permanence in Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica): which emerges first? J Comp Psychol 2009; 123:295-303. [PMID: 19685971 PMCID: PMC2836840 DOI: 10.1037/a0016303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Recent studies on the food-caching behavior of corvids have revealed complex physical and social skills, yet little is known about the ontogeny of food caching in relation to the development of cognitive capacities. Piagetian object permanence is the understanding that objects continue to exist even when they are no longer visible. Here, the authors focus on Piagetian Stages 3 and 4, because they are hallmarks in the cognitive development of both young children and animals. Our aim is to determine in a food-caching corvid, the Western scrub-jay, whether (1) Piagetian Stage 4 competence and tentative caching (i.e., hiding an item invisibly and retrieving it without delay), emerge concomitantly or consecutively; (2) whether experiencing the reappearance of hidden objects enhances the timing of the appearance of object permanence; and (3) discuss how the development of object permanence is related to behavioral development and sensorimotor intelligence. Our findings suggest that object permanence Stage 4 emerges before tentative caching, and independent of environmental influences, but that once the birds have developed simple object-permanence, then social learning might advance the interval after which tentative caching commences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucie H Salwiczek
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Schwab C, Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K. Preferential learning from non-affiliated individuals in jackdaws (Corvus monedula). Behav Processes 2008; 79:148-55. [PMID: 18674604 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2007] [Revised: 06/13/2008] [Accepted: 07/06/2008] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
It has been suggested that affiliated social relations may facilitate information transfer between individuals. We here tested this rarely examined hypothesis with juvenile and adult jackdaws (Corvus monedula) in three stimulus enhancement tasks, both in a non-food context (experiment 1) and in a food context (experiments 2 and 3). We first show that siblings and pair partners maintain stronger bonded social relations than do non-siblings and non-pair partners. We therefore tested individuals in sibling and non-sibling dyads and, later in ontogeny, in pair and non-pair dyads. Jackdaws either did not learn from any other conspecific (experiment 1), or they learned from non-affiliated individuals (non-siblings, non-pair partners in experiments 2 and 3). This may be related to two main characteristics of jackdaws' affiliated relationships. First, affiliates share food at a high rate and may rely on their knowledgeable partners to secure food rather than learning from them. Second, affiliates spend most time in close spatial proximity to each other which increases the probability that they simultaneously experience occurrences in their environment. Hence, spatially more distant individuals, which are more likely to be non-affiliated, face different foraging situations and may therefore provide more relevant information which may lead to selective social learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine Schwab
- Konrad-Lorenz-Research Station for Ethology, Gruenau, Austria; Department of Behavioural Biology, University of Vienna, Austria.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Emery NJ, Seed AM, von Bayern AMP, Clayton NS. Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2007; 362:489-505. [PMID: 17255008 PMCID: PMC2346513 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1991] [Citation(s) in RCA: 243] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
The 'social intelligence hypothesis' was originally conceived to explain how primates may have evolved their superior intellect and large brains when compared with other animals. Although some birds such as corvids may be intellectually comparable to apes, the same relationship between sociality and brain size seen in primates has not been found for birds, possibly suggesting a role for other non-social factors. But bird sociality is different from primate sociality. Most monkeys and apes form stable groups, whereas most birds are monogamous, and only form large flocks outside of the breeding season. Some birds form lifelong pair bonds and these species tend to have the largest brains relative to body size. Some of these species are known for their intellectual abilities (e.g. corvids and parrots), while others are not (e.g. geese and albatrosses). Although socio-ecological factors may explain some of the differences in brain size and intelligence between corvids/parrots and geese/albatrosses, we predict that the type and quality of the bonded relationship is also critical. Indeed, we present empirical evidence that rook and jackdaw partnerships resemble primate and dolphin alliances. Although social interactions within a pair may seem simple on the surface, we argue that cognition may play an important role in the maintenance of long-term relationships, something we name as 'relationship intelligence'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nathan J Emery
- Sub-department of Animal Behaviour, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB3 8AA, UK.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
|