1
|
Chang JC, Perich MG, Miller LE, Gallego JA, Clopath C. De novo motor learning creates structure in neural activity that shapes adaptation. Nat Commun 2024; 15:4084. [PMID: 38744847 PMCID: PMC11094149 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-48008-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2023] [Accepted: 04/18/2024] [Indexed: 05/16/2024] Open
Abstract
Animals can quickly adapt learned movements to external perturbations, and their existing motor repertoire likely influences their ease of adaptation. Long-term learning causes lasting changes in neural connectivity, which shapes the activity patterns that can be produced during adaptation. Here, we examined how a neural population's existing activity patterns, acquired through de novo learning, affect subsequent adaptation by modeling motor cortical neural population dynamics with recurrent neural networks. We trained networks on different motor repertoires comprising varying numbers of movements, which they acquired following various learning experiences. Networks with multiple movements had more constrained and robust dynamics, which were associated with more defined neural 'structure'-organization in the available population activity patterns. This structure facilitated adaptation, but only when the changes imposed by the perturbation were congruent with the organization of the inputs and the structure in neural activity acquired during de novo learning. These results highlight trade-offs in skill acquisition and demonstrate how different learning experiences can shape the geometrical properties of neural population activity and subsequent adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joanna C Chang
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Matthew G Perich
- Département de Neurosciences, Faculté de Médecine, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
- Mila, Québec Artificial Intelligence Institute, Montréal, QC, Canada
| | - Lee E Miller
- Departments of Physiology, Biomedical Engineering and Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Northwestern University and Shirley Ryan Ability Lab, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Juan A Gallego
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK.
| | - Claudia Clopath
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Losey DM, Hennig JA, Oby ER, Golub MD, Sadtler PT, Quick KM, Ryu SI, Tyler-Kabara EC, Batista AP, Yu BM, Chase SM. Learning leaves a memory trace in motor cortex. Curr Biol 2024; 34:1519-1531.e4. [PMID: 38531360 PMCID: PMC11097210 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2024.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2023] [Revised: 12/06/2023] [Accepted: 03/04/2024] [Indexed: 03/28/2024]
Abstract
How are we able to learn new behaviors without disrupting previously learned ones? To understand how the brain achieves this, we used a brain-computer interface (BCI) learning paradigm, which enables us to detect the presence of a memory of one behavior while performing another. We found that learning to use a new BCI map altered the neural activity that monkeys produced when they returned to using a familiar BCI map in a way that was specific to the learning experience. That is, learning left a "memory trace" in the primary motor cortex. This memory trace coexisted with proficient performance under the familiar map, primarily by altering neural activity in dimensions that did not impact behavior. Forming memory traces might be how the brain is able to provide for the joint learning of multiple behaviors without interference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darby M Losey
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Machine Learning Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Jay A Hennig
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Machine Learning Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Emily R Oby
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Matthew D Golub
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA; Paul G. Allen School of Computer Science & Engineering, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
| | - Patrick T Sadtler
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Kristin M Quick
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Stephen I Ryu
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA; Department of Neurosurgery, Palo Alto Medical Foundation, Palo Alto, CA 94301, USA
| | - Elizabeth C Tyler-Kabara
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Neurosurgery, Dell Medical School, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Aaron P Batista
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| | - Byron M Yu
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| | - Steven M Chase
- Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Department of Biomedical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Weng G, Clark K, Akbarian A, Noudoost B, Nategh N. Time-varying generalized linear models: characterizing and decoding neuronal dynamics in higher visual areas. Front Comput Neurosci 2024; 18:1273053. [PMID: 38348287 PMCID: PMC10859875 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2024.1273053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2023] [Accepted: 01/09/2024] [Indexed: 02/15/2024] Open
Abstract
To create a behaviorally relevant representation of the visual world, neurons in higher visual areas exhibit dynamic response changes to account for the time-varying interactions between external (e.g., visual input) and internal (e.g., reward value) factors. The resulting high-dimensional representational space poses challenges for precisely quantifying individual factors' contributions to the representation and readout of sensory information during a behavior. The widely used point process generalized linear model (GLM) approach provides a powerful framework for a quantitative description of neuronal processing as a function of various sensory and non-sensory inputs (encoding) as well as linking particular response components to particular behaviors (decoding), at the level of single trials and individual neurons. However, most existing variations of GLMs assume the neural systems to be time-invariant, making them inadequate for modeling nonstationary characteristics of neuronal sensitivity in higher visual areas. In this review, we summarize some of the existing GLM variations, with a focus on time-varying extensions. We highlight their applications to understanding neural representations in higher visual areas and decoding transient neuronal sensitivity as well as linking physiology to behavior through manipulation of model components. This time-varying class of statistical models provide valuable insights into the neural basis of various visual behaviors in higher visual areas and hold significant potential for uncovering the fundamental computational principles that govern neuronal processing underlying various behaviors in different regions of the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Geyu Weng
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| | - Kelsey Clark
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| | - Amir Akbarian
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| | - Behrad Noudoost
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| | - Neda Nategh
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Safaie M, Chang JC, Park J, Miller LE, Dudman JT, Perich MG, Gallego JA. Preserved neural dynamics across animals performing similar behaviour. Nature 2023; 623:765-771. [PMID: 37938772 PMCID: PMC10665198 DOI: 10.1038/s41586-023-06714-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2022] [Accepted: 10/04/2023] [Indexed: 11/09/2023]
Abstract
Animals of the same species exhibit similar behaviours that are advantageously adapted to their body and environment. These behaviours are shaped at the species level by selection pressures over evolutionary timescales. Yet, it remains unclear how these common behavioural adaptations emerge from the idiosyncratic neural circuitry of each individual. The overall organization of neural circuits is preserved across individuals1 because of their common evolutionarily specified developmental programme2-4. Such organization at the circuit level may constrain neural activity5-8, leading to low-dimensional latent dynamics across the neural population9-11. Accordingly, here we suggested that the shared circuit-level constraints within a species would lead to suitably preserved latent dynamics across individuals. We analysed recordings of neural populations from monkey and mouse motor cortex to demonstrate that neural dynamics in individuals from the same species are surprisingly preserved when they perform similar behaviour. Neural population dynamics were also preserved when animals consciously planned future movements without overt behaviour12 and enabled the decoding of planned and ongoing movement across different individuals. Furthermore, we found that preserved neural dynamics extend beyond cortical regions to the dorsal striatum, an evolutionarily older structure13,14. Finally, we used neural network models to demonstrate that behavioural similarity is necessary but not sufficient for this preservation. We posit that these emergent dynamics result from evolutionary constraints on brain development and thus reflect fundamental properties of the neural basis of behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mostafa Safaie
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Joanna C Chang
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Junchol Park
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, TX, USA
| | - Lee E Miller
- Departments of Physiology, Biomedical Engineering and Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Northwestern University and Shirley Ryan Ability Lab, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Joshua T Dudman
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, TX, USA
| | - Matthew G Perich
- Département de Neurosciences, Faculté de Médecine, Université de Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
- Mila, Quebec Artificial Intelligence Institute, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
| | - Juan A Gallego
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Chang JC, Perich MG, Miller LE, Gallego JA, Clopath C. De novo motor learning creates structure in neural activity space that shapes adaptation. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.05.23.541925. [PMID: 37293081 PMCID: PMC10245862 DOI: 10.1101/2023.05.23.541925] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Animals can quickly adapt learned movements in response to external perturbations. Motor adaptation is likely influenced by an animal's existing movement repertoire, but the nature of this influence is unclear. Long-term learning causes lasting changes in neural connectivity which determine the activity patterns that can be produced. Here, we sought to understand how a neural population's activity repertoire, acquired through long-term learning, affects short-term adaptation by modeling motor cortical neural population dynamics during de novo learning and subsequent adaptation using recurrent neural networks. We trained these networks on different motor repertoires comprising varying numbers of movements. Networks with multiple movements had more constrained and robust dynamics, which were associated with more defined neural 'structure'-organization created by the neural population activity patterns corresponding to each movement. This structure facilitated adaptation, but only when small changes in motor output were required, and when the structure of the network inputs, the neural activity space, and the perturbation were congruent. These results highlight trade-offs in skill acquisition and demonstrate how prior experience and external cues during learning can shape the geometrical properties of neural population activity as well as subsequent adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joanna C. Chang
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Matthew G. Perich
- Département de neurosciences, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
| | - Lee E. Miller
- Department of Neuroscience, Northwestern University, USA
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Northwestern University, and Shirley Ryan Ability Lab, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Juan A. Gallego
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK
| | - Claudia Clopath
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
What is the nature of motor adaptation to dynamic perturbations? PLoS Comput Biol 2022; 18:e1010470. [PMID: 36040962 PMCID: PMC9467354 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010470] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2022] [Revised: 09/12/2022] [Accepted: 08/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
When human participants repeatedly encounter a velocity-dependent force field that distorts their movement trajectories, they adapt their motor behavior to recover straight trajectories. Computational models suggest that adaptation to a force field occurs at the action selection level through changes in the mapping between goals and actions. The quantitative prediction from these models indicates that early perturbed trajectories before adaptation and late unperturbed trajectories after adaptation should have opposite curvature, i.e. one being a mirror image of the other. We tested these predictions in a human adaptation experiment and we found that the expected mirror organization was either absent or much weaker than predicted by the models. These results are incompatible with adaptation occurring at the action selection level but compatible with adaptation occurring at the goal selection level, as if adaptation corresponds to aiming toward spatially remapped targets. Motor adaptation is a fundamental component of the acquisition and maintenance of skilled behaviors. Yet the nature of motor adaptation remains poorly understood: when we encounter forces which repeatedly perturb our movements, do we change our actions or our plans? Current computational models of motor control favor the former, but this assumption has not been thoroughly investigated. To address this issue, we compared predictions of a model of motor adaptation based on changes at the action level with observations obtained from a group of human participants involved in a motor adaptation task. The behavior of the participants clearly differed from the model’s predictions. These results challenge contemporary perspectives on motor adaptation.
Collapse
|
7
|
Forano M, Franklin DW. Timescales of motor memory formation in dual-adaptation. PLoS Comput Biol 2020; 16:e1008373. [PMID: 33075047 PMCID: PMC7595703 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008373] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2020] [Revised: 10/29/2020] [Accepted: 09/09/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The timescales of adaptation to novel dynamics are well explained by a dual-rate model with slow and fast states. This model can predict interference, savings and spontaneous recovery, but cannot account for adaptation to multiple tasks, as each new task drives unlearning of the previously learned task. Nevertheless, in the presence of appropriate contextual cues, humans are able to adapt simultaneously to opposing dynamics. Consequently this model was expanded, suggesting that dual-adaptation occurs through a single fast process and multiple slow processes. However, such a model does not predict spontaneous recovery within dual-adaptation. Here we assess the existence of multiple fast processes by examining the presence of spontaneous recovery in two experimental variations of an adaptation-de-adaptation-error-clamp paradigm within dual-task adaptation in humans. In both experiments, evidence for spontaneous recovery towards the initially learned dynamics (A) was found in the error-clamp phase, invalidating the one-fast-two-slow dual-rate model. However, as adaptation is not only constrained to two timescales, we fit twelve multi-rate models to the experimental data. BIC model comparison again supported the existence of two fast processes, but extended the timescales to include a third rate: the ultraslow process. Even within our single day experiment, we found little evidence for decay of the learned memory over several hundred error-clamp trials. Overall, we show that dual-adaptation can be best explained by a two-fast-triple-rate model over the timescales of adaptation studied here. Longer term learning may require even slower timescales, explaining why we never forget how to ride a bicycle. Retaining motor skills is crucial to perform basic daily life tasks. However we still have limited understanding of the computational structure of these motor memories, an understanding that is critical for designing rehabilitation. Here we demonstrate that learning any task involves adaptation of independent fast, slow and ultraslow processes to build a motor memory. The selection of the appropriate motor memory is gated through a contextual cue. Together this work extends our understanding of the architecture of motor memories, by merging disparate computational theories to propose a new model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marion Forano
- Neuromuscular Diagnostics, Department of Sport and Health Sciences, Technical University of Munich, Germany
| | - David W. Franklin
- Neuromuscular Diagnostics, Department of Sport and Health Sciences, Technical University of Munich, Germany
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Maintained Representations of the Ipsilateral and Contralateral Limbs during Bimanual Control in Primary Motor Cortex. J Neurosci 2020; 40:6732-6747. [PMID: 32703902 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0730-20.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2020] [Revised: 07/15/2020] [Accepted: 07/16/2020] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Primary motor cortex (M1) almost exclusively controls the contralateral side of the body. However, M1 activity is also modulated during ipsilateral body movements. Previous work has shown that M1 activity related to the ipsilateral arm is independent of the M1 activity related to the contralateral arm. How do these patterns of activity interact when both arms move simultaneously? We explored this problem by training 2 monkeys (male, Macaca mulatta) in a postural perturbation task while recording from M1. Loads were applied to one arm at a time (unimanual) or both arms simultaneously (bimanual). We found 83% of neurons (n = 236) were responsive to both the unimanual and bimanual loads. We also observed a small reduction in activity magnitude during the bimanual loads for both limbs (25%). Across the unimanual and bimanual loads, neurons largely maintained their preferred load directions. However, there was a larger change in the preferred loads for the ipsilateral limb (∼25%) than the contralateral limb (∼9%). Lastly, we identified the contralateral and ipsilateral subspaces during the unimanual loads and found they captured a significant amount of the variance during the bimanual loads. However, the subspace captured more of the bimanual variance related to the contralateral limb (97%) than the ipsilateral limb (66%). Our results highlight that, even during bimanual motor actions, M1 largely retains its representations of the contralateral and ipsilateral limbs.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Previous work has shown that primary motor cortex (M1) represents information related to the contralateral limb, its downstream target, but also reflects information related to the ipsilateral limb. Can M1 still represent both sources of information when performing simultaneous movements of the limbs? Here we record from M1 during a postural perturbation task. We show that activity related to the contralateral limb is maintained between unimanual and bimanual motor actions, whereas the activity related to the ipsilateral limb undergoes a small change between unimanual and bimanual motor actions. Our results indicate that two independent representations can be maintained and expressed simultaneously in M1.
Collapse
|
9
|
Gallego JA, Perich MG, Chowdhury RH, Solla SA, Miller LE. Long-term stability of cortical population dynamics underlying consistent behavior. Nat Neurosci 2020; 23:260-270. [PMID: 31907438 PMCID: PMC7007364 DOI: 10.1038/s41593-019-0555-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 156] [Impact Index Per Article: 31.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2018] [Accepted: 11/11/2019] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Animals readily execute learned behaviors in a consistent manner over long periods of time, and yet no equally stable neural correlate has been demonstrated. How does the cortex achieve this stable control? Using the sensorimotor system as a model of cortical processing, we investigated the hypothesis that the dynamics of neural latent activity, which captures the dominant co-variation patterns within the neural population, must be preserved across time. We recorded from populations of neurons in premotor, primary motor and somatosensory cortices as monkeys performed a reaching task, for up to 2 years. Intriguingly, despite a steady turnover in the recorded neurons, the low-dimensional latent dynamics remained stable. The stability allowed reliable decoding of behavioral features for the entire timespan, while fixed decoders based directly on the recorded neural activity degraded substantially. We posit that stable latent cortical dynamics within the manifold are the fundamental building blocks underlying consistent behavioral execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan A Gallego
- Neural and Cognitive Engineering Group, Center for Automation and Robotics, Spanish National Research Council, Arganda del Rey, Spain.
- Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA.
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, London, UK.
| | - Matthew G Perich
- Department of Fundamental Neuroscience, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Raeed H Chowdhury
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
| | - Sara A Solla
- Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
- Department of Physics and Astronomy, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
| | - Lee E Miller
- Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA.
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA.
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Northwestern University, and Shirley Ryan Ability Lab, Chicago, IL, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Pandarinath C, Ames KC, Russo AA, Farshchian A, Miller LE, Dyer EL, Kao JC. Latent Factors and Dynamics in Motor Cortex and Their Application to Brain-Machine Interfaces. J Neurosci 2018; 38:9390-9401. [PMID: 30381431 PMCID: PMC6209846 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1669-18.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2018] [Revised: 09/24/2018] [Accepted: 09/25/2018] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
In the 1960s, Evarts first recorded the activity of single neurons in motor cortex of behaving monkeys (Evarts, 1968). In the 50 years since, great effort has been devoted to understanding how single neuron activity relates to movement. Yet these single neurons exist within a vast network, the nature of which has been largely inaccessible. With advances in recording technologies, algorithms, and computational power, the ability to study these networks is increasing exponentially. Recent experimental results suggest that the dynamical properties of these networks are critical to movement planning and execution. Here we discuss this dynamical systems perspective and how it is reshaping our understanding of the motor cortices. Following an overview of key studies in motor cortex, we discuss techniques to uncover the "latent factors" underlying observed neural population activity. Finally, we discuss efforts to use these factors to improve the performance of brain-machine interfaces, promising to make these findings broadly relevant to neuroengineering as well as systems neuroscience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chethan Pandarinath
- Wallace H. Coulter Department of Biomedical Engineering, Emory University and Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30322,
- Department of Neurosurgery, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322
| | - K Cora Ames
- Department of Neuroscience
- Center for Theoretical Neuroscience
- Grossman Center for the Statistics of Mind
- Zuckerman Institute, Columbia University, New York, New York 10027
| | - Abigail A Russo
- Department of Neuroscience
- Grossman Center for the Statistics of Mind
- Zuckerman Institute, Columbia University, New York, New York 10027
| | - Ali Farshchian
- Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611
| | - Lee E Miller
- Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611
| | - Eva L Dyer
- Wallace H. Coulter Department of Biomedical Engineering, Emory University and Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30322
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332
| | - Jonathan C Kao
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, and
- Neurosciences Program, University of California, Los Angeles, California 90095
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Perich MG, Gallego JA, Miller LE. A Neural Population Mechanism for Rapid Learning. Neuron 2018; 100:964-976.e7. [PMID: 30344047 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 90] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2018] [Revised: 08/16/2018] [Accepted: 09/21/2018] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Long-term learning of language, mathematics, and motor skills likely requires cortical plasticity, but behavior often requires much faster changes, sometimes even after single errors. Here, we propose one neural mechanism to rapidly develop new motor output without altering the functional connectivity within or between cortical areas. We tested cortico-cortical models relating the activity of hundreds of neurons in the premotor (PMd) and primary motor (M1) cortices throughout adaptation to reaching movement perturbations. We found a signature of learning in the "output-null" subspace of PMd with respect to M1 reflecting the ability of premotor cortex to alter preparatory activity without directly influencing M1. The output-null subspace planning activity evolved with adaptation, yet the "output-potent" mapping that captures information sent to M1 was preserved. Our results illustrate a population-level cortical mechanism to progressively adjust the output from one brain area to its downstream structures that could be exploited for rapid behavioral adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew G Perich
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA
| | - Juan A Gallego
- Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA; Neural and Cognitive Engineering Group, Centre for Automation and Robotics, CSIC-UPM, 28500 Arganda del Rey, Madrid, Spain
| | - Lee E Miller
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA; Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA; Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL 60611, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Gallego JA, Perich MG, Naufel SN, Ethier C, Solla SA, Miller LE. Cortical population activity within a preserved neural manifold underlies multiple motor behaviors. Nat Commun 2018; 9:4233. [PMID: 30315158 PMCID: PMC6185944 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-018-06560-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 142] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2017] [Accepted: 09/12/2018] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
Populations of cortical neurons flexibly perform different functions; for the primary motor cortex (M1) this means a rich repertoire of motor behaviors. We investigate the flexibility of M1 movement control by analyzing neural population activity during a variety of skilled wrist and reach-to-grasp tasks. We compare across tasks the neural modes that capture dominant neural covariance patterns during each task. While each task requires different patterns of muscle and single unit activity, we find unexpected similarities at the neural population level: the structure and activity of the neural modes is largely preserved across tasks. Furthermore, we find two sets of neural modes with task-independent activity that capture, respectively, generic temporal features of the set of tasks and a task-independent mapping onto muscle activity. This system of flexibly combined, well-preserved neural modes may underlie the ability of M1 to learn and generate a wide-ranging behavioral repertoire.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan A Gallego
- Department of Physiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, 303 E. Chicago Avenue, Chicago, IL, 60611, USA.
- Neural and Cognitive Engineering Group, Centre for Automation and Robotics CSIC-UPM, Ctra. Campo Real km 0.2 - La Poveda, 28500, Arganda del Rey, Spain.
| | - Matthew G Perich
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, 2145 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA
| | - Stephanie N Naufel
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, 2145 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA
| | - Christian Ethier
- Département de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Université Laval, CERVO Research Center, 2601 Ch. de la Canardière, Québec, QC, G1J 2G3, Canada
| | - Sara A Solla
- Department of Physiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, 303 E. Chicago Avenue, Chicago, IL, 60611, USA
- Department of Physics and Astronomy, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA
| | - Lee E Miller
- Department of Physiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, 303 E. Chicago Avenue, Chicago, IL, 60611, USA.
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, 2145 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA.
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, 60611, USA.
| |
Collapse
|