1
|
Tanyas H, Liss JV, Kuhlmann BG. Information accumulation on the item versus source test of source monitoring: Insights from diffusion modeling. Mem Cognit 2024:10.3758/s13421-024-01636-2. [PMID: 39363118 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01636-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/28/2024] [Indexed: 10/05/2024]
Abstract
Source monitoring involves attributing previous experiences (e.g., studied words as items) to their origins (e.g., screen positions as sources). The present study aimed toward a better understanding of temporal aspects of item and source processing. Participants made source decisions for recognized items either in succession (i.e., the standard format) or in separate test blocks providing independent measures of item and source decision speed. Comparable speeds of item and source decision across the test formats would suggest a full separation between item and source processing, whereas different speeds would imply their (partial) temporal overlap. To test these alternatives, we used the drift rate parameter of the diffusion model (Ratcliff, Psychological Review, 85, 59-108, 1978). We examined whether the drift rates, together with the other parameters, assessed separately for the item and source decision varied as a function of the test format. Threshold separation and nondecision time differed between the test formats, but item and source decision speeds represented by drift rates did not change significantly. Thus, despite facilitation on the source decision when the item decision was immediately followed by a test for source memory than when item and source were tested in separate blocks, findings did not suggest that source information already begins accumulating in the item test in the standard format. We discuss the temporal sequence of item and source processing in light of different assumptions about the contribution of familiarity and recollection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilal Tanyas
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany.
| | - Julia V Liss
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
| | - Beatrice G Kuhlmann
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Li C, Otgaar H, Muris P, Chen C. Retracted memories in the general population: are there differences between eastern and western countries? Memory 2024; 32:396-409. [PMID: 38466609 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2327108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2022] [Accepted: 02/26/2024] [Indexed: 03/13/2024]
Abstract
The main purpose of the current studies was to examine retracted experiences in the general population from various cultural backgrounds. More specifically, in two studies, we examined the details of memory retraction experiences, the reasons for retraction, and the outcomes of retraction in participants from China and other countries, mainly the United States of America. It was found that memory retraction experiences appeared to be quite common. In the sample of Chinese participants (Study 1: N = 1380), 50.58% reported at least one such an experience, whereas in respondents from other countries (Study 2; N = 425), a significantly lower but still substantial prevalence rate of 35% was found. In general, the retracted memories predominantly involved positive events and some respondents experienced pressure during the withdrawal. Social feedback and event plausibility were the two main reasons for the withdrawal. Compared to recollection scores, belief scores decreased significantly after withdrawal, and some respondents even formed nonbelieved memories. After retracting the memories, most respondents felt they gained benefits (e.g., they had resolved a psychological problem that had bothered them for years). These studies give us a more general understanding of retracted memory experiences in the general population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chunlin Li
- Faculty of Law and Criminology, Catholic University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Henry Otgaar
- Faculty of Law and Criminology, Catholic University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Peter Muris
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
- Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
| | - Cui Chen
- The Third Primary School in Tongjiang, Bazhong, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sharman SJ, Danby MC, Gray AD. Witnesses' susceptibility to misleading post-event information delivered in a social media-style video. Memory 2024; 32:100-110. [PMID: 38146968 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2023.2294692] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2023] [Accepted: 12/04/2023] [Indexed: 12/27/2023]
Abstract
In many criminal cases, outcomes rely on eyewitness evidence. Exposure to misleading information after an event reduces the accuracy of witnesses' memories. In some circumstances, warnings about misinformation can protect witnesses. As social media is a growing source of misleading information, this study examined the effect of misleading post-event information delivered via a social media-style video, as well as the utility of a minimal versus detailed warning. Participants (N = 145) watched a video showing an electrician stealing items from a client's home. Next, they received one of three pre-warnings regarding forthcoming misleading information: a minimal warning indicating that caution should be taken, a detailed warning specifying the presence of misleading information, or no warning. Participants received the misleading information via a social media video or a standard text-based narrative. Finally, they completed a recognition test. Although delivery method did not affect errors for misleading items, detailed warnings were only effective against text-based misleading information. Participants were more confident about their correct than incorrect responses for misleading items; confidence was not affected by delivery method or warning. This experiment is the first to demonstrate people's susceptibility to misleading post-event information delivered in a social media-style video using an eyewitness paradigm.
Collapse
|
4
|
Rollins L, Huffman DJ, Walters LA, Bennett K. Prolonged development of forced-choice recognition when targets are paired with non-corresponding lures. J Exp Child Psychol 2023; 236:105742. [PMID: 37481987 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105742] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2023] [Revised: 04/28/2023] [Accepted: 06/26/2023] [Indexed: 07/25/2023]
Abstract
Previous research suggests that mnemonic discrimination (i.e., the ability to discriminate between previously encountered and novel stimuli even when they are highly similar) improves substantially during childhood. To further understand the development of mnemonic discrimination during childhood, the current study had 4-year-old children, 6-year-old children, and young adults complete the forced-choice Mnemonic Similarity Task (MST). The forced-choice MST offers a significant advantage in the context of developmental research because it is not sensitive to age-related differences in response criteria and includes three test formats that are theorized to be supported by different cognitive processes. A target (i.e., a previously encountered item) is paired with either a novel item (A-X), a corresponding lure (A-A'; i.e., an item mnemonically similar to the target), or a non-corresponding lure (A-B'; i.e., an item mnemonically similar to a different previously encoded item). We observed that 4-year-olds performed more poorly than 6-year-olds on the A-X and A-A' test formats, whereas both 4- and 6-year-olds performed more poorly than young adults on the A-B' test format. The MINERVA 2.2 computational model effectively accounted for these age-related differences. The model suggested that 4-year-olds have a lower learning rate (i.e., probability of encoding stimulus features) than 6-year-olds and young adults and that both 4- and 6-year-olds have greater encoding variability than young adults. These findings provide new insight into possible mechanisms underlying memory development during childhood and serve as the basis for multiple avenues of future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leslie Rollins
- Department of Psychology, Christopher Newport University, Newport News, VA 23606, USA.
| | - Derek J Huffman
- Department of Psychology, Colby College, Waterville, ME 04901, USA
| | - Lauren A Walters
- Department of Psychology, Christopher Newport University, Newport News, VA 23606, USA
| | - Kaylee Bennett
- Department of Psychology, Christopher Newport University, Newport News, VA 23606, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Danby MC, Sharman SJ, van Golde C, Paterson H. Laypeople's perceptions of the effects of event repetition, reporting delay, and emotion on children's and adults' memory. Memory 2023; 31:205-217. [PMID: 36259521 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2022.2135737] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
For crimes such as child abuse and family violence, jurors' assessments of memory reports from key witnesses are vital to case outcomes in court. Since jurors are not experts on memory, the present research measured laypeople's (i.e., non-experts') beliefs about how three key factors affect witnesses' memory reports for an experienced event: how frequently an event was experienced (repeated, single), the delay between experiencing and reporting the event, and the emotional valence of the event. Across two studies, lay participants completed an online survey that measured their beliefs about each factor. In Study 1, 51 participants completed a survey about how the three factors affect children's memory. In Study 2, another 51 participants completed a survey about how the three factors affect adult's memory. Across both studies, delays were believed to worsen memory, and emotion was believed to improve memory. Beliefs about single and repeated events showed different patterns across the studies. In Study 1, participants' beliefs about children's memory for repeated experience were variable. In Study 2, participants believed that adults' memory was worse for repeated events than single events. Overall, laypeople demonstrated many accurate beliefs about memory, but showed some confusion about children's memory for repeated events.
Collapse
|
6
|
Meyer KR, Blades M, Krähenbühl S. The Gestural Misinformation Effect in Child Interviews in Switzerland. JOURNAL OF NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR 2023. [DOI: 10.1007/s10919-022-00419-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
AbstractGestures embody concepts in the form of universal representations. Researchers have highlighted that social communication often embodies nonverbal behavior. A forensic interviewer’s nonverbal behavior, such as gesturing during an interview, could communicate misleading information and may cause inaccuracies in the interviewees’ testimonies. The current study was conducted in Switzerland and included 108 child participants, in three age groups (a younger sample aged 6–9 years, n = 32) (a middle sample aged 10–11 years, n = 40) and an older sample aged 12–13 years, n = 36). Participants viewed a video and completed an interview about the video, individually, immediately after. During the questioning, the interviewer deliberately misled the interviewees with nonverbal gestures. The results showed that 95 children were misled by at least one gesture and that gestures led to a significant decrease in accuracy. Children also incorporated misleading gestures and reported false information; adding to existing evidence that misinformation can also be communicated through nonverbal gestures. Our findings demonstrate the negative influence of misleading gestures in child eyewitness interviews and provide more evidence for the robustness of the gestural misinformation effect, reported in previous research.
Collapse
|
7
|
Hoffman LJ, Ngo CT, Canada KL, Pasternak O, Zhang F, Riggins T, Olson IR. The fornix supports episodic memory during childhood. Cereb Cortex 2022; 32:5388-5403. [PMID: 35169831 PMCID: PMC9712741 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhac022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2021] [Revised: 01/12/2022] [Accepted: 01/13/2022] [Indexed: 12/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Episodic memory relies on the coordination of widespread brain regions that reconstruct spatiotemporal details of an episode. These topologically dispersed brain regions can rapidly communicate through structural pathways. Research in animal and human lesion studies implicate the fornix-the major output pathway of the hippocampus-in supporting various aspects of episodic memory. Because episodic memory undergoes marked changes in early childhood, we tested the link between the fornix and episodic memory in an age window of robust memory development (ages 4-8 years). Children were tested on the stories subtest from the Children's Memory Scale, a temporal order memory task, and a source memory task. Fornix streamlines were reconstructed using probabilistic tractography to estimate fornix microstructure. In addition, we measured fornix macrostructure and computed free water. To assess selectivity of our findings, we also reconstructed the uncinate fasciculus. Findings show that children's memory increases from ages 4 to 8 and that fornix micro- and macrostructure increases between ages 4 and 8. Children's memory performance across nearly every memory task correlated with individual differences in fornix, but not uncinate fasciculus, white matter. These findings suggest that the fornix plays an important role in supporting the development of episodic memory, and potentially semantic memory, in early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Linda J Hoffman
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, 1701 North 13th St., Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| | - Chi T Ngo
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Kelsey L Canada
- Institute of Gerontology, Wayne State University, 87 East Ferry St., Detroit, MI 48202, USA
| | - Ofer Pasternak
- Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 75 Francis St., Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 75 Francis St., Boston MA 02115, USA
| | - Fan Zhang
- Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 75 Francis St., Boston MA 02115, USA
| | - Tracy Riggins
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, 4094 Campus Dr., College Park, MD, 20742, USA
| | - Ingrid R Olson
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, 1701 North 13th St., Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
An overview of source monitoring theory and research regarding children’s training. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-022-03730-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
9
|
Sharman SJ, Danby MC, Christopoulos L. Mental context reinstatement improves adults’ reports of additional details from two instances of a repeated event. Memory 2022; 30:988-999. [DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2022.2068610] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
10
|
Moore KN, Lampinen JM, Nesmith BL, Bridges AJ, Gallo DA. The effect of feedback and recollection rejection instructions on the development of memory monitoring and accuracy. J Exp Child Psychol 2022; 221:105434. [PMID: 35489136 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105434] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2021] [Revised: 01/11/2022] [Accepted: 03/16/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Recollection rejection (a form of memory monitoring) involves rejecting false details on the basis of remembering true details (recall to reject), thereby increasing memory accuracy. This study examined how recollection rejection instructions and feedback affect memory accuracy and false recognition in 5-year-olds, 6- and 7-year-olds, 8- and 9-year-olds, and adults. Participants (N = 336) completed three study-test phases. Instructions and item-level feedback were manipulated during the first two phases, with the third phase including a test containing no instructions or feedback to evaluate learning effects. As predicted, in the younger children, as compared with the older children and adults, we found reduced accuracy scores (hits to studied items minus false alarms to related lures), reduced recollection rejection to related lures, and increased false recognition scores. We also found that, in the third phase, prior feedback reduced false recognition scores, potentially by improving monitoring, and typical developmental differences in false recognition were eliminated. However, there were mixed findings of instructions and feedback, and in some conditions these interventions harmed memory. These findings provide initial evidence that combining instructions and feedback with repeated task practice may improve monitoring effectiveness, but additional work is needed on how these factors improve and sometimes harm performance in young children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kara N Moore
- Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74074, USA.
| | | | - Blake L Nesmith
- Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74074, USA
| | - Ana J Bridges
- Department of Psychology, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR 72701, USA
| | - David A Gallo
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
The effects of recalling generic versus episodic information first on adults' reports of a repeated event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3924] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
12
|
Nicoladis E, Svob C, Smithson L. What is the source of preschool children's memories of events from their own lives? APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
13
|
Dolgoarshinnaia A, Martin-Luengo B. False Memories in Native and Foreign Languages. Front Psychol 2021; 12:716336. [PMID: 34650480 PMCID: PMC8505984 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.716336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2021] [Accepted: 08/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Human memory is prone to memory errors and distortion. Evidence from studies on cognitive functions in bilinguals indicates that they might be prone to different types of memory errors compared to monolinguals; however, the effect of language in false memories is still understudied. Source monitoring processes required for proper memory functioning, presumably, rely on inhibitory control, which is also heavily utilized by bilinguals. Moreover, it is suggested that thinking in a second language leads to more systematic and deliberate reasoning. All these results lead to expect that bilinguals are more analytical when processing information in their second language overcoming some memory errors depending on the language of information. To test this hypothesis, we run a classical misinformation experiment with an explicit source monitoring task with a sample of Russian–English bilinguals. The language of the misinformation presentation did not affect the degree of the misinformation effect between the Russian and English languages. Source monitoring demonstrated an overall higher accuracy for attributions to the English source over the Russian source. Furthermore, analysis on incorrect source attributions showed that when participants misattributed the sources of false information (English or Russian narrative), they favored the Russian source over the not presented condition. Taken together, these results imply that high proficiency in the second language does not affect misinformation and that information processing and memory monitoring in bilinguals can differ depending on the language of the information, which seems to lead to some memory errors and not others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aleksandra Dolgoarshinnaia
- Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Beatriz Martin-Luengo
- Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Simons JS, Ritchey M, Fernyhough C. Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Subjective Experience of Remembering. Annu Rev Psychol 2021; 73:159-186. [PMID: 34587777 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-030221-025439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
The ability to remember events in vivid, multisensory detail is a significant part of human experience, allowing us to relive previous encounters and providing us with the store of memories that shape our identity. Recent research has sought to understand the subjective experience of remembering, that is, what it feels like to have a memory. Such remembering involves reactivating sensory-perceptual features of an event and the thoughts and feelings we had when the event occurred, integrating them into a conscious first-person experience. It allows us to reflect on the content of our memories and to understand and make judgments about them, such as distinguishing events that actually occurred from those we might have imagined or been told about. In this review, we consider recent evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and studies of neurological and psychiatric conditions, which is shedding new light on how we subjectively experience remembering. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology, Volume 73 is January 2022. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jon S Simons
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom;
| | - Maureen Ritchey
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, Massachusetts 02467, USA
| | - Charles Fernyhough
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Dautriche I, Goupil L, Smith K, Rabagliati H. Knowing How You Know: Toddlers Reevaluate Words Learned From an Unreliable Speaker. Open Mind (Camb) 2021; 5:1-19. [PMID: 34485794 PMCID: PMC8412199 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2020] [Accepted: 11/25/2020] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
Abstract
There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Isabelle Dautriche
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Aix-Marseille University, CNRS, Marseille, France
| | - Louise Goupil
- School of Psychology, University of East London, London, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Marble KE, Caporaso JS, Bettencourt KM, Boseovski JJ, Pathman T, Marcovitch S, Scales ML. Children's Informant Judgments and Recall of Valenced Facts at a Science Center. Front Psychol 2021; 12:659633. [PMID: 34220631 PMCID: PMC8242232 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659633] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2021] [Accepted: 05/18/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In laboratory-based research, children recognize who is an expert and demonstrate an interest in learning from that person. However, children prefer positive information in the moment and sometimes prioritize positivity over expertise. To what extent do these social judgments (e.g., a preference for positivity) relate to information that children remember? We investigated the relation between these judgments and memory at a local science center to better understand children's learning outcomes in naturalistic settings. We examined the extent to which 4- to 8-year-olds accepted facts about an unfamiliar animal from a zookeeper informant (i.e., expert) and a maternal figure (i.e., non-expert) when these facts were positive, negative, or neutral. Children endorsed positive information as correct, regardless of expertise, but demonstrated the strongest memory for neutral information. We discuss the implications of this dissociation for learning outcomes in naturalistic contexts as well as theoretical frameworks regarding children's learning from others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly E Marble
- Development and Understanding of Children's Knowledge Lab, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, United States
| | - Jessica S Caporaso
- Development and Understanding of Children's Knowledge Lab, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, United States
| | - Kathleen M Bettencourt
- Development and Understanding of Children's Knowledge Lab, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, United States
| | - Janet J Boseovski
- Development and Understanding of Children's Knowledge Lab, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, United States
| | - Thanujeni Pathman
- Memory Development Lab, Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Stuart Marcovitch
- Development and Understanding of Children's Knowledge Lab, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, United States
| | - Margaret L Scales
- Department of Social Sciences and Health Policy, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC, United States
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Roberts KP, Wood KR, Wylie BE. Children's ability to edit their memories when learning about the environment from credible and noncredible websites. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2021; 6:42. [PMID: 34050824 PMCID: PMC8164076 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-021-00305-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2020] [Accepted: 05/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
One of the many sources of information easily available to children is the internet and the millions of websites providing accurate, and sometimes inaccurate, information. In the current investigation, we examined children’s ability to use credibility information about websites when learning about environmental sustainability. In two studies, children studied two different websites and were tested on what they had learned a week later using a multiple-choice test containing both website items and new distracters. Children were given either no information about the websites or were told that one of the websites (the noncredible website) contained errors and they should not use any information from that website to answer the test. In both studies, children aged 7- to 9-years reported information from the noncredible website even when instructed not to, whereas the 10- to 12-year-olds used the credibility warning to ‘edit out’ information that they had learned from the noncredible website. In Study 2, there was an indication that the older children spontaneously assessed the credibility of the website if credibility markers were made explicit. A plausible explanation is that, although children remembered information from the websites, they needed explicit instruction to bind the website content with the relevant source (the individual websites). The results have implications for children’s learning in an open-access, digital age where information comes from many sources, credible and noncredible. Education in credibility evaluation may enable children to be critical consumers of information thereby resisting misinformation provided through public sources.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kim P Roberts
- Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Ave W, Waterloo, ON, N2L 3C5, Canada.
| | - Katherine R Wood
- Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Ave W, Waterloo, ON, N2L 3C5, Canada.,Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Dr, Ottawa, ON, K1S 5B6, Canada
| | - Breanne E Wylie
- Wilfrid Laurier University, 75 University Ave W, Waterloo, ON, N2L 3C5, Canada.,Brock University, 1812 Sir Isaac Brock Way, St. Catharines, ON, L2S 3A1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abstract
Although false memories have largely been examined with the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm, little research has focused on the semantic context in which associates are encoded. Across three experiments, we varied semantic context during a sentence-processing task with DRM associates embedded within sentences. More meaningful sentences resulted in greater memory errors (Experiment 1). Furthermore, providing contextual information to discriminate old from new items did not reduce false alarms relative to encoding words in isolation when sentences converged on the meaning of the critical lure (Experiment 2), and actually increased memory errors (Experiment 3). These results suggest that semantic context that allows for meaningful relational processing of items within-lists and that converge on the semantic meaning of the critical lure increases the likelihood that the list theme is identified, resulting in more errors at test.
Collapse
|
19
|
Huang TST, Shanks DR. Examining the relationship between processing fluency and memory for source information. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2021; 8:190430. [PMID: 33996111 PMCID: PMC8059628 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.190430] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2020] [Accepted: 03/29/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Familiarity-based processes such as processing fluency can influence memory judgements in tests of item recognition. Many conventional accounts of source memory assume minimal influence of familiarity on source memory, but recent work has suggested that source memory judgements are affected when test stimuli are processed with greater fluency as a result of priming. The present experiments investigated the relationship between fluency and the accuracy of source memory decisions. Participants studied words presented with different source attributes. During test, they identified words that gradually clarified on screen through progressive demasking, made old/new and source memory judgements, and reported confidence ratings for those words. Response times (RTs) recorded from the item identification task formed the basis of a fluency measure, and identification RTs were compared across categories of item recognition, source accuracy and confidence. Identification RTs were faster in trials with correct retrieval of source information compared with trials for which source could not be accurately retrieved. These findings are consistent with the assumption that familiarity-based processes are related to source memory judgements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tina S.-T. Huang
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, UK
| | - David R. Shanks
- Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Mazerolle M, Smith AM, Torrance M, Thomas AK. Understanding Older Adults' Memory Distortion in the Light of Stereotype Threat. Front Psychol 2021; 12:628696. [PMID: 33776850 PMCID: PMC7987663 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.628696] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2020] [Accepted: 02/15/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Numerous studies have documented the detrimental impact of age-based stereotype threat (ABST) on older adults' cognitive performance and especially on veridical memory. However, far fewer studies have investigated the impact of ABST on older adults' memory distortion. Here, we review the subset of research examining memory distortion and provide evidence for the role of stereotype threat as a powerful socio-emotional factor that impacts age-related susceptibility to memory distortion. In this review we define memory distortion as errors in memory that are associated with gist-based errors or source misattributions. Whereas, some of the reviewed experiments support the conclusion that ABST should be considered in the context of age-related differences in memory distortion, others reported little or no impact of stereotype threat. These discrepancies suggest that the role of ABST, and socio-emotional processes generally, in age-related changes in memory distortion are less clear. In this review, we argue that ABST does play an important role in age-related changes in memory distortion. We present evidence suggesting that discrepancies in the reviewed literature may be reconciled when evaluated in the context of the leading theories about stereotype threat: the Executive Resource Depletion hypothesis and the Regulatory Focus theory. We also discuss how differences in methodology and participant characteristics can account for a priori contradictory results in the literature. Finally, we propose some recommendations for researchers and practitioners when assessing memory in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie Mazerolle
- Laboratoire de Recherches Intégratives en Neurosciences et Psychologie Cognitive & MSHE Ledoux, CNRS, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Amy M Smith
- Department of Psychology, Quinnipiac University, Hamden, CT, United States
| | - McKinzey Torrance
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, MA, United States
| | - Ayanna K Thomas
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
While research suggests that telling lies can distort memory for the truth, the effects of receiving feedback about the believability about one's lies are not known. We hypothesised that participants who exaggerated the number of stolen items (vs. told the truth) in a mock insurance claim and then received feedback that their statement was believable (vs. not believable) would incorporate this lie into memory. In a two-phase between-subjects design, participants saw images of an office pre- and post-theft and were randomly assigned to report the number of stolen items accurately (truth-tellers) or exaggerate (liars). They were then randomly assigned to receive feedback that their statement was believable or not. Approximately two weeks later, participants' recall of the theft was measured. Liars and truth-tellers recalled a similar number of stolen items, but liars had less accurate recall for exactly which items were stolen. Liars (vs. truth-tellers) also made more omission errors. The majority of participants who fabricated additional stolen items (65.7%) incorporated one or more of these (false) items into their memory. Results suggest that source monitoring errors may lead fabricated details to be incorporated into memory. Although no effects of believability feedback were observed, recommendations for future researchers are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Leanne Ten Brinke
- Psychology, University of British Columbia Okanagan, Kelowna, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Dilevski N, Paterson HM, Walker SA, van Golde C. Adult memory for specific instances of a repeated event: a preliminary review. PSYCHIATRY, PSYCHOLOGY, AND LAW : AN INTERDISCIPLINARY JOURNAL OF THE AUSTRALIAN AND NEW ZEALAND ASSOCIATION OF PSYCHIATRY, PSYCHOLOGY AND LAW 2020; 28:711-732. [PMID: 35571600 PMCID: PMC9103626 DOI: 10.1080/13218719.2020.1837031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
In cases of repeated victimisation, a complainant's statement of abuse, and therefore memory, is often critical evidence for forensic investigations and legal proceedings. It is therefore important to understand the functioning of adults' memory for repeated events. As such, the purpose of this paper was to review the extant literature on adult memory for instances of a repeated event. The results of the review revealed a small number of heterogeneous studies on adult repeated-event memory (N = 12). The literature so far shows that while adults might have difficulty in recalling information specific to instances (narrow accuracy), they are capable of remembering information across multiple instances (broad accuracy). It was also found that several factors may impact recall of instances including age, the number of experienced instances, rehearsing an event, repeated retrieval and event distinctiveness. The discussion highlights the forensic implications of this research and future research directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natali Dilevski
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | | | - Sarah A. Walker
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Celine van Golde
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Morgan EE, Watson CWM, Woods SP, Gilbert PE, Villalobos J, Verduzco M. Misattributions of the source of health-related information in HIV disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2020; 43:1-14. [PMID: 33302782 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2020.1851355] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Introduction: Growing access to both legitimate and dubious sources of health information makes accurate source memory increasingly important, yet it may be negatively impacted by conditions that impair prefrontal functioning, including HIV. This study hypothesized that instructions supporting source encoding on a health-related memory task would disproportionately benefit source memory of people with HIV (PWH), and to examine the pattern of source memory errors that are observed.Method: 102 individuals (61 HIV+, 41 HIV-) completed comprehensive neurobehavioral (including health literacy) and neuromedical evaluations, and were randomly assigned to one of two conditions for a health-related memory task: Attend to Source Instructions explicitly participants to attend to the source of health statements presented to them, which were either health professionals or lay-persons, whereas no such instruction was provided in a Control Instructions condition.Results: There was no significant interaction of HIV status by condition or main effect of HIV (ps>.05). There was a main effect of condition whereby those who received Attend to Source Instructions performed better on item-corrected source memory than those in the Control Instructions condition (p =.04). Those who received Control Instructions were more likely to misattribute the source of the health information to a health professional when the correct source was a lay-person (Cohen's d = -0.53), which was correlated with poorer overall cognitive performance (p =.008) and performance-based measures of health literacy (ps<.05).Conclusions: Given that people are rarely reminded to attend to the source of new health information in the real world, the risk for misattributing health information to a qualified health professional in the absence of such instructions raises the concern that people may readily incorporate questionable health recommendations into their health regimen, particularly among persons with poorer cognitive functioning and lower levels of health literacy. This may have significant downstream health consequences such as drug interactions, side effects, and inefficacy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin E Morgan
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Caitlin Wei-Ming Watson
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA.,SDSU/UCSD Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology
| | | | - Paul E Gilbert
- Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Javier Villalobos
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Marizela Verduzco
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Merkt M, Huff M. Does the position of source information for multiple documents matter? Insights from two experiments. CONTEMPORARY EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101900] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
25
|
Moore KN, Lampinen JM, Bridges AJ, Gallo DA. Developmental trends in children’s use of different monitoring processes to avoid false memories. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
26
|
James BT, Webster MF, Menzel CR, Whitham W, Beran MJ. Post-event misinformation effects in a language-trained chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 2020; 23:861-869. [PMID: 32388782 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-020-01391-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2020] [Revised: 04/20/2020] [Accepted: 04/28/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Errors of source monitoring are widespread human memory challenges, and our memories are subject to distortion upon the presentation of subsequent misinformation. Less is known about if and when misinformation effects occur in nonhuman species' memory. Here we tested a symbol-trained chimpanzee's recall memory of a hidden food item's identity after a 10-min delay. During this delay, the subject was sometimes (depending on the condition) shown consistent or inconsistent video information about the identity of the food, before being asked to name the item to a second experimenter blind to the reward and condition. Across all conditions, our subject, Sherman, correctly named the food item at above chance levels. In the Inconsistent condition, in which Sherman was shown a video with misleading information, his performance was the worst of all conditions (although accuracy was still high). Interestingly, however, during three of the four trials in this condition in which Sherman made a mistake, he incorrectly named the food item shown during the misleading video information. These results provide evidence that chimpanzees, like humans, may be vulnerable to misinformation effects, even when that misleading information is presented in a different modality (video) than the original live event memory, demonstrating further commonality between human and ape memory systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brielle T James
- Language Research Center, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA. .,Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA.
| | - Mackenzie F Webster
- Language Research Center, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA.,Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Charles R Menzel
- Language Research Center, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Will Whitham
- Language Research Center, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA.,Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Michael J Beran
- Language Research Center, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA. .,Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Liverani MC, Freitas LGA, Siffredi V, Mikneviciute G, Martuzzi R, Meskaldij D, Borradori Tolsa C, Ha‐Vinh Leuchter R, Schnider A, Van De Ville D, Hüppi PS. Get real: Orbitofrontal cortex mediates the ability to sense reality in early adolescents. Brain Behav 2020; 10:e01552. [PMID: 32073744 PMCID: PMC7177588 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.1552] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2019] [Revised: 01/06/2020] [Accepted: 01/08/2020] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Orbitofrontal reality filtering (ORFi) is a memory mechanism that distinguishes whether a thought is relevant to present reality or not. In adults, it is mediated by the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). This region is still not fully developed in preteenagers, but ORFi is already active from age 7. Here, we probe the neural correlates of ORFi in early adolescents, hypothesizing that OFC mediates the sense of reality in this population. METHODS Functional magnetic resonance images (fMRI) were acquired in 22 early adolescents during a task composed of two runs: run 1 measuring recognition capacity; run 2 measuring ORFi; each containing two types of images (conditions): distractors (D: images seen for the first time in the current run) and targets (T: images seen for the second time in the current run). Group region of interest (ROI) analysis was performed in a flexible factorial design with two factors (run and condition) using SPM12. RESULTS We found significant main effects for the experimental run and condition. The bilateral OFC activation was higher during ORFi than during the first run. Additionally, the OFC was more active while processing distractors than targets. CONCLUSION These results confirm, for the first time, the role of OFC in reality filtering in early adolescents.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Chiara Liverani
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
| | - Lorena G. A. Freitas
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
- Institute of BioengineeringÉcole Polytechnique Fédérale de LausanneLausanneSwitzerland
| | - Vanessa Siffredi
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
- Institute of BioengineeringÉcole Polytechnique Fédérale de LausanneLausanneSwitzerland
| | - Greta Mikneviciute
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
| | | | - Djalel‐Eddine Meskaldij
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
- Institute of MathematicsÉcole Polytechnique Fédérale de LausanneLausanneSwitzerland
| | - Cristina Borradori Tolsa
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
| | - Russia Ha‐Vinh Leuchter
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
| | - Armin Schnider
- Department of Clinical NeurosciencesDivision of NeurorehabilitationGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
| | - Dimitri Van De Ville
- Institute of BioengineeringÉcole Polytechnique Fédérale de LausanneLausanneSwitzerland
| | - Petra Susan Hüppi
- Department of Paediatrics, Gynecology and ObstetricsDivision of Development and GrowthGeneva University HospitalsGenevaSwitzerland
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Renoult L, Rugg MD. An historical perspective on Endel Tulving's episodic-semantic distinction. Neuropsychologia 2020; 139:107366. [PMID: 32007511 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107366] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2019] [Revised: 01/09/2020] [Accepted: 01/29/2020] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
The distinction between episodic and semantic memory, proposed by Endel Tulving in 1972, remains a key concept in contemporary Cognitive Neuroscience. Here we review how this distinction evolved in Tulving's writings over the years. Crucially, from 1972 onward, he argued that the two forms of memory were inter-dependent and that their interaction was an essential feature of normal episodic memory function. Moreover, later elaborations of the theory clearly proposed that these interactions formed the basis of normal declarative memory functioning. A later but crucial aspect of Tulving's contribution was his stress on the importance of subjective experience, which, according to him, "should be the ultimate object of interest, the central aspect of remembering that is to be explained and understood". We relate these and his numerous other ideas to current perspectives about the organization and function of human memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Louis Renoult
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK.
| | - Michael D Rugg
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK; Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas, Dallas, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Contaminated confessions: How source and consistency of confession details influence memory and attributions. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.08.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
30
|
Misirlisoy M, Tanyas H, Atalay NB. Does survival context enhance memory for source? A within-subjects comparison. Memory 2019; 27:780-791. [DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2019.1566928] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mine Misirlisoy
- Department of Psychology, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Hilal Tanyas
- Department of Psychology, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Nart Bedin Atalay
- Department of Psychology, TOBB University of Economics and Technology, Ankara, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Zhang HH, Roberts KP, Teoh YS. Children's recall and source monitoring of a repeated event using a timeline as an interview aid. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3451] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Huan Huan Zhang
- Department of Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Ontario Canada
| | - Kim P. Roberts
- Department of Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Ontario Canada
| | - Yee-San Teoh
- Department of Psychology; National Taiwan University; Taipei Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Danby MC, Sharman SJ, Brubacher SP, Powell MB. The effects of episode similarity on children's reports of a repeated event. Memory 2018; 27:561-567. [PMID: 30295155 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1529798] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Much research has tested techniques to improve children's reporting of episodes from a repeated event by interviewing children after they have experienced multiple episodes of a scripted event. However, these studies have not considered any effects of the similarity shared between event episodes on children's reports. In the current study, 5- to 9-year-olds experienced four episodes of a scripted repeated event that shared a high (n = 76) or low (n = 76) degree of similarity, and were subsequently interviewed about individual episodes. The proportional amount and accuracy of children's reported details were tallied. Children reported proportionally more details and more script deviations after experiencing the high, compared to low, similarity event. Conversely, children were more accurate in their episodic reports when they experienced the low, compared to high, similarity event. The current findings have implications for the generalisability and comparability of past results across laboratory studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meaghan C Danby
- a School of Psychology , Deakin University , Burwood , VIC , Australia
| | | | - Sonja P Brubacher
- b Centre for Investigative Interviewing , Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University , Mount Gravatt , Australia
| | - Martine B Powell
- b Centre for Investigative Interviewing , Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University , Mount Gravatt , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Sleep and mindfulness meditation as they relate to false memory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 84:1084-1111. [PMID: 30244286 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1098-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2018] [Accepted: 09/17/2018] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
By a systematic analysis of the current literature, we compare two states of sleep and meditation in terms of their role in the formation or suppression of Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) false memory. We aim to suggest that the occurrence of false memory under these two states is a result of reinforcing some abilities and changes in cognitive systems which can ultimately improve some aspects of cognitive functions. In our analogy, we propose that: (1) both sleep and meditation may improve source monitoring ability whose failure is one of the most important mechanisms in producing false memories, and (2) despite improvement in source monitoring ability, adaptive cognitive processes, as mechanisms which are common in sleep and meditation, can still produce false memories. In conclusion, we propose that in spite of their contribution to false memory through adaptive processes, the beneficial role of sleep and meditation in cognition may be more prominent than their harmful role.
Collapse
|
34
|
Brubacher SP, Earhart B, Roberts KP, Powell MB. Effects of label training and recall order on children's reports of a repeated event. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sonja P. Brubacher
- Department of Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Ontario Canada
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University; Mt. Gravatt Queensland Australia
| | - Becky Earhart
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University; Mt. Gravatt Queensland Australia
| | - Kim P. Roberts
- Department of Psychology; Wilfrid Laurier University; Waterloo Ontario Canada
| | - Martine B. Powell
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Griffith Criminology Institute, Griffith University; Mt. Gravatt Queensland Australia
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Parker A, Dagnall N. Reduced impact of imagery processing on false autobiographical recollection: the effects of dynamic visual noise. Memory 2018; 27:163-173. [PMID: 29975174 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1495236] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Research has demonstrated that merely imagining an autobiographical event can bring about false memories for that event. One explanation for this is that imagination leads to the creation and incorporation of visual-imagistic information into the event representation. This idea was tested in two experiments in which visual-imagery processing was disrupted by the use of Dynamic Visual Noise (DVN). In Experiment 1, autobiographical memories that were rated as "known" and lacking in event detail were subsequently rated as more "remembered" following imagination. In Experiment 2, imagination led to improbable autobiographical events being rated as more believable and vivid. In both experiments, interfering with imagery processing by DVN reduced these effects. It was concluded that visual-imagistic processing plays an important role in altering the mnemonic status of autobiographical representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew Parker
- a Department of Psychology , Manchester Metropolitan University , Manchester , UK
| | - Neil Dagnall
- a Department of Psychology , Manchester Metropolitan University , Manchester , UK
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Ünal E, Papafragou A. Relations Between Language and Cognition: Evidentiality and Sources of Knowledge. Top Cogn Sci 2018; 12:115-135. [PMID: 29932304 PMCID: PMC7379197 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12355] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2017] [Revised: 03/30/2018] [Accepted: 04/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Understanding and acquiring language involve mapping language onto conceptual representations. Nevertheless, several issues remain unresolved with respect to (a) how such mappings are performed, and (b) whether conceptual representations are susceptible to cross-linguistic influences. In this article, we discuss these issues focusing on the domain of evidentiality and sources of knowledge. Empirical evidence in this domain yields growing support for the proposal that linguistic categories of evidentiality are tightly linked to, build on, and reflect conceptual representations of sources of knowledge that are shared across speakers of different languages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ercenur Ünal
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University.,Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen
| | - Anna Papafragou
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Cleveland KC, Quas JA, Lyon TD. The effects of implicit encouragement and the putative confession on children's memory reports. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2018; 80:113-122. [PMID: 29604502 PMCID: PMC5953828 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2017] [Revised: 03/10/2018] [Accepted: 03/14/2018] [Indexed: 05/11/2023]
Abstract
The current study tested the effects of two interview techniques on children's report productivity and accuracy following exposure to suggestion: implicit encouragement (backchanneling, use of children's names) and the putative confession (telling children that a suspect "told me everything that happened and wants you to tell the truth"). One hundred and forty-three, 3-8-year-old children participated in a classroom event. One week later, they took part in a highly suggestive conversation about the event and then a mock forensic interview in which the two techniques were experimentally manipulated. Greater use of implicit encouragement led to increases, with age, in children's narrative productivity. Neither technique improved or reduced children's accuracy. No increases in errors about previously suggested information were evident when children received either technique. Implications for the use of these techniques in child forensic interviews are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyndra C Cleveland
- Vanderbilt University, Department of Psychology and Human Development, Peabody College, 230 Appleton Place #552, Jesup Building Room 105, Nashville, TN 37203-5721, United States.
| | - Jodi A Quas
- 4328 Social & Behavioral Sciences Gateway, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, United States.
| | - Thomas D Lyon
- University of Southern California, 699 Exposition Boulevard, Los Angeles, CA 90089, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Otgaar H, Muris P, Howe ML, Merckelbach H. What Drives False Memories in Psychopathology? A Case for Associative Activation. Clin Psychol Sci 2017; 5:1048-1069. [PMID: 29170722 PMCID: PMC5665161 DOI: 10.1177/2167702617724424] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2017] [Accepted: 07/12/2017] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
In clinical and court settings, it is imperative to know whether posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression may make people susceptible to false memories. We conducted a review of the literature on false memory effects in participants with PTSD, a history of trauma, or depression. When emotional associative material was presented to these groups, their levels of false memory were raised relative to those in relevant comparison groups. This difference did not consistently emerge when neutral or nonassociative material was presented. Our conclusion is supported by a quantitative comparison of effect sizes between studies using emotional associative or neutral, nonassociative material. Our review suggests that individuals with PTSD, a history of trauma, or depression are at risk for producing false memories when they are exposed to information that is related to their knowledge base.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Peter Muris
- Maastricht University
- Stellenbosch University
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Carneiro P, Lapa A, Finn B. The effect of unsuccessful retrieval on children's subsequent learning. J Exp Child Psychol 2017; 166:400-420. [PMID: 29032207 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2017] [Revised: 09/05/2017] [Accepted: 09/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
It is well known that successful retrieval enhances subsequent adults' learning by promoting long-term retention. Recent research has also found benefits from unsuccessful retrieval, but the evidence is not as clear-cut when the participants are children. In this study, we employed a methodology based on guessing-the weak associate paradigm-to test whether children can learn from generated errors or whether errors are harmful for learning. We tested second- and third-grade children in Experiment 1 and tested preschool and kindergarten children in Experiment 2. With slight differences in the method, in both experiments children heard the experimenter saying one word (cue) and were asked to guess an associate word (guess condition) or to listen to the correspondent target-associated word (study condition), followed by corrective feedback in both conditions. At the end of the guessing phase, the children undertook a cued-recall task in which they were presented with each cue and were asked to say the corrected target. Together, the results showed that older children-those in kindergarten and early elementary school-benefited from unsuccessful retrieval. Older children showed more correct responses and fewer errors in the guess condition. In contrast, preschoolers produced similar levels of correct and error responses in the two conditions. In conclusion, generating errors seems to be beneficial for future learning of children older than 5years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paula Carneiro
- Centro de Investigação em Ciência Psicológica (CICPSI), Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, 1649-013 Lisboa, Portugal.
| | - Ana Lapa
- Centro de Investigação em Ciência Psicológica (CICPSI), Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, 1649-013 Lisboa, Portugal
| | - Bridgid Finn
- Educational Testing Service, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Memory Errors in Police Interviews: The Bait Question as a Source of Misinformation. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.01.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
41
|
Szojka ZA, Andrews SJ, Lamb ME, Stolzenberg SN, Lyon TD. Challenging the Credibility of Alleged Victims of Child Sexual Abuse in Scottish Courts. PSYCHOLOGY, PUBLIC POLICY, AND LAW : AN OFFICIAL LAW REVIEW OF THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA COLLEGE OF LAW AND THE UNIVERSITY OF MIAMI SCHOOL OF LAW 2017; 23:200-210. [PMID: 31555043 PMCID: PMC6760857 DOI: 10.1037/law0000117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the effects of credibility-challenging questions (n = 2,729) on 62 5- to 17-year-olds' testimony in child sexual abuse cases in Scotland by categorizing the type, source, and content of the credibility-challenging questions defense lawyers asked and assessing how children responded. Credibility-challenging questions comprised 14.9% of all questions asked during cross-examination. Of defense lawyers' credibility-challenging questions, 77.8% focused generally on children's honesty, whereas the remainder referred to specific inconsistencies in the children's testimony. Children resisted credibility challenges 54% of the time, significantly more often than they provided compliant responses (26.8%). The tendency to resist was significantly lower for questions focused on specific rather than general inconsistencies, and peripheral rather than central content. Overall, children resisted credibility challenges more often when the aim and content of the question could be understood easily. As this was a field study, the accuracy of children's responses could not be assessed. The findings suggest that credibility-challenging questions that place unrealistic demands on children's memory capacities (e.g., questions focused on peripheral content or highly specific details) occur frequently, and that juries should be made aware of the disproportionate effects of such questioning on the consistency of children's testimony.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zsófia A Szojka
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Samantha J Andrews
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Michael E Lamb
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Stacia N Stolzenberg
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| | - Thomas D Lyon
- Zsófia A. Szojka, School of Law, Royal Holloway, University of London; Samantha J. Andrews and Michael E. Lamb, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge; Stacia N. Stolzenberg, School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University; Thomas D. Lyon, Gould School of Law, University of Southern California
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Macris DM, Sobel DM. The Role of Evidence Diversity and Explanation in 4- and 5-Year-Olds’ Resolution of Counterevidence. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2017. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1323755] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
43
|
Abstract
A common finding in the source monitoring literature is that greater similarity impairs source discriminability. Experiments traditionally manipulate similarity overtly by describing or showing sources with explicitly differentiable features. However, people may also infer source characteristics themselves, which should also affect discriminability. Two studies examined inferred source characteristics by capitalizing on the out-group homogeneity effect, whereby in-group members are conceptualized as more diverse than out-group members. Participants learned about two sources who were described only as members of an in-group or an out-group and whose actions did not have higher a priori association with either group. Source memory was superior when participants believed the sources to be in-group members. This demonstrates that people spontaneously include inferred features with source representations and can capitalize on these features during source monitoring. Interestingly, information suggesting membership in one's in-group improved performance even for sources who had previously been considered out-group members (Experiment 2).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Greenstein
- 1 Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Nancy Franklin
- 1 Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Jessica Klug
- 1 Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Danby MC, Brubacher SP, Sharman SJ, Powell MB, Roberts KP. Children's Reasoning About Which Episode of a Repeated Event is Best Remembered. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Meaghan C. Danby
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Deakin University; Burwood Victoria Australia
| | - Sonja P. Brubacher
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Deakin University; Burwood Victoria Australia
| | - Stefanie J. Sharman
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Deakin University; Burwood Victoria Australia
| | - Martine B. Powell
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing; Deakin University; Burwood Victoria Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Jams T, Ratzon NZ. Can You Imagine? The Effect of Mental Practice on the Acquisition and Retention of a Motor Skill as a Function of Age. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/153944920002000302] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The differences among participants of various ages in their ability to use mental practice for motor skill acquisition have not been examined to date. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of age on mental practice. Participants were divided into 3 age groups: children, adults, and older adults; each group was randomly subdivided into physical and mental-physical practice groups. The results indicated that mental practice affected the 3 age groups differently. Mental practice in the acquisition phase of bimanual coordination task was found to be beneficial for children and older adults only. In the retention phase, this benefit was confined to older adults. Adults in the mental-physical and physical practice groups did not differ in performance of the acquired task. These findings indicate that at both ends of our lives—in childhood and old age—we benefit more from mental practice. Mental practice was especially beneficial for older adults regarding their retention process.
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
This paper analyses the rhetorical structure of psychological experiments investigating children's suggestibility, the wider context of debates that these inform, and the cultural-political status of psychological expertise that such claims bolster. It draws on the debates around childhood accounting to reverse the specular apparatus of the psychological gaze and so to inspect its practitioners according to those discourses and rhetorical devices by which it accounts for children. The stories psychologists tell about children are analysed as not only indicative of the contemporary legitimizing practices of the discipline, but also as suggesting a motivated forgetting or recasting of psychology's past approaches to the topics of children's beliefs, knowledge and memories. Thus, by turning the focus from the stories (we make) children tell, to our own storytelling practices as psychologists, this paper engages in a practice of critical relativism that opens the terms of developmental psychological inquiry up for critical scrutiny, and thereby both limits and secures the grounds for its claims.
Collapse
|
47
|
Otgaar H, Howe ML, Brackmann N, Smeets T. The malleability of developmental trends in neutral and negative memory illusions. J Exp Psychol Gen 2016; 145:31-55. [PMID: 26709588 PMCID: PMC4694095 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000127] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Among many legal professionals and memory researchers there exists the assumption that susceptibility to false memory decreases with age. In 4 misinformation experiments, we show that under conditions that focus on the meaning of experiences, children are not always the most susceptible to suggestion-induced false memories. We begin by presenting a short overview of previous developmental false memory studies, the majority of which have found that the susceptibility to misinformation decreases with age. In Experiment 1, 6/7-year-olds, 11/12-year-olds, and adults received a video and were confronted with misinformation about related but nonpresented details. Older children and adults had higher misinformation acceptance rates than younger children. In Experiment 2, we replicated this finding adding a younger child group (4/6-year-olds). In Experiments 3 and 4, we used new material and again found that susceptibility to misinformation increased with age. Together, these experiments show that children's memory accuracy is not necessarily inferior to that of adults.'
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Henry Otgaar
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University
| | - Mark L Howe
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University
| | | | - Tom Smeets
- Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Sui J, Zhu Y. Five-year-olds can show the self-reference advantage. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/01650250500172673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The current study developed a new paradigm to determine the age at which children begin to show the self-reference advantage in memory. Four-, 5-, and 10-year-olds studied lists of colourful object pictures presented together with self or other face image, and participants were asked to report aloud “who is pointing at the (object).” Then incidental free recall was carried out, followed by source judgments based on the earlier test where participants had to distinguish who pointed to the object. In Experiment 1, only 5-year-old children showed self-reference advantage in the recall, but not in source judgments. By increasing task demand in Experiment 2, 5 and 10-year-olds also showed the self-reference advantage in the recall, but not in source judgments. These results indicated that the new paradigm is appropriate to measure children's self-reference effect in memory, and children as young as 5 years begin to show this effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jie Sui
- Peking University and Northeast Normal University, People's
Republic of China
| | - Ying Zhu
- Peking University, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Johnson MK, Nolde SF, Mather M, Kounios J, Schacter DL, Curran T. The Similarity of Brain Activity Associated with True and False Recognition Memory Depends On Test Format. Psychol Sci 2016. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00421.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were compared for correct recognitions of previously presented words and false recognitions of associatively related, nonpresented words (lures) When the test items were presented blocked by test type (old, new, lure), waveforms for old and lure items were different, especially at frontal and left parietal electrode sites, consistent with previous positron emission tomography (PET) data (Schacter, Reiman, et al, 1996) When the test format randomly intermixed the types of items, waveforms for old and lure items were more similar We suggest that test format affects the type of processing subjects engage in, consistent with expectations from the source-monitoring framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993) These results also indicate that brain activity as assessed by neuroimaging designs requiring blocked presentation of trials (e.g., PET) do not necessarily reflect the brain activity that occurs in cognitive-behavioral paradigms, in which types of test trials are typically intermixed
Collapse
|
50
|
Cleveland KC, Quas JA, Lyon TD. Valence, Implicated Actor, and Children's Acquiescence to False Suggestions. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2016; 43:1-7. [PMID: 26955204 DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Although adverse effects of suggestive interviewing on children's accuracy are well documented, it remains unclear as to whether these effects vary depending on the valence of and the actor implicated in suggestions. In this study, 124 3-8-year-olds participated in a classroom activity and were later questioned about positive and negative false details. The interviewer provided positive reinforcement when children acquiesced to suggestions and negative feedback when they did not. Following reinforcement or feedback, young children were comparably suggestible for positive and negative details. With age, resistance to suggestions about negative details emerged first, followed by resistance to suggestions about positive details. Across age, more negative feedback was required to induce acquiescence to negative than positive false details. Finally, children were less willing to acquiesce when they (versus the confederate) were implicated. Findings highlight the interactive effects of valence and children's age on their eyewitness performance in suggestive contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyndra C Cleveland
- , , 4201 Social & Behavioral Sciences Gateway, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697-7085 United States
| | - Jodi A Quas
- , , 4328 Social & Behavioral Sciences Gateway, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697-7085 United States
| | - Thomas D Lyon
- , , University of Southern California, 699 Exposition Boulevard, Los Angeles, CA 90089 United States
| |
Collapse
|