1
|
Neuenschwander S, Rosso G, Branco N, Freitag F, Tehovnik EJ, Schmidt KE, Baron J. On the Functional Role of Gamma Synchronization in the Retinogeniculate System of the Cat. J Neurosci 2023; 43:5204-5220. [PMID: 37328291 PMCID: PMC10342227 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1550-22.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/12/2022] [Revised: 02/06/2023] [Accepted: 03/09/2023] [Indexed: 06/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Fast gamma oscillations, generated within the retina, and transmitted to the cortex via the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), are thought to carry information about stimulus size and continuity. This hypothesis relies mainly on studies conducted under anesthesia and the extent to which it holds under more naturalistic conditions remains unclear. Using multielectrode recordings of spiking activity in the retina and the LGN of both male and female cats, we show that visually driven gamma oscillations are absent for awake states and are highly dependent on halothane (or isoflurane). Under ketamine, responses were nonoscillatory, as in the awake condition. Response entrainment to the monitor refresh was commonly observed up to 120 Hz and was superseded by the gamma oscillatory responses induced by halothane. Given that retinal gamma oscillations are contingent on halothane anesthesia and absent in the awake cat, such oscillations should be considered artifactual, thus playing no functional role in vision.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Gamma rhythms have been proposed to be a robust encoding mechanism critical for visual processing. In the retinogeniculate system of the cat, many studies have shown gamma oscillations associated with responses to static stimuli. Here, we extend these observations to dynamic stimuli. An unexpected finding was that retinal gamma responses strongly depend on halothane concentration levels and are absent in the awake cat. These results weaken the notion that gamma in the retina is relevant for vision. Notably, retinal gamma shares many of the properties of cortical gamma. In this respect, oscillations induced by halothane in the retina may serve as a valuable preparation, although artificial, for studying oscillatory dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sergio Neuenschwander
- Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, 59076-550, Natal, Brazil
| | - Giovanne Rosso
- Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, 59076-550, Natal, Brazil
| | - Natalia Branco
- Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, 59076-550, Natal, Brazil
| | - Fabio Freitag
- Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, 59076-550, Natal, Brazil
| | - Edward J Tehovnik
- Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, 59076-550, Natal, Brazil
| | - Kerstin E Schmidt
- Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, 59076-550, Natal, Brazil
| | - Jerome Baron
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Minas Gerais, 31270-901, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Three weeks of SSRI administration enhances the visual perceptual threshold - a randomized placebo-controlled study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2019; 236:1759-1769. [PMID: 30623228 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-018-5158-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2018] [Accepted: 12/21/2018] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
RATIONALE The serotonergic system has been repeatedly linked to visual attention in general, but the effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) on specific components of visual attention remain unknown. Changes in distinct perceptual and cognitive processes are not readily evident in most attention paradigms. OBJECTIVE In this study, we isolate basic components of visual attention to investigate potential effects of longer-term SSRI administration on non-emotional aspects of visual attention in healthy males. METHODS In a randomized double-blind placebo-controlled design, 32 young healthy males were tested on multiple attentional parameters, before and after a 3-week SSRI intervention with fluoxetine (40 mg daily) or placebo. Data were modeled with a computational theory of visual attention to derive independent estimates of five distinct components of visual attention. RESULTS The SSRI intervention selectively and significantly lowered the threshold for conscious visual perception. Specifically, we demonstrate that this improvement does not stem from a general increase in the speed of visual processing, as previously suggested, but specifically from a change in the perceptual threshold. CONCLUSIONS The study provides a novel description of the attentional dynamics affected by SSRI, while supporting previous findings on attentional effects of SSRI. Furthermore, it accentuates the utility of employing accuracy-based measures of attentional performance when conducting psychopharmacological research.
Collapse
|
3
|
Borghuis BG, Tadin D, Lankheet MJ, Lappin JS, van de Grind WA. Temporal Limits of Visual Motion Processing: Psychophysics and Neurophysiology. Vision (Basel) 2019; 3:vision3010005. [PMID: 31735806 PMCID: PMC6802765 DOI: 10.3390/vision3010005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2018] [Revised: 01/11/2019] [Accepted: 01/11/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Under optimal conditions, just 3–6 ms of visual stimulation suffices for humans to see motion. Motion perception on this timescale implies that the visual system under these conditions reliably encodes, transmits, and processes neural signals with near-millisecond precision. Motivated by in vitro evidence for high temporal precision of motion signals in the primate retina, we investigated how neuronal and perceptual limits of motion encoding relate. Specifically, we examined the correspondence between the time scale at which cat retinal ganglion cells in vivo represent motion information and temporal thresholds for human motion discrimination. The timescale for motion encoding by ganglion cells ranged from 4.6 to 91 ms, and depended non-linearly on temporal frequency, but not on contrast. Human psychophysics revealed that minimal stimulus durations required for perceiving motion direction were similarly brief, 5.6–65 ms, and similarly depended on temporal frequency but, above ~10%, not on contrast. Notably, physiological and psychophysical measurements corresponded closely throughout (r = 0.99), despite more than a 20-fold variation in both human thresholds and optimal timescales for motion encoding in the retina. The match in absolute values of the neurophysiological and psychophysical data may be taken to indicate that from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) through to the level of perception little temporal precision is lost. However, we also show that integrating responses from multiple neurons can improve temporal resolution, and this potential trade-off between spatial and temporal resolution would allow for loss of temporal resolution after the LGN. While the extent of neuronal integration cannot be determined from either our human psychophysical or neurophysiological experiments and its contribution to the measured temporal resolution is unknown, our results demonstrate a striking similarity in stimulus dependence between the temporal fidelity established in the retina and the temporal limits of human motion discrimination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bart G. Borghuis
- Department of Anatomical Sciences and Neurobiology, University of Louisville School of Medicine, Louisville, KY 40202, USA
- Helmholtz Institute and Department of Functional Neurobiology, Utrecht University, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands
- Correspondence:
| | - Duje Tadin
- Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Center for Visual Science, Neuroscience, and Ophthalmology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627, USA
- Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37235, USA
| | - Martin J.M. Lankheet
- Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, 6700 AH Wageningen, The Netherlands
- Helmholtz Institute and Department of Functional Neurobiology, Utrecht University, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Joseph S. Lappin
- Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37235, USA
- Helmholtz Institute and Department of Functional Neurobiology, Utrecht University, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Wim A. van de Grind
- Helmholtz Institute and Department of Functional Neurobiology, Utrecht University, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Knez I. Affective and cognitive reactions to subliminal flicker from fluorescent lighting. Conscious Cogn 2014; 26:97-104. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2013] [Revised: 02/10/2014] [Accepted: 02/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
5
|
Avissar M, Wittig JH, Saunders JC, Parsons TD. Refractoriness enhances temporal coding by auditory nerve fibers. J Neurosci 2013; 33:7681-90. [PMID: 23637161 PMCID: PMC3865560 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3405-12.2013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2012] [Revised: 02/17/2013] [Accepted: 03/19/2013] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
A universal property of spiking neurons is refractoriness, a transient decrease in discharge probability immediately following an action potential (spike). The refractory period lasts only one to a few milliseconds, but has the potential to affect temporal coding of acoustic stimuli by auditory neurons, which are capable of submillisecond spike-time precision. Here this possibility was investigated systematically by recording spike times from chicken auditory nerve fibers in vivo while stimulating with repeated pure tones at characteristic frequency. Refractory periods were tightly distributed, with a mean of 1.58 ms. A statistical model was developed to recapitulate each fiber's responses and then used to predict the effect of removing the refractory period on a cell-by-cell basis for two largely independent facets of temporal coding: faithful entrainment of interspike intervals to the stimulus frequency and precise synchronization of spike times to the stimulus phase. The ratio of the refractory period to the stimulus period predicted the impact of refractoriness on entrainment and synchronization. For ratios less than ∼0.9, refractoriness enhanced entrainment and this enhancement was often accompanied by an increase in spike-time precision. At higher ratios, little or no change in entrainment or synchronization was observed. Given the tight distribution of refractory periods, the ability of refractoriness to improve temporal coding is restricted to neurons responding to low-frequency stimuli. Enhanced encoding of low frequencies likely affects sound localization and pitch perception in the auditory system, as well as perception in nonauditory sensory modalities, because all spiking neurons exhibit refractoriness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Avissar
- Department of Clinical Studies, New Bolton Center, School of Veterinary Medicine
| | - John H. Wittig
- Department of Clinical Studies, New Bolton Center, School of Veterinary Medicine
- Department of Bioengineering, School of Engineering and Applied Science, and
| | - James C. Saunders
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology: Head and Neck Surgery, School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104
| | - Thomas D. Parsons
- Department of Clinical Studies, New Bolton Center, School of Veterinary Medicine
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology: Head and Neck Surgery, School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Using electroretinograms to assess flicker fusion frequency in domestic hens Gallus gallus domesticus. Vision Res 2012; 62:125-33. [PMID: 22521657 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2011] [Revised: 03/16/2012] [Accepted: 04/04/2012] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The assessment of flicker fusion frequency (FFF), the stimulus frequency at which a flickering light stimulus can no longer be resolved and appears continuous, and critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF; the highest frequency at any light intensity that an observer can resolve flicker) are useful methods for comparing temporal resolution capabilities between animals. Behavioural experiments have found that average CFFs in domestic chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) are in the range of ca. 75-87 Hz, measured in response to full spectrum (i.e. white light plus UV) stimuli. In order to examine whether the chicken retina is able to detect flicker at higher frequencies, we used electroretinograms (ERGs) to assess FFF/CFF in adult hens from two commercial genotypes, Lohmann Selected Leghorns (LSLs) and Lohmann Browns (LBs). ERGs were recorded in response to flickering light at ten full spectrum light intensities ranging from 0.7 to 2740 cd m(-2). Two methods were used to determine FFF/CFF from the ERG recordings and these methods yielded very similar results, with average FFF ranging from ca. 20Hz at 0.7 cd m(-2) to an average CFF of ca. 105 Hz at 2740 cd m(-2). In some individuals, CFFs of 118-119 Hz were recorded. The Intensity/FFF (I/FFF) curves are double-branched with a break point representing the rod-cone transition occurring between 2.5 and 5.9 cd m(-2). No significant differences in the I/FFF curves were found between the two genotypes. At stimulus light intensities >250 cd m(-2), the ERG-derived FFF and CFF values are all higher than those from behavioural studies using the same stimuli. Although hens do not appear to be able to consciously perceive flicker above approximately 90 Hz, the finding that the ERG responses are able to remain in phase with light flickering at frequencies >100 Hz means that the retinae of domestic poultry housed in artificial light conditions may be able to resolve flicker from fluorescent lamps. As range of detrimental effects have been reported in humans as a result of exposure to such "invisible flicker", the possibility exists that flicker from fluorescent lamps also acts as stressor in domesticated birds.
Collapse
|
7
|
Charman WN. In the News. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2008. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.1988.tb01190.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- W. N. Charman
- University of Manchester Institute of Science and Technology
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Skrandies W. The Effect of Stimulation Frequency and Retinal Stimulus Location on Visual Evoked Potential Topography. Brain Topogr 2007; 20:15-20. [PMID: 17587164 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-007-0026-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/29/2007] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The activity of cortical neurons is influenced by retinal stimulus location and temporal modulation. We investigated how reversal frequency of black-and-white checkerboard patterns presented in different parts of the visual field affects evoked potential topography. Visual evoked potentials were recorded from an array of 16 electrodes over the occipital cortex in 12 healthy adults. A checkerboard reversal stimulus (40' check size) was presented with frequencies between 1.95 reversals/s and 7.81 reversals/s in the center or in the left or right hemiretina. Evoked potential fields displayed the well-known components of pattern reversal evoked activity. Computation of FFT and wavelets displayed electrical brain responses directly related to stimulation frequency. Further analysis showed that both retinal stimulus location and stimulation frequency affected visual evoked activity. Field strength as well as scalp field topography changed significantly with different reversal frequency. In addition, the pattern of lateralization of components also depended on temporal frequency of stimulation. Electrical brain activity elicited by visual stimuli shows globally similar features which are modulated by stimulus location and frequency. Our results indicate that--at least partly--different neuronal assemblies are activated by stimuli of different temporal characteristics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wolfgang Skrandies
- Institute of Physiology, Justus-Liebig University, Aulweg 129, Giessen 35392, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Seitz AR, Nanez JE, Holloway SR, Watanabe T. Visual experience can substantially alter critical flicker fusion thresholds. Hum Psychopharmacol 2005; 20:55-60. [PMID: 15568203 DOI: 10.1002/hup.661] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Studies of psychopharmacology often use the test of the critical flicker fusion (CFF) threshold as a measure of total information processing. It is true that studies of practice effects have shown that CFF thresholds are remarkably stable within and across multiple days of testing. This study confirms that subjects who undergo CFF testing on sequential days have stable thresholds, but also demonstrates that in subjects who conducted 1 h of motion training per day for 9 days the CFF thresholds increased by an average of 30%. The results show that the perceptual experience of subjects can dramatically alter the CFF thresholds and should be an important consideration in the control of studies employing the CFF as a measure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron R Seitz
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Ave, Boston, MA 02115, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Bossini L, Martinucci M, Paolini K, Castrogiovanni P. Panic-agoraphobic spectrum and light sensitivity in a general population sample in Italy. CANADIAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHIATRY. REVUE CANADIENNE DE PSYCHIATRIE 2005; 50:39-45. [PMID: 15754664 DOI: 10.1177/070674370505000108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study aimed to verify a possible correlation between panic symptoms and photosensitivity, not only in panic disorder (PD) but also in the panic-agoraphobic spectrum. METHOD One hundred and sixty-nine healthy and drug-free subjects completed the Structured Clinical Interview for Panic-Agoraphobic Spectrum-Lifetime version (SCI-PAS-Lifetime) and the Photosensitivity Assessment Questionnaire (PAQ). RESULTS The SCI-PAS-Lifetime total score was positively correlated with the total score of the PAQ photophobia subdimension (r = 0.44; P < 0.001); the SCI-PAS-Lifetime total score was not significantly correlated with the photophilia subdimension. As photophobia increased, we observed significant score increases in all SCI-PAS-Lifetime domains. Bivariate correlation showed higher coefficient correlation between the panic-like symptoms domain and photophobia (r = 0.44; P < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS A high total score in the SCI-PAS-Lifetime, which denotes more typical features of the spectrum, is associated with a higher level of light sensitivity and intolerance toward bright stimuli. This finding reflects clinical evidence that widely documents photophobic behaviours in subjects with PD and the importance of light stimuli exposure during the onset and course of such a disorder. Bright stimulation seems to be relevant both in PD diagnosed according to current DSM criteria and in the entire panic-agoraphobic spectrum, from nuclear elements of the disorder through subclinical states to the normal condition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Letizia Bossini
- Department of Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, University of Siena, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Krolak-Salmon P, Hénaff MA, Tallon-Baudry C, Yvert B, Guénot M, Vighetto A, Mauguière F, Bertrand O. Human lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex respond to screen flicker. Ann Neurol 2003; 53:73-80. [PMID: 12509850 DOI: 10.1002/ana.10403] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
The first electrophysiological study of the human lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), optic radiation, striate, and extrastriate visual areas is presented in the context of presurgical evaluation of three epileptic patients (Patients 1, 2, and 3). Visual-evoked potentials to pattern reversal and face presentation were recorded with depth intracranial electrodes implanted stereotactically. For Patient 1, electrode anatomical registration, structural magnetic resonance imaging, and electrophysiological responses confirmed the location of two contacts in the geniculate body and one in the optic radiation. The first responses peaked approximately 40 milliseconds in the LGN in Patient 1 and 60 milliseconds in the V1/V2 complex in Patients 2 and 3. Moreover, steady state visual-evoked potentials evoked by the unperceived but commonly experienced video-screen flicker were recorded in the LGN, optic radiation, and V1/V2 visual areas. This study provides topographic and temporal propagation characteristics of steady state visual-evoked potentials along human visual pathways. We discuss the possible relationship between the oscillating signal recorded in subcortical and cortical areas and the electroencephalogram abnormalities observed in patients suffering from photosensitive epilepsy, particularly video-game epilepsy. The consequences of high temporal frequency visual stimuli delivered by ubiquitous video screens on epilepsy, headaches, and eyestrain must be considered.
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
The conclusions drawn by Benjamin Libet from his work with colleagues on the timing of somatosensorial conscious experiences has met with a lot of praise and criticism. In this issue we find three examples of the latter. Here I attempt to place the divide between the two opponent camps in a broader perspective by analyzing the question of the relation between physical timing, neural timing, and experiential (mental) timing. The nervous system does a sophisticated job of recombining and recoding messages from the sensorial surfaces and if these processes are slighted in a theory, it might become necessary to postulate weird operations, including subjective back-referral. Neuroscientifically inspired theories are of necessity still based on guesses, extrapolations, and philosophically dubious manners of speech. They often assume some neural correlate of consciousness (NCC) as a part of the nervous system that transforms neural activity in reportable experiences. The majority of neuroscientists appear to assume that the NCC can compare and bind activity patterns only if they arrive simultaneously at the NCC. This leads to a search for synchrony or to theories in terms of the compensation of differences in neural delays (latencies). This is the main dimension of the Libet discussion. Examples from vision research, such as "temporal-binding-by-synchrony" and the "flash-lag" effect, are then used to illustrate these reasoning patterns in more detail. Alternatively one could assume symbolic representations of time and space (symbolic "tags") that are not coded in their own dimension (not time in time and space in space). Unless such tags are multiplexed with the quality message (tickle, color, or motion), one gets a binding problem for tags. One of the hidden aspects of the discussion between Libet and opponents appears to be the following. Is the NCC smarter than the rest of the nervous system, so that it can solve the problems of local sign (e.g., "where is the event"?) and timing (e.g., "when did it occur?" and "how long did it last?") on its own, or are these pieces of information coded symbolically early on in the system? A supersmart NCC appears to be the assumption of Libet's camp (which includes Descartes, but also mystics). The wish to distribute the smartness evenly across all stages of processing in the nervous system (smart recodings) appears to motivate the opponents. I argue that there are reasons to side with the latter group.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wim van de Grind
- Helmholtz Research School and Department of Functional Neurobiology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Abstract
Several studies have investigated the seasonal distribution of the birth dates of patients with psychiatric diseases. Our purpose was to verify if there is a specific distribution (by month) of birth dates in subjects with panic disorder (PD). The birth dates of 843 outpatients with a diagnosis of PD were compared with those of 1,181 subjects with other mental diseases. The birth dates of psychiatric patients were compared to those of the general Tuscane and Italian populations. The monthly distribution of birth in patients with PD (with and without comorbidity) peaked in September to December, while no relevant deviation in birth rate was observed in other mental diseases. Our results suggest a pathogenic role of birth seasonality in the development of PD.
Collapse
|
14
|
Küller R, Laike T. The impact of flicker from fluorescent lighting on well-being, performance and physiological arousal. ERGONOMICS 1998; 41:433-447. [PMID: 9557586 DOI: 10.1080/001401398186928] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
In working environments all over the world, fluorescent tubes are by far the dominating light source. Still, there have been very few studies on the impact of the non-visible flicker from fluorescent tubes. The purpose of the study was to compare the impact on subjective well-being, performance and physiological arousal of fluorescent light powered by conventional and high-frequency ballasts. Thirty-seven healthy males and females were subjected to either condition in a laboratory office on two separate occasions with 1 week in between. Although the methodology was quite extensive, only a few general effects were observed. However, when the light was powered by the conventional ballasts, individuals with high critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF) responded with a pronounced attenuation of EEG alpha waves, and an increase in speed and decrease in accuracy of performance. These results may be understood in terms of heightened arousal in the central nervous system in response to the pronounced light modulation caused by the conventional ballasts. In order to alleviate this potential stress source, it is recommended that fluorescent lighting be powered by electronic high-frequency ballasts of good quality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Küller
- Environmental Psychology Unit, School of Architecture, Lund Institute of Technology, Sweden
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Kennedy A, Brysbaert M, Murray WS. The effects of intermittent illumination on a visual inspection task. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. A, HUMAN EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 1998; 51:135-51. [PMID: 9532965 DOI: 10.1080/713755746] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Two experiments are described in which eye movements were monitored as subjects performed a simple target-spotting task under conditions of intermittent illumination produced by varying the display-screen frame rate on a computer VDU. In Experiment 1, subjects executed a saccade from a fixation point to a target which appeared randomly at a fixed eccentricity of 14 character positions to the left or right. Saccade latency did not differ reliably as a function of screen refresh rate, but average saccade extent at 70 Hz and 110 Hz was reliably shorter than at 90 Hz and 100 Hz. Experiment 2 examined the same task using a range of target eccentricities (7, 14, and 28 character positions to the left and right) and across a wider range of screen refresh rates. The results confirmed the curvilinear relationship obtained in Experiment 1, with average saccade extent reliably shorter at refresh rates of 50 Hz and 125 Hz than at 75 Hz and 100 Hz. While the effect was greater for remote targets, analyses of the proportional target error failed to show a reliable interaction between target eccentricity and display refresh rate. In contrast to Experiment 1, there was a pronounced effect of refresh rate on saccade latency (corrected for time to write the screen frame), with shorter latencies at higher refresh rates. It may be concluded that pulsation at frequencies above fusion disrupts saccade control. However, the curvilinear functional relationship between screen refresh rate and saccade extent obtained in these studies differs from previously reported effects of intermittent illumination on the average size of "entry saccades" (the first saccade to enter a given word) in a task involving word identification (Kennedy & Murray, 1993a, 1996). This conflict of data may arise in part because within-word adjustments in viewing position, which are typical of normal reading, influence measures of average saccade extent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Kennedy
- Psychology Department, University, Dundee, Scotland, U.K.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Sandström M, Lyskov E, Berglund A, Medvedev S, Mild KH. Neurophysiological effects of flickering light in patients with perceived electrical hypersensitivity. J Occup Environ Med 1997; 39:15-22. [PMID: 9029427 DOI: 10.1097/00043764-199701000-00006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
An increasing number of people in Sweden are claiming that they are hypersensitive to electricity. These patients suffer from skin as well as neurological symptoms when they are near computer monitors, fluorescent tubes, or other electrical appliances. Provocation studies with electromagnetic fields emitted from these appliances have, with only one exception, all been negative, indicating that there are other factors in the office environment that can effect the autonomic and/or central nervous system, resulting in the symptoms reported. Flickering light is one such factor and was therefore chosen as the exposure parameter in this study. Ten patients complaining of electrical hypersensitivity and the same number of healthy voluntary control subjects were exposed to amplitude-modulated light. The sensitivity of the brain to this type of visual stimulation was tested by means of objective electrophysiological methods such as electroretinography and visual evoked potential. A higher amplitude of brain cortical responses at all frequencies of stimulation was found when comparing patients with the control subjects, whereas no differences in retinal responses were revealed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Sandström
- Department of Environmental Medicine, University of Umeå, Sweden
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Bair W, Koch C. Temporal precision of spike trains in extrastriate cortex of the behaving macaque monkey. Neural Comput 1996; 8:1185-202. [PMID: 8768391 DOI: 10.1162/neco.1996.8.6.1185] [Citation(s) in RCA: 270] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
How reliably do action potentials in cortical neurons encode information about a visual stimulus? Most physiological studies do not weigh the occurrences of particular action potentials as significant but treat them only as reflections of average neuronal excitation. We report that single neurons recorded in a previous study by Newsome et al. (1989; see also Britten et al. 1992) from cortical area MT in the behaving monkey respond to dynamic and unpredictable motion stimuli with a markedly reproducible temporal modulation that is precise to a few milliseconds. This temporal modulation is stimulus dependent, being present for highly dynamic random motion but absent when the stimulus translates rigidly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W Bair
- Computation and Neural Systems Program, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena 91125, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Wilkins AJ, Wilkinson P. A tint to reduce eye-strain from fluorescent lighting? Preliminary observations. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1991; 11:172-5. [PMID: 2062542 DOI: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.1991.tb00217.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
The rapid modulation of light from fluorescent lamps is responsible for eye-strain and headaches. The modulation is greater at certain wavelengths than at others, and it can therefore be reduced by wearing tinted spectacles. A tint was designed: (1) to minimize the luminous pulsation of light from conventional halophosphate fluorescent lamps; (2) to avoid as much as possible any concomitant increase in the pulsation from triphosphor lamps; (3) to interfere with colour perception as little as possible; and (4) to have a cosmetically acceptable colour appearance. The four design criteria conflict. A compromise design is described, together with case histories of patients who appear to have benefited from the use of the tint.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A J Wilkins
- Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
We examined 20 individuals who had worn coloured glasses (Irlen filters) for a period of at least 3 months and who claimed to find them beneficial. Sixteen had a history of reading difficulties. The performance of a variety of visual tasks was compared: (1) using the coloured lenses; (2) using neutral density filters of similar photopic transmittance; and (3) using trial lenses to correct any residual refractive error. The coloured lenses appeared to reduce discomfort and susceptibility to anomalous perceptual effects upon viewing grating patterns. They also improved the speed of visual search by a small amount. The lenses had idiosyncratic effects on ocular muscle balance and acuity. They did not affect contrast sensitivity at a spatial frequency of 4 c/deg.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Wilkins
- MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
Under three types of artificial lighting 24 women with chronic agoraphobia and 24 female control subjects assessed their mood and bodily symptoms, and their heart rate was measured. One of the three types of lighting was incandescent. The other two were fluorescent, one pulsating in the conventional manner 100 times per second and the other relatively steady. Both were provided by a single fluorescent lamp controlled from one of two circuits. When exposed to the conventional pulsating fluorescent light under double-blind conditions the agoraphobic group showed a higher heart rate and reported more anomalous visual effects in response to an epileptogenic pattern. Control subjects reported more bodily symptoms under the conventional fluorescent light than under the two other lighting conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Hazell
- Clinical Psychology Service, Walsall Health Authority, West Midlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
|
22
|
Abstract
Refractory periods of the visual system were investigated in 12 healthy subjects by simultaneously recording retinal (ERG) and cortical (VEP) evoked electrical activity. Double-flash stimuli were presented at different interstimulus intervals, and response components evoked by the second flash were analyzed in detail, and related to psychophysical detection thresholds. With short interstimulus intervals ERG b-wave peak latencies were increased and b-wave amplitudes were significantly reduced, while P100 component latencies of the VEP were significantly influenced only at long interstimulus intervals. Regression analysis of the individual data as well as analysis of the retinocortical transmission times showed that the cortical latency changes were not simply caused by changes on the level of the retina. Additional influences of the interstimulus interval on nonretinal structures of the human visual system must be assumed. The subjective psychophysical detection thresholds were significantly higher than the threshold values at which reliable electrical or cortical response components could be elicited.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W Skrandies
- Max-Planck-Institute for Physiological and Clinical Research, Bad Nauheim, F.R.G
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Rose SP, Bell M, Michie W. Comparison of artificial light sources and lighting programmes for laying hens on long ahemeral light cycles. Br Poult Sci 1985; 26:357-65. [PMID: 4027738 DOI: 10.1080/00071668508416823] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Three lighting treatments were given to laying hens, for which the bright and dim light of 28-h ahemeral light cycles was provided by incandescent (tungsten filament) lamps only or by combinations of tubular fluorescent or compact gas-discharge lamps with incandescent lamps. There were no differences in entrainment (the proportion of eggs laid in 4, 6 or 8 h modal periods) between the three bright:dim treatments. A fourth ahemeral lighting treatment in which the dim lights were extinguished except during a designated work period (09.00 h to 12.00 h daily) was termed bright:dim:dark. Hens given the bright:dim:dark treatment showed an increased entrainment compared with the three bright:dim light treatments. There were no differences (P greater than 0.05) in egg numbers or mean egg weight between all 4 lighting treatments. Birds given the bright:dim:dark treatment tended to have a lower (P greater than 0.05) food intake compared to the three bright:dim treatments.
Collapse
|
24
|
Binnie CD, Findlay J, Wilkins AJ. Mechanisms of epileptogenesis in photosensitive epilepsy implied by the effects of moving patterns. ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY AND CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 1985; 61:1-6. [PMID: 2408857 DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)91065-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
The triggering of epileptiform EEG discharges by pattern is thought to depend on the intensity of excitation within the visual cortex. The present study investigates the role of the synchronisation of neuronal activity by the stimulus. In 10 pattern-sensitive subjects the effects of the following patterns have been compared: (1) static gratings, (2) gratings oscillating in a direction orthogonal to the lines (which should synchronise activity in direction-sensitive cortical units), and (3) gratings drifting at the same angular velocity (which should produce little or no synchronisation, because the contours enter and leave the overlapping receptive fields of individual neurones asynchronously). The oscillating gratings were most, and the drifting least, epileptogenic. In 2 further subjects oscillating and phase-reversing patterns were more epileptogenic than drifting gratings. Although open to alternative explanations, the findings conform with predictions from the hypothesis that synchronisation of individual cortical neurones by the stimulus contributes to epileptogenesis in photosensitive subjects.
Collapse
|