1
|
Sabiniewicz A, Sorokowska A, Palmiero M, Nicotra M, Sorokowski P. Gender differences in the relationship between neuroticism and odor memory. Physiol Behav 2024; 276:114475. [PMID: 38278502 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2024.114475] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2023] [Revised: 01/16/2024] [Accepted: 01/22/2024] [Indexed: 01/28/2024]
Abstract
Given the indications on the relationship between neuroticism and various olfactory functions as well as the emotionality of odor memory, it is surprising that no research so far has investigated whether neuroticism corresponds with odor memory performance. Thus, the purpose of our study was to address this question in the context of gender differences. Participants (73 men) completed the 60-items NEO Five Factor Inventory and participated in 32-items "Test for odor memory". In the separate analyses conducted for men and women, we demonstrated that women outperformed men regarding the number of correct odor recognitions in the odor memory test and had higher neuroticism level than men. Spearman correlation showed further that neuroticism was positively associated with D-prime (R = 0.3, p = .021) and the number of correct odor recognitions in women (R = 0.32, p = .014). This effect was, respectively, moderately (B10 = 5.2) and strongly confirmed using the Bayesian approach (B10 = 8). However, Bonferroni post-hoc correction for multiple comparisons further revealed that in case of neuroticism and the number of hits, as well as D-prime, alpha level was below significance, indicated by p = .006. The correlation between neuroticism level and odor memory performance was significantly higher in women compared to men (Z = 1.8, p = .038), while in case of neuroticism and D-prime, it was on a trend level (Z = 1.5, p = .074). Overall, the present results indicate that neuroticism might be linked to conscious odors' remembering for women, which can be explained by the warning role of odor memory and its connection to limbic structures, as well as by the higher level of neuroticism and odor memory performance in women compared to men.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agnieszka Sabiniewicz
- Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland; Interdisciplinary Center "Smell & Taste", Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany.
| | - Agnieszka Sorokowska
- Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland; Interdisciplinary Center "Smell & Taste", Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | | | - Michele Nicotra
- Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Foinant D, Lafraire J, Thibaut JP. Tears for pears: Influence of children’s neophobia on categorization performance and strategy in the food domain. Front Nutr 2022; 9:951890. [PMID: 36211481 PMCID: PMC9533737 DOI: 10.3389/fnut.2022.951890] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2022] [Accepted: 08/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Preschoolers’ neophobic dispositions mainly target fruits and vegetables. They received a great deal of attention in the past decades as these dispositions represent the main psychological barrier to dietary variety. Recently, children’s food neophobia has been found to be negatively correlated with their categorization performance (i.e., the accuracy to discriminate between food categories). We investigated categorization strategies among neophobic children, tendencies to favor one type of error over the other (misses over false alarms), in order to compensate for their poor categorization performance. To capture children’s categorization strategies, we used the Signal Detection Theory framework. A first experiment assessed 120 3-to-6-years old children’ sensitivity to discriminate between foods and nonfoods as well as their decision criterion (i.e., response strategy). In a second experiment, we manipulated the influence of food processing. The hypothesis was that food processing acts as a sign of human interventions that decreases uncertainty about edibility and thus promotes feelings of safety in the food domain. 137 children were tested on a food versus nonfood categorization task contrasting whole and sliced stimuli. In both experiments, increased levels of food neophobia were significantly associated with poorer categorization sensitivity and with a more conservative decision criterion (i.e., favoring “it is inedible” errors). Additionally, results from Experiment 2 revealed that food processing did not influence neophobic children, whereas their neophilic counterparts adopted a more liberal decision criterion for sliced stimuli than for whole stimuli. These findings are the first demonstration of a relationship between a decision criterion and food neophobia in young children. These results have strong implications for theories of food neophobia and laid the groundwork for designing novel types of food education interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Damien Foinant
- LEAD – CNRS UMR-5022, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
- Institut Paul Bocuse Research Center, Écully, France
- *Correspondence: Damien Foinant,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Velázquez AL, Vidal L, Varela P, Ares G. Can children use the A‐not a test? J SENS STUD 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/joss.12749] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ana Laura Velázquez
- Sensometrics & Consumer Science, Instituto Polo Tecnológico de Pando, Facultad de Química Universidad de la República Pando Canelones Uruguay
- Graduate Program in Chemistry, Facultad de Química Universidad de la República Pando Uruguay
| | - Leticia Vidal
- Sensometrics & Consumer Science, Instituto Polo Tecnológico de Pando, Facultad de Química Universidad de la República Pando Canelones Uruguay
| | | | - Gastón Ares
- Sensometrics & Consumer Science, Instituto Polo Tecnológico de Pando, Facultad de Química Universidad de la República Pando Canelones Uruguay
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Galler M, Mikkelsen E, Næs T, Liland KH, Ares G, Varela P. Children’s sweet tooth: Explicit ratings vs. Implicit bias measured by the Approach avoidance task (AAT). Food Qual Prefer 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104416] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
|
5
|
Bordim J, Lise CC, Marques C, Oldoni TC, Varela P, Mitterer‐Daltoé ML. Potential use of naturally colored antioxidants in the food industry—A study of consumers' perception and acceptance. J SENS STUD 2021. [DOI: 10.1111/joss.12657] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Jéssica Bordim
- Department of Chemistry Federal Technological University of Paraná (UTFPR) Pato Branco Paraná Brazil
| | - Carla Cristina Lise
- Department of Chemistry Federal Technological University of Paraná (UTFPR) Pato Branco Paraná Brazil
| | - Caroline Marques
- Department of Chemistry Federal Technological University of Paraná (UTFPR) Pato Branco Paraná Brazil
| | - Tatiane Cadorin Oldoni
- Department of Chemistry Federal Technological University of Paraná (UTFPR) Pato Branco Paraná Brazil
| | - Paula Varela
- Sensory & Consumer Sciences Nofima Ås Norway
- Department of Chemistry, Biotechnology and Food Science (KBM) The Norwegian University of Life Science Ås Norway
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wenzel ES, Cameron EL, Møller P, Köster EP. Novelty Detection in Memory for Common and Uncommon Odors over Short Delays. Chem Senses 2021; 46:6174308. [PMID: 33725719 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjab013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Odor memory is commonly believed to be very strong and long-lasting. The present study examined factors that impact odor recognition memory over short delay intervals (immediately or 30 s after target presentation) with emphasis on memory task (forced-choice vs "monadic"/single stimulus yes/no), odor category, and target/foil relationship. We explored trial-by-trial confidence as well as the effect of target familiarity, pleasantness, and intensity ratings, and odor nameability on memory for odors. Overall odor recognition memory in terms of proportion correct and sensitivity measures did not decline significantly during the 30-s delay interval in either task. However, hit rates were lower at 30 s and correct rejection rates for common odors remained consistently high. Recognition memory was better on trials in which the odor pairs were highly dissimilar, as well as on trials in which the target was an uncommon odor, particularly if it could be named. Familiarity, pleasantness, and intensity had no systematic effect on recognition memory. Whereas the results provide evidence of a fading memory trace, indicated by the decreased hit rates after a 30-s delay, the constant rates of correct rejections and high confidence ratings on those trials, even after delay, suggests that novelty detection (i.e., recognition that an odor is not one that has been encountered previously in that context) may play an important role in the memory for odors over short delays. Whether there is a separate short-term odor memory store is also addressed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - E Leslie Cameron
- Department of Psychological Science, Carthage College, Kenosha, WI, USA
| | - Per Møller
- Department of Food Science, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
| | - Egon P Köster
- Helmholtz Institute, University of Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
MAZON S, MENIN D, CELLA BM, LISE CC, VARGAS TDO, DALTOÉ MLM. Exploring consumers’ knowledge and perceptions of unconventional food plants: case study of addition of Pereskia aculeata Miller to ice cream. FOOD SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1590/fst.39218] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Suelen MAZON
- Universidade Tecnológica Federal do Paraná, Brasil
| | - Daiana MENIN
- Universidade Tecnológica Federal do Paraná, Brasil
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
|
9
|
|
10
|
Thomas A, van der Stelt A, Schlich P, Lawlor JB. Temporal drivers of liking for oral nutritional supplements for older adults throughout the day with monitoring of hunger and thirst status. Food Qual Prefer 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
|
11
|
Tasty or Sustainable? The Effect of Product Sensory Experience on a Sustainable New Food Product: An Application of Discrete Choice Experiments on Chianina Tinned Beef. SUSTAINABILITY 2018. [DOI: 10.3390/su10082795] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
This study aims to contribute to the existing literature by verifying whether the degree of liking of a new food product influences people’s preferences and willingness to pay from a discrete choice experiment when dealing with sustainable food products. To this purpose, we considered the case study of the introduction into the Italian market of a new food product: tinned Chianina meat. Among the attributes considered for this new product, two in particular were related to sustainability: organic breeding and the preservation of a traditional rural landscape. Half of the respondents underwent a sensory test before taking part in the hypothetical market (discrete choice experiment), while the remaining were administered the tests in reverse order. Tasting the product before the discrete choice experiment did not produce different willingness to pay (WTP) parameters as estimated by a taste factor interaction. However, separating the respondents into those who liked or disliked the product in the tasting condition revealed differences in willingness to pay results. The preferences are different for more than 50% of the attributes considered, and the magnitude of this difference is quite relevant. The WTP for one well known and certified sustainability related attribute—organic breeding—was not affected by the liking, while, for the other—the preservation of a traditional rural landscape—the effect of liking decreases the WTP. As a consequence, we suggest that tasting and liking studies should be routinely coupled with discrete choice studies when analyzing the introduction of new food products, especially when considering sustainable attributes in the experimental design. In the case of organic products where the expectations about taste are higher, neglecting to consider their sensory perception, along with the other discrete choice experiment attributes, could seriously undermine their long lasting success on the market.
Collapse
|
12
|
Rioux C, Leglaye L, Lafraire J. Inductive reasoning, food neophobia, and domain-specificity in preschoolers. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
|
13
|
Schwartz C, Vandenberghe-Descamps M, Sulmont-Rossé C, Tournier C, Feron G. Behavioral and physiological determinants of food choice and consumption at sensitive periods of the life span, a focus on infants and elderly. INNOV FOOD SCI EMERG 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ifset.2017.09.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
|
14
|
Visual exposure and categorization performance positively influence 3- to 6-year-old children's willingness to taste unfamiliar vegetables. Appetite 2018; 120:32-42. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.08.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2017] [Revised: 08/01/2017] [Accepted: 08/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
15
|
The smell of terroir! Olfactory discrimination between wines of different grape variety and different terroir. Food Qual Prefer 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.12.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
16
|
Mojet J, Köster E, Holthuysen N, Van Veggel R, De Wijk R, Schepers H, Vermeer F. The emotional influence of flowers on social perception and memory: An exploratory study. Food Qual Prefer 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
17
|
Song X, Giacalone D, Bølling Johansen SM, Frøst MB, Bredie WL. Changes in orosensory perception related to aging and strategies for counteracting its influence on food preferences among older adults. Trends Food Sci Technol 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tifs.2016.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
18
|
Lafraire J, Rioux C, Roque J, Giboreau A, Picard D. Rapid categorization of food and nonfood items by 3- to 4-year-old children. Food Qual Prefer 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
19
|
Doets EL, Kremer S. The silver sensory experience – A review of senior consumers’ food perception, liking and intake. Food Qual Prefer 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
20
|
Research challenges and methods to study food preferences in school-aged children: A review of the last 15years. Food Qual Prefer 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.07.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
21
|
Köster EP, Mojet J. From mood to food and from food to mood: A psychological perspective on the measurement of food-related emotions in consumer research. Food Res Int 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.04.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 101] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
22
|
Mojet J, Dürrschmid K, Danner L, Jöchl M, Heiniö RL, Holthuysen N, Köster E. Are implicit emotion measurements evoked by food unrelated to liking? Food Res Int 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.06.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
23
|
|
24
|
Kollndorfer K, Kowalczyk K, Nell S, Krajnik J, Mueller CA, Schöpf V. The inability to self-evaluate smell performance. How the vividness of mental images outweighs awareness of olfactory performance. Front Psychol 2015; 6:627. [PMID: 26042062 PMCID: PMC4434946 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00627] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2015] [Accepted: 04/28/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
To rate one’s individual olfactory performance is difficult and in many cases differs clearly from validated objective olfactory performance measures. This study aimed to investigate the basis for this measurement drift between objective and subjective olfactory performance evaluation. In absence of an actual odor, one may imagine an olfactory stimulus to evaluate his subjective olfactory performance. Therefore, the impact of the vividness of mental images on self-evaluation of smell performance in patients with mild to severe olfactory dysfunction and healthy controls was investigated. Fifty-nine patients with peripheral olfactory dysfunction ranging from reduced olfactory function (hyposmia) to complete loss of olfactory perception (anosmia) and 16 healthy controls were included. Olfactory performance was assessed using the Sniffin’ Sticks battery, the vividness of olfactory mental images was evaluated using the vividness of olfactory imagery questionnaire (VOIQ). Decreased vividness of odor images was obtained for anosmic patients, and a trend of poorer odor imagery was determined in hyposmic patients. Multiple regression analyses revealed the VOIQ score as significant predictor for olfactory self-evaluation for hyposmic patients and healthy controls. In contrast, for anosmic patients, the only significant predictor for self-rating of olfactory performance was the threshold-detection-identification (TDI) score, measuring overall olfactory performance. The results of this study indicate that sensory perception and mental images are closely related to each other. Furthermore, subjects who were able to perceive odors, even to a smaller extent, rely on the vividness of their mental odor images to evaluate their olfactory performance. In contrast, anosmic patients rather trust in their knowledge that they are not able to perceive odors. We are therefore able to subjectively rate our olfactory performance levels, if we are not able to perceive odors, but not if we are able to perceive olfactory input.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathrin Kollndorfer
- Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna , Vienna, Austria
| | - Ksenia Kowalczyk
- Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna , Vienna, Austria
| | - Stefanie Nell
- Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna , Vienna, Austria
| | - Jacqueline Krajnik
- Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna , Vienna, Austria
| | - Christian A Mueller
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Medical University of Vienna , Vienna, Austria
| | - Veronika Schöpf
- Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna , Vienna, Austria ; Department of Psychology, University of Graz , Graz, Austria ; BioTechMed , Graz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Exemplarity measurement and estimation of the level of interjudge agreement for two categories of French red wines. Food Qual Prefer 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
26
|
Memory processes in the development of reduced-salt foods. Appetite 2014; 83:125-134. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.08.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2013] [Revised: 07/25/2014] [Accepted: 08/13/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
27
|
Köster EP, van der Stelt O, Nixdorf RR, Linschoten MRI, de Wijk RA, Mojet J. Olfactory Imagination and Odor Processing: Three Same–Different Experiments. CHEMOSENS PERCEPT 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s12078-014-9165-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
28
|
Parma V, Castiello U, Köster EP, Mojet J. Selecting food. The contribution of memory, liking, and action. Appetite 2014; 76:186-96. [PMID: 24560690 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2013] [Revised: 02/01/2014] [Accepted: 02/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The goal of the present experiment was twofold: identifying similarities and differences between flavour memory and visual memory mechanisms and investigating whether kinematics could serve as an implicit measure for food selection. To test flavour and visual memory an 'implicit' paradigm to represent real-life situations in a controlled lab setting was implemented. A target, i.e., a piece of cake shaped like either an orange or a tangerine, covered with either orange- or a tangerine-flavoured icing, was provided to participants on Day 1. On Day 2, without prior notice, participants were requested to recognize the target amongst a set of distractors, characterized by various flavours (orange vs. tangerine) and/or sizes (orange-like vs. tangerine-like). Similarly, targets and distractors consisting of 2D figures varying in shape and size were used to assess visual memory. Reach-to-grasp kinematics towards the targets were recorded and analysed by means of digitalization techniques. Correlations between kinematic parameters, memory and liking for each food item were also calculated. Results concerned with memory recollection indices provided evidence of different key mechanisms which could be based either on novelty of flavour memory or visual memory, respectively. To a moderate extent, kinematics may serve as an implicit index of food selection processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valentina Parma
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.
| | - Umberto Castiello
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Egon Peter Köster
- Psychology Department, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Jos Mojet
- Wageningen-UR, Food and Bio-based Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Köster EP, Møller P, Mojet J. A "Misfit" Theory of Spontaneous Conscious Odor Perception (MITSCOP): reflections on the role and function of odor memory in everyday life. Front Psychol 2014; 5:64. [PMID: 24575059 PMCID: PMC3920064 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2013] [Accepted: 01/16/2014] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Our senses have developed as an answer to the world we live in (Gibson, 1966) and so have the forms of memory that accompany them. All senses serve different purposes and do so in different ways. In vision, where orientation and object recognition are important, memory is strongly linked to identification. In olfaction, the guardian of vital functions such as breathing and food ingestion, perhaps the most important (and least noticed and researched) role of odor memory is to help us not to notice the well-known odors or flavors in our everyday surroundings, but to react immediately to the unexpected ones. At the same time it provides us with a feeling of safety when our expectancies are met. All this happens without any smelling intention or conscious knowledge of our expectations. Identification by odor naming is not involved in this and people are notoriously bad at it. Odors are usually best identified via the episodic memory of the situation in which they once occurred. Spontaneous conscious odor perception normally only occurs in situations where attention is demanded, either because the inhaled air or the food smell is particularly good or particularly bad and people search for its source or because people want to actively enjoy the healthiness and pleasantness of their surroundings or food. Odor memory is concerned with novelty detection rather than with recollection of odors. In this paper, these points are illustrated with experimental results and their consequences for doing ecologically valid odor memory research are drawn. Furthermore, suggestions for ecologically valid research on everyday odor memory and some illustrative examples are given.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Egon P. Köster
- Psychological Laboratory, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht UniversityUtrecht, Netherlands
| | - Per Møller
- Department of Food Science, University of CopenhagenFrederiksberg, Denmark
| | - Jozina Mojet
- Wageningen – UR, Food and Biobased ResearchWageningen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Laureati M, Pagliarini E. Learning and retention time effect on memory for sweet taste in children. Food Qual Prefer 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
31
|
Song BM, Faumont S, Lockery S, Avery L. Recognition of familiar food activates feeding via an endocrine serotonin signal in Caenorhabditis elegans. eLife 2013; 2:e00329. [PMID: 23390589 PMCID: PMC3564447 DOI: 10.7554/elife.00329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2012] [Accepted: 12/22/2012] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Familiarity discrimination has a significant impact on the pattern of food intake across species. However, the mechanism by which the recognition memory controls feeding is unclear. Here, we show that the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans forms a memory of particular foods after experience and displays behavioral plasticity, increasing the feeding response when they subsequently recognize the familiar food. We found that recognition of familiar food activates the pair of ADF chemosensory neurons, which subsequently increase serotonin release. The released serotonin activates the feeding response mainly by acting humorally and directly activates SER-7, a type 7 serotonin receptor, in MC motor neurons in the feeding organ. Our data suggest that worms sense the taste and/or smell of novel bacteria, which overrides the stimulatory effect of familiar bacteria on feeding by suppressing the activity of ADF or its upstream neurons. Our study provides insight into the mechanism by which familiarity discrimination alters behavior.DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.00329.001.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bo-mi Song
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, United States
- Department of Molecular Biology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, United States
| | - Serge Faumont
- Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, United States
| | - Shawn Lockery
- Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, United States
| | - Leon Avery
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, United States
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Chambaron S, De Facq P, Virely X, Chabanet C, Issanchou S, Sulmont-Rossé C. Impact of Retention Interval on Recognition of a New Sensory Variant of Orange Juice. CHEMOSENS PERCEPT 2012. [DOI: 10.1007/s12078-012-9132-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
33
|
Møller P, Köster EP, Dijkman N, de Wijk R, Mojet J. Same–Different Reaction Times to Odors: Some Unexpected Findings. CHEMOSENS PERCEPT 2012. [DOI: 10.1007/s12078-012-9124-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|