1
|
Akyüz N, Marien H, Stok FM, Driessen JMA, Aarts H. Choice effects on temporal binding of action and outcomes: Examining the role of outcome focus and measures of time interval estimation. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2024; 248:104434. [PMID: 39079191 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2024.104434] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2023] [Revised: 07/12/2024] [Accepted: 07/24/2024] [Indexed: 08/24/2024] Open
Abstract
The ability to make one's own choices is vital to the experience of intentional behavior. Such agency experiences are reflected in the perceptual compression of time between actions and resulting outcomes. Whereas some studies show that choice limitations weaken temporal binding, other studies do not find such an effect. Reviewing the literature, we noted two potential factors that may moderate choice limitation effects on temporal binding: (a) the extent to which individuals represent their actions in terms of the consequences they produce; and (b) the response mode of the time interval estimation measurement where participants report numbers or use a slider to indicate time intervals. Testing these conceptual and methodological factors in two separate experiments yielded clear effects of choice limitation on temporal binding but no clear moderator role of the two factors. Interestingly, overall analyses showed that the choice limitation effect gradually vanishes over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nil Akyüz
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - Hans Marien
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - F Marijn Stok
- Department of Interdisciplinary Social Science, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - Josi M A Driessen
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - Henk Aarts
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Qin K, Marien H, Custers R, Aarts H. How the environment evokes actions that lead to different goals: the role of object multi-functionality in pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2023:1-14. [PMID: 37359629 PMCID: PMC10088748 DOI: 10.1007/s12144-023-04612-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/23/2023] [Indexed: 06/28/2023]
Abstract
Research shows that stimuli in the environment can trigger behavior via the activation of goal representations. This process can be tested in the Pavlovian-to-Instrumental Transfer (PIT) paradigm, where stimuli can only affect behavior through the activation of the representation of its desired outcome (i.e., the PIT effect). Previous research has demonstrated that the PIT effect is stronger when the goal is more desirable. While this research only looked at actions that have single outcomes (e.g., obtaining a snack to satisfy appetite), in the present paper, we reason that actions that are instrumental in obtaining outcomes that are desirable in multiple ways (e.g., obtaining a snack to satisfy one's appetite, giving it to a friend, trading it for money) should produce stronger PIT effects. In two experiments, participants learned to perform left and right key presses to earn a snack, either framed as having a single function or multiple functions. Participants also learned to associate the two differently framed snacks with two cues. In a PIT test, they were required to press the keys as fast as possible upon exposure to the cues (i.e., the PIT effect). We found that cues associated with the multi-functional snack facilitated the actions that earned those snacks before, while cues associated with the single-functional snack did not facilitate such actions. We discuss these findings in the context of research on free choice and personal autonomy and how people appreciate the multi-functional nature of their goal-directed behavior in the environment. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s12144-023-04612-2.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaiyang Qin
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, PO BOX 80140, Utrecht, 3508 TC The Netherlands
| | - Hans Marien
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, PO BOX 80140, Utrecht, 3508 TC The Netherlands
| | - Ruud Custers
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, PO BOX 80140, Utrecht, 3508 TC The Netherlands
| | - Henk Aarts
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, PO BOX 80140, Utrecht, 3508 TC The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Reddy NN. Non-motor cues do not generate the perception of self-agency: A critique of cue-integration. Conscious Cogn 2022; 103:103359. [PMID: 35687981 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2022.103359] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2021] [Revised: 04/24/2022] [Accepted: 05/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
How does one know that (s)he is the causal agent of their motor actions? Earlier theories of sense of agency have attributed the capacity for perception of self-agency to the comparator process of the motor-control/action system. However, with the advent of the findings implying a role of non-motor cues (like affective states, beliefs, primed concepts, and social instructions or previews of actions) in the sense of agency literature, the perception of self-agency is hypothesized to be generated even by non-motor cues (based on their relative reliability or weighting estimate); and, this theory is come to be known as the cue-integration of sense of agency. However, the cue-integration theory motivates skepticism about whether it is falsifiable and whether it is plausible that non-motor cues that are sensorily unrelated to typical sensory processes of self-agency have the capacity to produce a perception of self-agency. To substantiate this skepticism, I critically analyze the experimental operationalizations of cue-integration-with the (classic) vicarious agency experiment as a case study-to show that (1) the participants in these experiments are ambiguous about their causal agency over motor actions, (2) thus, these participants resort to reports of self-agency as heuristic judgments (under ambiguity) rather than due to cue-integration per se, and (3) there might not have occurred cue-integration based self-agency reports if these experimental operationalizations had eliminated ambiguity about the causal agency. Thus, I conclude that the reports of self-agency (observed in typical non-motor cues based cue-integration experiments) are not instances of perceptual effect-that are hypothesized to be produced by non-motor cues-but are of heuristic judgment effect.
Collapse
|
4
|
Kip A, Blom D, van der Weiden A. On the course of goal pursuit: The influence of goal progress on explicit judgments of self-agency. Conscious Cogn 2021; 96:103222. [PMID: 34687990 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2020] [Revised: 07/09/2021] [Accepted: 10/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The experience of causing our own actions and resulting outcomes (i.e., self-agency) is essential for the regulation of our actions during goal pursuit. In two experiments, participants indicated experienced self-agency over presented outcomes, which varied in distance to their goal in an agency-ambiguous task. In Study 1, progress was manipulated at trial level (i.e., stimuli moved randomly or sequentially towards the goal). In Study 2, progress was constant at trial level (sequential), but varied at task level (i.e., goal discrepancy of the outcomes was random or decreased over trials). Study 1 showed that self-agency gradually increased in the progress condition as unsuccessful outcomes were objectively closer to the goal, while self-agency increased exponentially upon full goal attainment in the absence of progress. The gradual pattern for the progress condition was replicated in Study 2. These studies indicate that explicit judgments of self-agency are more flexible when there is goal progress.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anneloes Kip
- Department of Social, Health, and Organisational Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584CS Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - Demi Blom
- Department of Social, Health, and Organisational Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584CS Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Anouk van der Weiden
- Department of Social, Health, and Organisational Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584CS Utrecht, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Potts CA, Carlson RA. What am I doing? It depends: agency and action identification. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2021; 86:651-666. [PMID: 33839940 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-021-01510-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2020] [Accepted: 03/23/2021] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Actions can be identified at a range of levels, from higher level, outcome-related descriptions to lower level, movement-related descriptions. But how do these levels of identification influence the experience of control (agency) over a task? We addressed the relation between the level of action identification and agency using a hierarchical task modeled from typing. Participants memorized letter sequences and reported them by moving a cursor to targets that contained letters. To manipulate lower level (aiming) difficulty, the targets were either large or small. To manipulate higher level (memory) difficulty, the letter sequences were either constant or random within a block. We found effects of higher and lower level difficulty on agency and action identification. Moreover, we found interactive effects of higher and lower level difficulty on performance. We discuss these findings in terms of contributions to the study of agency, and some differences from the results of previous studies of action identification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cory A Potts
- Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802, USA.
| | - Richard A Carlson
- Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Marien H, Custers R, Aarts H. Studying Human Habits in Societal Context: Examining Support for a Basic Stimulus–Response Mechanism. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2019. [DOI: 10.1177/0963721419868211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Human habits are considered to be an important root of societal problems. The significance of habits has been demonstrated for a variety of behaviors in different domains, such as work, transportation, health, and ecology, suggesting that habits have a pervasive impact on human life. Studying and changing habits in societal context requires a broad view of behavior, which poses a challenge for applying basic models to complex human habits. We address the conceptualization and operationalization of habits in the current literature and note that claims about the role of habits in societal context rarely agree with the basic definition of habits as goal-independent behavior. We consider future directions that are important for making progress in the study of habit change in societal context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hans Marien
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University
| | | | - Henk Aarts
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
van der Weiden A, Liepelt R, van Haren NEM. A matter of you versus me? Experiences of control in a joint go/no-go task. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2019; 83:842-851. [PMID: 28840393 PMCID: PMC6557877 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-017-0903-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2017] [Accepted: 08/04/2017] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
When interacting with others, people represent their own as well as their interaction partners' actions. Such joint action representation is essential for action coordination, but may also interfere with action control. We investigated how joint action representations affect experienced control over people's own actions and their interaction partners' actions. Participants performed a joint go/no-go task, which is commonly used to measure to what extent people represent their own actions in spatial reference to their interaction partner (e.g., as 'left' vs. 'right'). After each second trial, participants indicated experienced control over their own action, their interaction partner's action, or over action inhibition. Despite this frequent interruption of the go/no-go task, we found strong evidence for the spatial representation of joint actions. However, this joint action representation did not affect experiences of control. Possible explanations and implications of these findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anouk van der Weiden
- Social Health and Organizational Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584 CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
- Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Huispostnummer A.01.126, PO Box 85500, 3508 GA, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Roman Liepelt
- Department of Psychology, German Sport University Cologne, Am Sportpark Müngersdorf 6, 50933, Cologne, Germany
- Institute for Psychology, University of Muenster, Fliednerstrasse 21, 48149, Münster, Germany
| | - Neeltje E M van Haren
- Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Huispostnummer A.01.126, PO Box 85500, 3508 GA, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Impaired self-agency inferences in schizophrenia: The role of cognitive capacity and causal reasoning style. Eur Psychiatry 2017; 47:27-34. [PMID: 29096130 DOI: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.08.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2017] [Revised: 08/28/2017] [Accepted: 08/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The sense of self-agency, i.e., experiencing oneself as the cause of one's own actions, is impaired in patients with schizophrenia. Normally, inferences of self-agency are enhanced when actual outcomes match with pre-activated outcome information, where this pre-activation can result from explicitly set goals (i.e., goal-based route) or implicitly primed outcome information (i.e., prime-based route). Previous research suggests that patients show specific impairments in the prime-based route, implicating that they do not rely on matches between implicitly available outcome information and actual action-outcomes when inferring self-agency. The question remains: Why? Here, we examine whether neurocognitive functioning and self-serving bias (SSB) may explain abnormalities in patients' agency inferences. METHODS Thirty-six patients and 36 healthy controls performed a commonly used agency inference task to measure goal- and prime-based self-agency inferences. Neurocognitive functioning was assessed with the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) and the SSB was assessed with the Internal Personal and Situational Attributions Questionnaire. RESULTS Results showed a substantial smaller effect of primed outcome information on agency experiences in patients compared with healthy controls. Whereas patients and controls differed on BACS and marginally on SSB scores, these differences were not related to patients' impairments in prime-based agency inferences. CONCLUSIONS Patients showed impairments in prime-based agency inferences, thereby replicating previous studies. This finding could not be explained by cognitive dysfunction or SSB. Results are discussed in the context of the recent surge to understand and examine deficits in agency experiences in schizophrenia.
Collapse
|
9
|
Attentional control and inferences of agency: Working memory load differentially modulates goal-based and prime-based agency experiences. Conscious Cogn 2015; 38:38-49. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2014] [Revised: 10/01/2015] [Accepted: 10/04/2015] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
10
|
Renes RA, van der Weiden A, Prikken M, Kahn RS, Aarts H, van Haren NEM. Abnormalities in the experience of self-agency in schizophrenia: A replication study. Schizophr Res 2015; 164:210-3. [PMID: 25843918 DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2015] [Accepted: 03/17/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
People usually experience agency over their actions and subsequent outcomes. These agency inferences over action-outcomes are essential to social interaction, and occur when an actual outcome corresponds with either a specific goal (goal-based), and matches with action-outcome information that is subtly pre-activated in the situation at hand (prime-based). Recent research showed that schizophrenia patients exhibit goal-based inferences, but not prime-based inferences. Intrigued by these findings, and underscoring their potential role in explaining poor social functioning, we replicate patients' deficit in prime-based agency inferences. Additionally, we exclude the account that patients are unable to visually process and attend to primed information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert A Renes
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | - Anouk van der Weiden
- Department of Psychiatry of the Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Merel Prikken
- Department of Psychiatry of the Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - René S Kahn
- Department of Psychiatry of the Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Henk Aarts
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Neeltje E M van Haren
- Department of Psychiatry of the Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Chambon V, Sidarus N, Haggard P. From action intentions to action effects: how does the sense of agency come about? Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8:320. [PMID: 24860486 PMCID: PMC4030148 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 90] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2014] [Accepted: 04/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Sense of agency refers to the feeling of controlling an external event through one's own action. On one influential view, agency depends on how predictable the consequences of one's action are, getting stronger as the match between predicted and actual effect of an action gets closer. Thus, sense of agency arises when external events that follow our action are consistent with predictions of action effects made by the motor system while we perform or simply intend to perform an action. According to this view, agency is inferred retrospectively, after an action has been performed and its consequences are known. In contrast, little is known about whether and how internal processes involved in the selection of actions may influence subjective sense of control, in advance of the action itself, and irrespective of effect predictability. In this article, we review several classes of behavioral and neuroimaging data suggesting that earlier processes, linked to fluency of action selection, prospectively contribute to sense of agency. These findings have important implications for better understanding human volition and abnormalities of action experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valérian Chambon
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, INSERM U960 Paris, France ; Institut Jean Nicod, Ecole Normale Supérieure-EHESS, CNRS UMR-8129 Paris, France ; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London London, UK
| | - Nura Sidarus
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London London, UK
| | - Patrick Haggard
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Kumar N, Manjaly JA, Miyapuram KP. Feedback about action performed can alter the sense of self-agency. Front Psychol 2014; 5:145. [PMID: 24611059 PMCID: PMC3933776 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2013] [Accepted: 02/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Sense of agency refers to the sense of authorship of an action and its outcome. Sense of agency is often explained through computational models of motor control (e.g., the comparator model). Previous studies using the comparator model have manipulated action-outcome contingency to understand its effect on the sense of agency. More recent studies have shown that cues related to outcome, priming outcome and priming action have an effect on agency attribution. However, relatively few studies have focused on the effect of recalibrating internal predictions on the sense of agency. This study aims to investigate how feedback about action can recalibrate prediction and modulates the sense of agency. While participants performed a Flanker task, we manipulated the feedback about the validity of the action performed, independent of their responses. When true feedback is given, the sense of agency would reflect congruency between the sensory outcome and the action performed. The results show an opposite effect on the sense of agency when false feedback was given. We propose that feedback about action performed can recalibrate the prediction of sensory outcome and thus alter the sense of agency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neeraj Kumar
- Cognitive Science Program, Indian Institute of Technology Gandhinagar Ahmedabad, India
| | - Jaison A Manjaly
- Cognitive Science Program, Indian Institute of Technology Gandhinagar Ahmedabad, India
| | - Krishna P Miyapuram
- Cognitive Science Program, Indian Institute of Technology Gandhinagar Ahmedabad, India
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Damen TG, van Baaren RB, Dijksterhuis A. You should read this! Perceiving and acting upon action primes influences one's sense of agency. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
14
|
van der Weiden A, Aarts H, Ruys K. On The Nature of Experiencing Self-Agency: The Role of Goals and Primes in Inferring Oneself as the Cause of Behavior. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2013. [DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
15
|
Renes RA, Vermeulen L, Kahn RS, Aarts H, van Haren NEM. Abnormalities in the establishment of feeling of self-agency in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2013. [PMID: 23178108 DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND People usually feel they cause their own actions and the consequences of those actions, i.e., they attribute behavior to the proper agent. Research suggests that there are two routes to the experience of self-agency: 1) an explicit route, where one has the intention to obtain a goal (if it occurs, I must have done it) and 2) an implicit route, where information about the goal is unconsciously available and increases the feeling of self-agency. Schizophrenia patients typically experience no behavioral control and exhibit difficulties in distinguishing one's own actions from those of others. The present study investigates differences in both routes to self-agency experiences between schizophrenia patients and controls. METHODS Twenty-three schizophrenia patients and 23 controls performed a task where they performed an action (button press) and subsequently indicated whether or not they were the agent of the consequence of this action (the outcome) on a 9-point scale. The task can be manipulated to measure both the explicit and implicit route (by using priming) to the experience of self-agency. RESULTS In the explicit condition (participants intended to produce a specific outcome, and this outcome matched their goal), both groups experienced enhanced self-agency. In the implicit condition (the outcome matched the primed outcome), healthy controls showed increased self-agency over the outcome, while patients did not. Potential differences in task motivation and attention did not explain these findings. CONCLUSIONS These findings provide new evidence for the idea that implicit processes leading to feelings of self-agency may be disturbed in schizophrenia.
Collapse
|
16
|
Dannenberg L, Förster J, Jostmann NB. “If only…”: When counterfactual thoughts can reduce illusions of personal authorship. Conscious Cogn 2012; 21:456-63. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2011] [Revised: 11/04/2011] [Accepted: 11/24/2011] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
17
|
Marien H, Aarts H, Custers R. Being flexible or rigid in goal-directed behavior: When positive affect implicitly motivates the pursuit of goals or means. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.08.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
18
|
van der Weiden A, Aarts H, Ruys KI. Prime and probability: Causal knowledge affects inferential and predictive effects on self-agency experiences. Conscious Cogn 2011; 20:1865-71. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2011] [Revised: 08/23/2011] [Accepted: 09/07/2011] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
19
|
Dogge M, Schaap M, Custers R, Wegner DM, Aarts H. When moving without volition: implied self-causation enhances binding strength between involuntary actions and effects. Conscious Cogn 2011; 21:501-6. [PMID: 22115726 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.10.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/21/2011] [Revised: 10/18/2011] [Accepted: 10/30/2011] [Indexed: 10/15/2022]
Abstract
The conscious awareness of voluntary action is associated with systematic changes in time perception: The interval between actions and outcomes is experienced as compressed in time. Although this temporal binding is thought to result from voluntary movement and provides a window to the sense of agency, recent studies challenge this idea by demonstrating binding in involuntary movement. We offer a potential account for these findings by proposing that binding between involuntary actions and effects can occur when self-causation is implied. Participants made temporal judgements concerning a key press and a tone, while they learned to consider themselves as the cause of the effect or not. Results showed that implied self-causation (vs. no implied self-causation) increased temporal binding. Since intrinsic motor cues of movement were absent, these results suggest that sensory evidence about the key press caused binding in retrospect and in line with the participant's sense of being an agent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Myrthel Dogge
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, PO Box 80 140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
The concept of an ability to make choices and to determine one’s own outcomes fits well with experiences that most people have, and these experiences form the basis for beliefs in free will. However, the existence of conscious free will is challenged by modern research findings highlighting the unconscious origins of goal-directed behavior that gives rise to free-will beliefs. This report expands on these insights by revealing that both conscious and unconscious processes play an important role in free-will beliefs. Specifically, Experiment 1 demonstrates that free-will beliefs are strengthened when conscious intentions to produce action outcomes bind the perception of action and outcome together in time. Experiment 2 shows that these beliefs are strengthened when unconscious priming of action outcomes creates illusory experiences of self-agency when the primed outcomes occur. Together, these findings suggest that beliefs in free will are associated with self-agency and are enhanced by both conscious and unconscious information processing of goal-directed behavior.
Collapse
|
21
|
Belayachi S, Van der Linden M. Feeling of doing in obsessive–compulsive checking. Conscious Cogn 2010; 19:534-46. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2009] [Revised: 01/22/2010] [Accepted: 02/02/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|