1
|
Konrad JD, Lohse KR, Marrus N, Lang CE. Trial-to-trial motor behavior during a reinforcement learning task in children ages 6 to 12. Hum Mov Sci 2024; 99:103317. [PMID: 39667095 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2024.103317] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2024] [Revised: 11/14/2024] [Accepted: 12/04/2024] [Indexed: 12/14/2024]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION During practice, learners use available feedback from one trial to develop and implement motor commands for the next trial. Unsuccessful trials (i.e., "misses") should be followed by different motor behavior (e.g., goal-directed changes and/or exploration of movement parameters), while successful trials (i.e., "hits") should maintain the same behavior (e.g., minimize variance and recapitulate the same motor plan to the best of one's ability). Measuring the trial-to-trial changes in motor behavior can provide insights into how the motor system uses feedback and regulates movement variability while trying to improve performance. There have been no reports on the trial-to-trial motor behavior of typically developing children despite the profound motor development that occurs in this period and its relevance to long-term functional outcomes. METHODS We recruited 72 typically developing children from ages 6 to 12 to perform a reinforcement learning beanbag toss to a target. Their target errors were used to examine their motor exploration and autocorrelation. RESULTS Comparing variability at different trial-to-trial intervals showed that children exhibit motor exploration above and beyond the effect of sampling bias. Mean autocorrelations of different lags were near zero suggesting that successive trials were largely unrelated. CONCLUSION We found evidence that children utilize motor exploration in the target space of a target throwing task. After failed trials they exhibited increased variability to search for more optimal motor solutions. After successes, they minimized variability to create the same successful performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey D Konrad
- Program in Physical Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, CB 8502, 4444 Forest Park Ave., Suite 1101, St. Louis, MO 63108-2212, United States
| | - Keith R Lohse
- Program in Physical Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, CB 8502, 4444 Forest Park Ave., Suite 1101, St. Louis, MO 63108-2212, United States; Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, MSC 8111-29-9000, 660 S. Euclid Ave., St. Louis, MO 63110, United States
| | - Natasha Marrus
- Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, CB 8509, 660 South Euclid Ave., St. Louis, MO 63110, United States
| | - Catherine E Lang
- Program in Physical Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, CB 8502, 4444 Forest Park Ave., Suite 1101, St. Louis, MO 63108-2212, United States; Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, MSC 8505-66-1, 4444 Forest Park Avenue, St. Louis, MO 63108-2212, United States; Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, MSC 8111-29-9000, 660 S. Euclid Ave., St. Louis, MO 63110, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Park SW, Cardinaux A, Crozier D, Russo M, Kjelgaard M, Sinha P, Sternad D. Developmental change in predictive motor abilities. iScience 2023; 26:106038. [PMID: 36824276 PMCID: PMC9941204 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.106038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2022] [Revised: 12/14/2022] [Accepted: 01/17/2023] [Indexed: 01/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Prediction is critical for successful interactions with a dynamic environment. To test the development of predictive processes over the life span, we designed a suite of interceptive tasks implemented as interactive video games. Four tasks involving interactions with a flying ball with titrated challenge quantified spatiotemporal aspects of prediction. For comparison, reaction time was assessed in a matching task. The experiments were conducted in a museum, where over 400 visitors across all ages participated, and in a laboratory with a focused age group. Results consistently showed that predictive ability improved with age to reach adult level by age 12. In contrast, reaction time continued to decrease into late adolescence. Inter-task correlations revealed that the tasks tested different aspects of predictive processes. This developmental progression complements recent findings on cerebellar and cortical maturation. Additionally, these results can serve as normative data to study predictive processes in individuals with neurodevelopmental conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Se-Woong Park
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX 78249, USA
- Department of Biology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
| | - Annie Cardinaux
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
| | - Dena Crozier
- Department of Biology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Physics, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Cleveland Clinic Lerner College of Medicine, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH 44195, USA
| | - Marta Russo
- Department of Neurology, Tor Vergata Polyclinc, Rome, Italy
- Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| | - Margaret Kjelgaard
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Bridgewater State University, Bridgewater, MA 02325, USA
| | - Pawan Sinha
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
| | - Dagmar Sternad
- Department of Biology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Physics, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bitu F, Galinon-Mélénec B, Molina M. Modifying Sensory Afferences on Tablet Changes Originality in Drawings. Front Psychol 2022; 13:806093. [PMID: 35774942 PMCID: PMC9239698 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.806093] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2021] [Accepted: 05/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
According to some recent empirical studies revealing that creativity is linked to sensorimotor components, the current research was aimed at evaluating whether sensory afferences could modulate originality in drawing of children and adolescents. Sixty-nine children from 1st, 3rd, 6th, and 8th grades were required to produce a man who exists and a man who doesn’t exist with fingers or stylus on a tablet and with a pen on paper. Drawings were assessed with an originality scale comparing original drawings to unoriginal ones. Since, in comparison to drawings made on paper with a pen, drawing with fingers enhances proprioceptive information, this condition was expected, according to cognitive load theory, to favor originality in drawing by reducing cognitive resources devoted to motor control of the graphic gesture (lowering intrinsic load). On the contrary, since the use of a stylus involves a proprioceptive loss of information, which enhances intrinsic load by increasing cognitive resources devoted to motor control, it was expected that drawing with a stylus on the tablet would lead to the least original drawings. Results only partially confirmed these hypotheses. While the use of fingers on the tablet led to the highest original scores, using a stylus on the tablet did not impair originality in drawing of children and adolescents. On the opposite, the use of a stylus led 3rd–8th graders to perform better than with pen on paper. This modulation of the tool on originality does not confirm the hypotheses formulated in accordance with the cognitive load framework. However, it could be explained according to an embodied perspective of creativity considering the creative process as relying on a sensorimotor prediction process in which sensory afferences are central to generating and evaluate creative ideas. This research opens new avenues on creativity and proposes to consider the development of predictive motor control as a significant part of creativity development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fabien Bitu
- Laboratory of Psychology of Caen Normandy (LPCN EA7452), University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- *Correspondence: Fabien Bitu,
| | - Béatrice Galinon-Mélénec
- Identity and Differentiation of Spaces, Environment and Societies (UMR IDEES 6266 CNRS), University Le Havre Normandy, Le Havre, France
| | - Michèle Molina
- Laboratory of Psychology of Caen Normandy (LPCN EA7452), University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Chua YW, Lu S, Anzulewicz A, Sobota K, Tachtatzis C, Andonovic I, Rowe P, Delafield‐Butt J. Developmental differences in the prospective organisation of goal-directed movement between children with autism and typically developing children: A smart tablet serious game study. Dev Sci 2022; 25:e13195. [PMID: 34800316 PMCID: PMC9287065 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2021] [Revised: 10/15/2021] [Accepted: 11/15/2021] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
Movement is prospective. It structures self-generated engagement with objects and social partners and is fundamental to children's learning and development. In autistic children, previous reports of differences in movement kinematics compared to neurotypical peers suggest that its prospective organisation might be disrupted. Here, we employed a smart tablet serious game paradigm to assess differences in the feedforward and feedback mechanisms of prospective action organisation, between autistic and neurotypical preschool children. We analysed 3926 goal-directed finger movements made during smart-tablet ecological gameplay, from 28 children with Childhood Autism (ICD-10; ASD) and 43 neurotypical children (TD), aged 3-6 years old. Using linear and generalised linear mixed-effect models, we found the ASD group executed movements with longer movement time (MT) and time to peak velocity (TTPV), lower peak velocity (PV), with PV less likely to occur in the first movement unit (MU) and with a greater number of movement units after peak velocity (MU-APV). Interestingly, compared to the TD group, the ASD group showed smaller increases in PV, TTPV and MT with an increase in age (ASD × age interaction), together with a smaller reduction in MU-APV and an increase in MU-APV at shorter target distances (ASD × Dist interaction). Our results are the first to highlight different developmental trends in anticipatory feedforward and compensatory feedback mechanisms of control, contributing to differences in movement kinematics observed between autistic and neurotypical children. These findings point to differences in integration of prospective perceptuomotor information, with implications for embodied cognition and learning from self-generated action in autism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yu Wei Chua
- Laboratory for Innovation in AutismUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
- Faculty of Humanities and Social SciencesUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
| | - Szu‐Ching Lu
- Laboratory for Innovation in AutismUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
- Faculty of Humanities and Social SciencesUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
| | | | | | - Christos Tachtatzis
- Laboratory for Innovation in AutismUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
- Department of Electronic and Electrical EngineeringUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
| | - Ivan Andonovic
- Laboratory for Innovation in AutismUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
- Department of Electronic and Electrical EngineeringUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
| | - Philip Rowe
- Laboratory for Innovation in AutismUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
- Department of Biomedical EngineeringUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
| | - Jonathan Delafield‐Butt
- Laboratory for Innovation in AutismUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
- Faculty of Humanities and Social SciencesUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowScotlandUK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Maimon-Mor RO, Schone HR, Henderson Slater D, Faisal AA, Makin TR. Early life experience sets hard limits on motor learning as evidenced from artificial arm use. eLife 2021; 10:66320. [PMID: 34605407 PMCID: PMC8523152 DOI: 10.7554/elife.66320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2021] [Accepted: 10/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The study of artificial arms provides a unique opportunity to address long-standing questions on sensorimotor plasticity and development. Learning to use an artificial arm arguably depends on fundamental building blocks of body representation and would therefore be impacted by early life experience. We tested artificial arm motor-control in two adult populations with upper-limb deficiencies: a congenital group—individuals who were born with a partial arm, and an acquired group—who lost their arm following amputation in adulthood. Brain plasticity research teaches us that the earlier we train to acquire new skills (or use a new technology) the better we benefit from this practice as adults. Instead, we found that although the congenital group started using an artificial arm as toddlers, they produced increased error noise and directional errors when reaching to visual targets, relative to the acquired group who performed similarly to controls. However, the earlier an individual with a congenital limb difference was fitted with an artificial arm, the better their motor control was. Since we found no group differences when reaching without visual feedback, we suggest that the ability to perform efficient visual-based corrective movements is highly dependent on either biological or artificial arm experience at a very young age. Subsequently, opportunities for sensorimotor plasticity become more limited.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roni O Maimon-Mor
- WIN Centre, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom.,Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Hunter R Schone
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom.,Laboratory of Brain & Cognition, NIMH, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, United States
| | | | - A Aldo Faisal
- Departments of Bioengineering and of Computing, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Tamar R Makin
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Miehlbradt J, Cuturi LF, Zanchi S, Gori M, Micera S. Immersive virtual reality interferes with default head-trunk coordination strategies in young children. Sci Rep 2021; 11:17959. [PMID: 34580325 PMCID: PMC8476578 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-96866-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2021] [Accepted: 08/10/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The acquisition of postural control is an elaborate process, which relies on the balanced integration of multisensory inputs. Current models suggest that young children rely on an 'en-block' control of their upper body before sequentially acquiring a segmental control around the age of 7, and that they resort to the former strategy under challenging conditions. While recent works suggest that a virtual sensory environment alters visuomotor integration in healthy adults, little is known about the effects on younger individuals. Here we show that this default coordination pattern is disrupted by an immersive virtual reality framework where a steering role is assigned to the trunk, which causes 6- to 8-year-olds to employ an ill-adapted segmental strategy. These results provide an alternate trajectory of motor development and emphasize the immaturity of postural control at these ages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenifer Miehlbradt
- Bertarelli Foundation Chair in Translational Neuroengineering, Center for Neuroprosthetics, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, 1202, Geneva, Switzerland. .,Brain Electrophysiology Attention Movement Laboratory, Institute of Psychology, Université de Lausanne, 1015, Lausanne, Switzerland.
| | - Luigi F Cuturi
- Unit for Visually Impaired People, Center for Human Technologies, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 16152, Genova, Italy
| | - Silvia Zanchi
- Unit for Visually Impaired People, Center for Human Technologies, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 16152, Genova, Italy.,Robotics Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Center for Human Technologies, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 16152, Genova, Italy.,DIBRIS Department, Università di Genova, 16145, Genova, Italy
| | - Monica Gori
- Unit for Visually Impaired People, Center for Human Technologies, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 16152, Genova, Italy
| | - Silvestro Micera
- Bertarelli Foundation Chair in Translational Neuroengineering, Center for Neuroprosthetics, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, 1202, Geneva, Switzerland.,The Biorobotics Institute and Department of Excellence in Robotics and AI, Scuola Superiore Sant'Anna, 56025, Pontedera, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Havaei N, Mahmodzadeh M, Azad A, Meimandi M, Rezaei M, Kahjoogh MA, Mohammadi A. Handwriting status among Iranian primary school students: a cross-sectional study. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THERAPY AND REHABILITATION 2021. [DOI: 10.12968/ijtr.2020.0007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
Background/Aims Handwriting difficulties among school students are the primary reason for referral to school-based occupational therapy. Appropriate criteria and a complete view of the students' handwriting status are lacking in Iran. The purpose of this study was to gather comprehensive information about handwriting issues for therapists and related disciplines in Iran. Methods In this cross-sectional study, the handwriting status of 1262 Persian-language elementary school students (grades 2 and 3) from governmental schools was surveyed using the Persian handwriting assessment tool. Results There were statistically significant differences in handwriting components between second and third grade students except for word size and text slant. Handwriting components were different between boys and girls, and between right- and left-handed writers. Regression analysis revealed a positive relationships between age and grade and most of the handwriting components. Conclusions The results of this study have implications for teachers, therapists and other specialists who work and research on handwriting status of typically healthy students and students who are disabled in Iran. Handwriting skills improve over time and it seems that girls and right-handed writers are better writers regarding legibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Naser Havaei
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran
| | - Mahdieh Mahmodzadeh
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran
| | - Akram Azad
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Mahsa Meimandi
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Mandana Rezaei
- Department of Physiotherapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran
| | - Mina Ahmadi Kahjoogh
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran
| | - Ahmad Mohammadi
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Martel M, Finos L, Koun E, Farnè A, Roy AC. The long developmental trajectory of body representation plasticity following tool use. Sci Rep 2021; 11:559. [PMID: 33436755 PMCID: PMC7804961 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-79476-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2020] [Accepted: 12/04/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans evolution is distinctly characterized by their exquisite mastery of tools, allowing them to shape their environment in more elaborate ways compared to other species. This ability is present ever since infancy and most theories indicate that children become proficient with tool use very early. In adults, tool use has been shown to plastically modify metric aspects of the arm representation, as indexed by changes in movement kinematics. To date, whether and when the plastic capability of updating the body representation develops during childhood remains unknown. This question is particularly important since body representation plasticity could be impacted by the fact that the human body takes years to achieve a stable metric configuration. Here we assessed the kinematics of 90 young participants (8-21 years old) required to reach for an object before and after tool use, as a function of their pubertal development. Results revealed that tool incorporation, as indexed by the adult typical kinematic pattern, develops very slowly and displays a u-shaped developmental trajectory. From early to mid puberty, the changes in kinematics following tool use seem to reflect a shortened arm representation, opposite to what was previously reported in adults. This pattern starts reversing after mid puberty, which is characterized by the lack of any kinematics change following tool use. The typical adult-like pattern emerges only at late puberty, when body size is stable. These findings reveal the complex dynamics of tool incorporation across development, possibly indexing the transition from a vision-based to a proprioception-based body representation plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie Martel
- Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage, CNRS UMR5596, Lyon, France.
- University of Lyon II, Lyon, France.
- Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team - ImpAct, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS U5292, Lyon, France.
- University UCBL Lyon 1, University of Lyon, Villeurbanne, France.
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway University of London, Egham Hill, Surrey, Egham, TW20 0EX, UK.
| | - Livio Finos
- Department of Statistical Sciences, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| | - Eric Koun
- Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team - ImpAct, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS U5292, Lyon, France
- University UCBL Lyon 1, University of Lyon, Villeurbanne, France
| | - Alessandro Farnè
- Integrative Multisensory Perception Action and Cognition Team - ImpAct, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS U5292, Lyon, France
- University UCBL Lyon 1, University of Lyon, Villeurbanne, France
- Hospices Civils de Lyon, Mouvement Et Handicap and Neuro-Immersion, Lyon, France
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Trento, Italy
| | - Alice Catherine Roy
- Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage, CNRS UMR5596, Lyon, France
- University of Lyon II, Lyon, France
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Trento, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Van der Looven R, Deschrijver M, Hermans L, De Muynck M, Vingerhoets G. Hand size representation in healthy children and young adults. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 203:105016. [PMID: 33246254 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2020] [Revised: 09/21/2020] [Accepted: 09/22/2020] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Whereas we experience our body as a coherent volumetric object, the brain appears to maintain highly fragmented representations of individual body parts. Little is known about how body representations of hand size and shape are built and evolve during infancy and young adulthood. This study aimed to investigate the effect of hand side, handedness, and age on the development of central hand size representation. The observational study with comparison groups was conducted with 90 typically developing Belgian school children and young adults (48 male and 42 female; age range = 5.0-23.0 years; 49 left-handed and 41 right-handed). Participants estimated their hand size and shape using two different tasks. In the localization task, participants were verbally cued to judge the locations of 10 anatomical landmarks of an occluded hand. An implicit hand size map was constructed and compared with actual hand dimensions. In the template selection task, the explicit hand shape was measured with a depictive method. Hand shape indexes were calculated and compared for the actual, implicit, and explicit conditions. Participants were divided into four age groups (5-8 years, 9-10 years, 11-16 years, and 17-23 years). Implicit hand maps featured underestimation of finger length and overestimation of hand width, which is already present in the youngest children. Linear mixed modeling revealed no influence of hand side on finger length underestimation; nonetheless, a significant main effect of age (p = .001) was exposed. Sinistrals aged 11 to 16 years showed significantly less underestimation (p = .03) than dextrals of the same age. As for the hand shape, the implicit condition differed significantly with the actual and explicit conditions (p < .001). Again, the implicit shape index was subjected to handedness and age effects, with significant differences being found between sinistrals and dextrals in the age groups of 9 and 10 years (p = .029) and 11 to 16 years (p < .001). In conclusion, the implicit metric component of the hand representation in children and young adults is misperceived, featuring shortened fingers and broadened hands since a very young age. Crucially, the finger length underestimation increases with age and shows a different developmental trajectory for sinistrals and dextrals. In contrast, the explicit hand shape is approximately veridical and seems immune from age and handedness effects. This study confirms the dual character of somatoperception and establishes a point of reference for children and young adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth Van der Looven
- Child Rehabilitation Centre, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Ghent University Hospital, 9000 Ghent, Belgium.
| | - Miguel Deschrijver
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Ghent University Hospital, 9000 Ghent, Belgium
| | - Linda Hermans
- Child Rehabilitation Centre, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Ghent University Hospital, 9000 Ghent, Belgium
| | - Martine De Muynck
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Ghent University Hospital, 9000 Ghent, Belgium
| | - Guy Vingerhoets
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Ghent University, 9000 Ghent, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Almotairy N, Kumar A, Welander N, Grigoriadis A. Age-related changes in oral motor-control strategies during unpredictable load demands in humans. Eur J Oral Sci 2020; 128:299-307. [PMID: 32749023 DOI: 10.1111/eos.12721] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2020] [Revised: 06/04/2020] [Accepted: 06/19/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
To investigate age-related changes in oral motor strategies in response to unpredictable load demands. Sixty-five healthy children (aged 3-17 yr) were divided into five age-groups based on their dental eruption stages and compared with a group of healthy adults (aged 18-35 yr). Each participant was asked to perform a standardized motor control task involving 'pulling' and 'holding' a force transducer with the anterior teeth. Different loads were attached to the force transducer in an unpredictable manner. The temporal force profile was divided into two time-segments (an initial segment and a later segment). The peak force and peak force rate during the initial time-segment, and the holding force and intra-trial variability (coefficient of variation) during the later time-segment, were measured. The results showed no differences in the peak force, peak force rate, holding force, and force variability in children compared with adults. However, the trends in the data evaluated using a segmented regression analysis showed that a breakpoint (abrupt change) consistently occurred in the late-mixed dentition group (age 9-11 yr) for most of the outcome variables. The results indicate that while the motor control strategies in children appear to be similar to those in adults, there is a shift in the oral motor developmental trend during the late-mixed dentition stage.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nabeel Almotairy
- Unit of Oral Rehabilitation, Department of Dental Medicine, Division of Oral Diagnostics and Rehabilitation, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.,SCON, Scandinavian Center for Orofacial Neurosciences, Huddinge, Sweden.,Department of Preventive Dentistry, Division of Orthodontics, Qassim University, Buraidah, Saudi Arabia
| | - Abhishek Kumar
- Unit of Oral Rehabilitation, Department of Dental Medicine, Division of Oral Diagnostics and Rehabilitation, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.,SCON, Scandinavian Center for Orofacial Neurosciences, Huddinge, Sweden
| | - Nadia Welander
- Unit of Oral Rehabilitation, Department of Dental Medicine, Division of Oral Diagnostics and Rehabilitation, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Anastasios Grigoriadis
- Unit of Oral Rehabilitation, Department of Dental Medicine, Division of Oral Diagnostics and Rehabilitation, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden.,SCON, Scandinavian Center for Orofacial Neurosciences, Huddinge, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Gehringer JE, Arpin DJ, VerMaas JR, Trevarrow MP, Wilson TW, Kurz MJ. The Strength of the Movement-related Somatosensory Cortical Oscillations Differ between Adolescents and Adults. Sci Rep 2019; 9:18520. [PMID: 31811232 PMCID: PMC6898653 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-55004-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2019] [Accepted: 11/22/2019] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Adolescents demonstrate increasing mastery of motor actions with age. One prevailing hypothesis is that maturation of the somatosensory system during adolescence contributes to the improved motor control. However, limited efforts have been made to determine if somatosensory cortical processing is different in adolescents during movement. In this study, we used magnetoencephalographic brain imaging to begin addressing this knowledge gap by applying an electrical stimulation to the tibial nerve as adolescents (Age = 14.8 ± 2.5 yrs.) and adults (Age = 36.8 ± 5.0 yrs.) produced an isometric ankle plantarflexion force, or sat with no motor activity. Our results showed strong somatosensory cortical oscillations for both conditions in the alpha-beta (8–30 Hz) and gamma (38–80 Hz) ranges that occurred immediately after the stimulation (0–125 ms), and a beta (18–26 Hz) oscillatory response shortly thereafter (300–400 ms). Compared with the passive condition, all of these frequency specific cortical oscillations were attenuated while producing the ankle force. The attenuation of the alpha-beta response was greater in adolescents, while the adults had a greater attenuation of the beta response. These results imply that altered attenuation of the somatosensory cortical oscillations might be central to the under-developed somatosensory processing and motor performance characteristics in adolescents.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James E Gehringer
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.,Department Physical Therapy, Munroe Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - David J Arpin
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.,Department Physical Therapy, Munroe Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Jacy R VerMaas
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.,Department Physical Therapy, Munroe Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Michael P Trevarrow
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.,Department Physical Therapy, Munroe Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Tony W Wilson
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.,Department of Neurological Sciences, UNMC, Omaha, Nebraska, United States
| | - Max J Kurz
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States. .,Department Physical Therapy, Munroe Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Domellöf E, Bäckström A, Johansson AM, Rönnqvist L, von Hofsten C, Rosander K. Kinematic characteristics of second-order motor planning and performance in 6- and 10-year-old children and adults: Effects of age and task constraints. Dev Psychobiol 2019; 62:250-265. [PMID: 31502277 PMCID: PMC7064938 DOI: 10.1002/dev.21911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2019] [Revised: 07/02/2019] [Accepted: 07/27/2019] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
This study explored age-related differences in motor planning as expressed in arm-hand kinematics during a sequential peg moving task with varying demands on goal insertion complexity (second-order planning). The peg was a vertical cylinder with either a circular or semicircular base. The task was to transport the peg between two positions and rotate it various amounts horizontally before fitting into its final position. The amount of rotation required was either 0°, 90°, 180°, or -90°. The reaching for the peg, the displacement of it, and the way the rotation was accomplished was analyzed. Assessments of end state comfort, goal interpretation errors, and type of grip used were also included. Participants were two groups of typically developing children, one younger (Mage = 6.7 years) and one older (Mage = 10.3 years), and one adult group (Mage = 34.9 years). The children, particularly 6-year-olds, displayed less efficient prehensile movement organization than adults. Related to less efficient motor planning, 6-year-olds, mainly, had shorter reach-to-grasp onset latencies, higher velocities, and shorter time to peak velocities, and longer grasp durations than adults. Importantly, the adults rotated the peg during transport. In contrast, the children made corrective rotations after the hand had arrived at the goal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erik Domellöf
- Department of Psychology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Angulo-Barroso R, Ferrer-Uris B, Busquets A. Enhancing Children's Motor Memory Retention Through Acute Intense Exercise: Effects of Different Exercise Durations. Front Psychol 2019; 10:2000. [PMID: 31555181 PMCID: PMC6722207 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02000] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2019] [Accepted: 08/15/2019] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Physical exercise has been proposed as a viable means to stimulate motor learning. Exercise characteristics, including intensity and duration, may play a role in modulating the exercise effect on motor learning. While some evidence exists regarding the benefits of intense and relatively long exercise, little is known about the effect of short exercise bouts on motor learning, especially in children. This study aimed to assess the effect of long versus short intense exercise bouts on the adaptation and consolidation of a rotational visuomotor adaptation task. The participants were 71 healthy children from two sites divided into three groups: long exercise bout (LONG), short exercise bout (SHORT), and no exercise (CON). Children performed a rotated (clockwise 60° rotation) motor task on four different occasions: an adaptation set and 1 h, 24 h, and 7 days delayed retention sets. Exercise bouts were performed prior to the adaptation set. Results showed a group effect during motor adaptation [F(2,68) = 3.160; p = 0.049; η p 2 = 0.087], but no statistical differences were found between groups. Regarding retention tests, both exercise groups (LONG and SHORT) showed superior retention compared to CON group [F(2,68) = 7.102; p = 0.002; η p 2 = 0.175]. No differences were found between exercise groups, indicating similar benefits for the two exercise interventions. Overall, whether the exercise duration was long or short, exercise improved motor memory retention as an estimate of memory consolidation process. The use of short exercise bouts may be suitable to improve children's motor memory consolidation in environments where time constraints exist.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rosa Angulo-Barroso
- Institut Nacional d’Educació Física de Catalunya (INEFC), Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
- Department of Kinesiology, California State University, Northridge, CA, United States
| | - Blai Ferrer-Uris
- Institut Nacional d’Educació Física de Catalunya (INEFC), Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Albert Busquets
- Institut Nacional d’Educació Física de Catalunya (INEFC), Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
The capacity to learn new motor and perceptual calibrations develops concurrently in childhood. Sci Rep 2019; 9:9322. [PMID: 31249379 PMCID: PMC6597729 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-45074-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2019] [Accepted: 05/29/2019] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Learning new movements through an error-based process called motor adaptation is thought to involve multiple mechanisms which are still largely not understood. Previous studies have shown that young children adapt movement more slowly than adults, perhaps supporting the involvement of distinct neural circuits that come online at different stages of development. Recent studies in adults have shown that in addition to recalibrating a movement, motor adaptation also leads to changes in the perception of that movement. However, we do not yet understand the relationship between the processes that underlie motor and perceptual recalibration. Here we studied motor and perceptual recalibration with split-belt walking adaptation in adults and children aged 6-8 years. Consistent with previous work, we found that this group of children adapted their walking patterns more slowly than adults, though individual children ranged from slow to adult-like in their adaptation rates. Perceptual recalibration was also reduced in the same group of children compared to adults, with individual children ranging from having no recalibration to having adult-like recalibration. In sum, faster motor adaptation and the ability to recalibrate movement perception both come online within a similar age-range, raising the possibility that the same sensorimotor mechanisms underlie these processes.
Collapse
|
15
|
Gehringer JE, Arpin DJ, Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW, Kurz MJ. Practice modulates motor-related beta oscillations differently in adolescents and adults. J Physiol 2019; 597:3203-3216. [PMID: 31045245 PMCID: PMC7105901 DOI: 10.1113/jp277326] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2018] [Accepted: 04/18/2019] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
KEY POINTS Magnetoencephalography data were acquired during a leg force task in pre-/post-practice sessions in adolescents and adults. Strong peri-movement alpha and beta oscillations were mapped to the cortex. Following practice, performance improved and beta oscillations were altered. Beta oscillations decreased in the sensorimotor cortex in adolescents after practice, but increased in adults. No pre-/post-practice differences were detected for alpha oscillations. ABSTRACT There is considerable evidence that there are motor performance and practice differences between adolescents and adults. Behavioural studies have suggested that these motor performance differences are simply due to experience. However, the neurophysiological nexus for these motor performance differences remains unknown. The present study investigates the short-term changes (e.g. fast motor learning) in the alpha and beta event-related desynchronizations (ERDs) associated with practising an ankle plantarflexion motor action. To this end, we utilized magnetoencephalography to identify changes in the alpha and beta ERDs in healthy adolescents (n = 21; age = 14 ± 2.1 years) and middle-aged adults (n = 22; age = 36.6 ± 5 years) after practising an isometric ankle plantarflexion target-matching task. After practice, all of the participants matched more targets and matched the targets faster, and had improved accuracy, faster reaction times and faster force production. However, the motor performance of the adults exceeded what was seen in the adolescents regardless of practice. In conjunction with the behavioural results, the strength of the beta ERDs across the motor planning and execution stages was reduced after practice in the sensorimotor cortices of the adolescents, but was stronger in the adults. No pre-/post-practice changes were found in the alpha ERDs. These outcomes suggest that there are age-dependent changes in the sensorimotor cortical oscillations after practising a motor task. We suspect that these noted differences might be related to familiarity with the motor task, GABA levels and/or maturational differences in the integrity of the white matter fibre tracts that comprise the respective cortical areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James E Gehringer
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
- Department of Physical Therapy, Munroe-Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
| | - David J Arpin
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
- Department of Physical Therapy, Munroe-Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
| | - Elizabeth Heinrichs-Graham
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
- Department of Neurological Sciences, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
| | - Tony W Wilson
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
- Department of Neurological Sciences, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
| | - Max J Kurz
- Center for Magnetoencephalography, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
- Department of Physical Therapy, Munroe-Meyer Institute, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Mani H, Miyagishima S, Kozuka N, Kodama Y, Takeda K, Asaka T. Development of postural control during single-leg standing in children aged 3-10 years. Gait Posture 2019; 68:174-180. [PMID: 30497037 DOI: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.11.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2018] [Revised: 08/25/2018] [Accepted: 11/17/2018] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The ability to control the center of mass (COM) during single-leg standing (SLS) is imperative for individuals to walk independently. However, detailed biomechanical features of postural control during SLS performed by children remain to be comprehensively investigated. RESEARCH QUESTION We aimed to investigate the development of postural control during SLS in children aged 3-10 years. METHODS Forty-eight healthy children (26 boys and 22 girls) aged 3-10 years and 11 young adults participated in this experiment. The child population was divided into four groups by age: 3-4, 5-6, 7-8, and 9-10 years. The SLS task included standing on a single leg as long and as steady as possible for up to 30 s. A three-dimensional motion capture system and two force plates were used for calculating the COM and center of pressure (COP). The task was divided into three phases (accelerated, decelerated, and steady) on the basis of the relationship between COM and COP. RESULTS COP-COM distances in the 5-6 years' and 7-8 years' groups were significantly increased during the acceleration phase when compared with those in the adult group. Furthermore, COP-COM distances during the decelerated phase were significantly higher in all children's groups compared with those in the adult group. Lastly, COP-COM distance during the steady phase was significantly higher in the 3-4 year age group than in the 9-10 year age and adults groups. SIGNIFICANCE These results suggest that postural control during the acceleration and steady phases mature by 9 years. Conversely, children ∼10 years did not attain adult-like levels of postural control during the decelerated phase. The developmental process for postural control at each phase possibly plays a significant role in the basic biomechanics of movement and does not display a monotonic pattern.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiroki Mani
- Department of Rehabilitation Science, Faculty of Health Sciences, Hokkaido University. Sapporo, N12-W5, Kita-Ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-0812, Japan.
| | - Saori Miyagishima
- Division of Rehabilitation, Sapporo Medical University Hospital, Sapporo Medical University, S1-W16, Chuo-Ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-8543, Japan.
| | - Naoki Kozuka
- Department of Physical Therapy, School of Health Sciences, Sapporo Medical University, Sapporo, S1-W17, Chuo-Ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-8556, Japan.
| | - Yuya Kodama
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Hokkaido University, N12-W5, Kita-Ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-0812, Japan.
| | - Kenta Takeda
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Hokkaido University, N12-W5, Kita-Ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-0812, Japan.
| | - Tadayoshi Asaka
- Department of Rehabilitation Science, Faculty of Health Sciences, Hokkaido University. Sapporo, N12-W5, Kita-Ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido, 060-0812, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Kim S, Jackson GM, Dyke K, Jackson SR. Impaired forward model updating in young adults with Tourette syndrome. Brain 2018; 142:209-219. [DOI: 10.1093/brain/awy306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2018] [Accepted: 10/12/2018] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Soyoung Kim
- School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, UK
- Institute of Mental Health, University of Nottingham, UK
| | - Georgina M Jackson
- School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, UK
- Institute of Mental Health, University of Nottingham, UK
| | - Katherine Dyke
- Institute of Mental Health, University of Nottingham, UK
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, UK
| | - Stephen R Jackson
- Institute of Mental Health, University of Nottingham, UK
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Taylor S, McLean B, Falkmer T, Carey LM, Girdler S, Elliott C, Blair E. Assessing body sensations in children: Intra-rater reliability of assessment and effects of age. Br J Occup Ther 2018. [DOI: 10.1177/0308022618786933] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Introduction This article examines the effect of age and gender on somatosensory capacity for children and adolescents, and provides preliminary normative data and reliability for the SenScreen© Kids, a new standardised measure of touch, wrist position sense and haptic object recognition. Method A cross-sectional study of 88 typically developing children aged 6–15 years (mean 10.3 years; SD 2.6 years) was used to determine the developmental effects of age and gender on somatosensory capacity. Intra-rater reliability was assessed in 22 of the 88 participants at two time points (mean 8.8 years; SD 2.6 years). Results Statistically significant differences were observed between age groups for tactile discrimination, wrist position sense and haptic object recognition, but not for touch registration for which all except one participant achieved a maximum score. There was no effect of gender. Three of four SenScreen Kids subtests demonstrated good intra-rater agreement between time points. Conclusions Somatosensory capacity increased with age for typically developing children aged 6–15 years. Three subtests of the SenScreen Kids demonstrated good intra-rater reliability with typically developing children. Further investigation of reliability is required, and all subtests require psychometric testing with clinical populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan Taylor
- Sessional academic, Senior occupational therapist, School of Occupational Therapy and Social Work, Curtin University, Australia; PhD candidate, Department of Paediatric Rehabilitation, Perth Children's Hospital, Australia
| | - Belinda McLean
- PhD candidate, Senior occupational therapist, School of Paediatrics and Child Health, University of Western Australia, Australia; Research associate, Department of Paediatric Rehabilitation, Perth Children's Hospital, Australia
| | - Torbjorn Falkmer
- Dean of Research at the Faculty of Health Sciences, School of Occupational Therapy and Social Work, Curtin University, Australia
| | - Leeanne M Carey
- Professor in Occupational Therapy – Discipline Lead, School of Allied Health, La Trobe University, Australia; Founding Head, Neurorehabilitation and Recovery, The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Australia
| | - Sonya Girdler
- Professor at the Faculty of Health Sciences, School of Occupational Therapy and Social Work, Curtin University, Australia; Research associate, Population Sciences, Telethon Kids Institute, Australia
| | - Catherine Elliott
- Chair of Kids Rehab Western Australia, Department of Paediatric Rehabilitation, Perth Children's Hospital, Australia; Professor, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| | - Eve Blair
- Adjunct Associate Professor, Population Sciences, Telethon Kids Institute, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Golenia L, Schoemaker MM, Otten E, Tuitert I, Bongers RM. The development of consistency and flexibility in manual pointing during middle childhood. Dev Psychobiol 2018; 60:511-519. [PMID: 29785799 DOI: 10.1002/dev.21741] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2017] [Revised: 04/03/2018] [Accepted: 04/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Goal-directed actions become truly functional and skilled when they are consistent yet flexible. In manual pointing, end-effector consistency is characterized by the end position of the index fingertip, whereas flexibility in movement execution is captured by the use of abundant arm-joint configurations not affecting the index finger end position. Because adults have been shown to exploit their system's flexibility in challenging conditions, we wondered whether during middle childhood children are already able to exploit motor flexibility when demanded by the situation. We had children aged 5-10 years and adults perform pointing movements in a nonchallenging and challenging condition. Results showed that end-effector errors and flexibility in movement execution decreased with age. Importantly, only the 9-10-year-olds and adults showed increased flexibility in the challenging condition. Thus, while consistency increases and flexibility decreases during mid-childhood development, from the age of nine children appear able to employ more flexibility with increasing task demands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Golenia
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Marina M Schoemaker
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Egbert Otten
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Inge Tuitert
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
- CNRS, Institut des Sciences du Mouvement, Aix-Marseille University, Marseille, France
| | - Raoul M Bongers
- Center for Human Movement Sciences, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Guilbert J, Jouen F, Molina M. Motor imagery development and proprioceptive integration: Which sensory reweighting during childhood? J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 166:621-634. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2017] [Revised: 09/27/2017] [Accepted: 09/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
21
|
Corporaal SHA, Bruijn SM, Hoogkamer W, Chalavi S, Boisgontier MP, Duysens J, Swinnen SP, Gooijers J. Different neural substrates for precision stepping and fast online step adjustments in youth. Brain Struct Funct 2018; 223:2039-2053. [PMID: 29368052 PMCID: PMC5884917 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-017-1586-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2017] [Accepted: 11/30/2017] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Humans can navigate through challenging environments (e.g., cluttered or uneven terrains) by modifying their preferred gait pattern (e.g., step length, step width, or speed). Growing behavioral and neuroimaging evidence suggests that the ability to modify preferred step patterns requires the recruitment of cognitive resources. In children, it is argued that prolonged development of complex gait is related to the ongoing development of involved brain regions, but this has not been directly investigated yet. Here, we aimed to elucidate the relationship between structural brain properties and complex gait in youth aged 9–18 years. We used volumetric analyses of cortical grey matter (GM) and whole-brain voxelwise statistical analyses of white matter (WM), and utilized a treadmill-based precision stepping task to investigate complex gait. Moreover, precision stepping was performed on step targets which were either unperturbed or perturbed (i.e., unexpectedly shifting to a new location). Our main findings revealed that larger unperturbed precision step error was associated with decreased WM microstructural organization of tracts that are particularly associated with attentional and visual processing functions. These results strengthen the hypothesis that precision stepping on unperturbed step targets is driven by cortical processes. In contrast, no significant correlations were found between perturbed precision stepping and cortical structures, indicating that other (neural) mechanisms may be more important for this type of stepping.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sharissa H A Corporaal
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Sjoerd M Bruijn
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, MOVE Research Institute Amsterdam, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Wouter Hoogkamer
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
- Department of Integrative Physiology, University of Colorado, Boulder, USA
| | - Sima Chalavi
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Matthieu P Boisgontier
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Jacques Duysens
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Stephan P Swinnen
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium
- Leuven Research Institute for Neuroscience and Disease (LIND), KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Jolien Gooijers
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, box 1501, 3001, Leuven, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
End-State Comfort Across the Lifespan: A Cross-Sectional Investigation of How Movement Context Influences Motor Planning in an Overturned Glass Task. Motor Control 2017; 22:211-230. [PMID: 28771106 DOI: 10.1123/mc.2016-0064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Young adults plan actions in advance to minimize the cost of movement. This is exemplified by the end-state comfort (ESC) effect. A pattern of improvement in ESC in children is linked to the development of cognitive control processes, and decline in older adults is attributed to cognitive decline. This study used a cross-sectional design to examine how movement context (pantomime, demonstration with image/glass as a guide, actual grasping) influences between-hand differences in ESC planning. Children (5- to 12-year-olds), young adults, and two groups of older adults (aged 60-70, and aged 71 and older) were assessed. Findings provide evidence for adult-like patterns of ESC in 8-year-olds. Results are attributed to improvements in proprioceptive acuity and proficiency in generating and implementing internal representations of action. For older adults early in the aging process, sensitivity to ESC did not differ from young adults. However, with increasing age, differences reflect challenges in motor planning with increases in cognitive demand, similar to previous work. Findings have implications for understanding lifespan motor behavior.
Collapse
|
23
|
Marini F, Hughes CML, Morasso P, Masia L. The effects of age and amplitude on wrist proprioceptive acuity. IEEE Int Conf Rehabil Robot 2017; 2017:609-614. [PMID: 28813887 DOI: 10.1109/icorr.2017.8009315] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
This study examined wrist proprioception in a cross-sectional sample of 44 children aged between 8-to 14-years and a control group of 10 neurologically and physically healthy adults. Using a 3-degrees of freedom robotic device, participants performed an ipsilateral joint position matching task in which target amplitude (40% or 80% functional range of motion [fRoM]) and degrees-of-freedom (Flexion/Extension [FE], Radial/Ulnar deviation [RUD], Pronation/Supination [PS]) were manipulated. Results indicated that proprioceptive function became more accurate and consistent over the developmental spectrum, but that the ability to utilize proprioceptive feedback did not reach adult levels till the age of 10-11 years. Furthermore, proprioceptive acuity was influenced by target amplitude, such that movements were more accurate for the 80% fROM compared to the 40% fROM target for both the RUD and PS DoFs, independently of age. The present results provide critical information about the typical development of wrist proprioception that will enable clinicians to chart the course of development and dysfunction in neurological disorders in children, and help establish protocols for the robotic diagnosis and assessment of neurodevelopmental disorders.
Collapse
|
24
|
Corporaal SHA, Gooijers J, Chalavi S, Cheval B, Swinnen SP, Boisgontier MP. Neural predictors of motor control and impact of visuo-proprioceptive information in youth. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38:5628-5647. [PMID: 28782899 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23754] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2017] [Revised: 07/10/2017] [Accepted: 07/24/2017] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
For successful motor control, the central nervous system is required to combine information from the environment and the current body state, which is provided by vision and proprioception respectively. We investigated the relative contribution of visual and proprioceptive information to upper limb motor control and the extent to which structural brain measures predict this performance in youth (n = 40; age range 9-18 years). Participants performed a manual tracking task, adopting in-phase and anti-phase coordination modes. Results showed that, in contrast to older participants, younger participants performed the task with lower accuracy in general and poorer performance in anti-phase than in-phase modes. However, a proprioceptive advantage was found at all ages, that is, tracking accuracy was higher when proprioceptive information was available during both in- and anti-phase modes at all ages. The microstructural organization of interhemispheric connections between homologous dorsolateral prefrontal cortices, and the cortical thickness of the primary motor cortex were associated with sensory-specific accuracy of tracking performance. Overall, the findings suggest that manual tracking performance in youth does not only rely on brain regions involved in sensorimotor processing, but also on prefrontal regions involved in attention and working memory. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5628-5647, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sharissa H A Corporaal
- Department of Movement Sciences, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Jolien Gooijers
- Department of Movement Sciences, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Sima Chalavi
- Department of Movement Sciences, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Boris Cheval
- Department of General Internal Medicine, Rehabilitation and Geriatrics, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland.,Swiss NCCR "LIVES - Overcoming Vulnerability: Life Course Perspectives", University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Stephan P Swinnen
- Department of Movement Sciences, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Matthieu P Boisgontier
- Department of Movement Sciences, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Taylor S, McLean B, Falkmer T, Carey L, Girdler S, Elliott C, Blair E. Does somatosensation change with age in children and adolescents? A systematic review. Child Care Health Dev 2016; 42:809-824. [PMID: 27470009 DOI: 10.1111/cch.12375] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2015] [Revised: 06/01/2016] [Accepted: 06/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Somatosensory modalities, such as touch, proprioception and haptic ability, greatly influence the achievement of developmental milestones for children. Describing somatosensory impairment, natural variability and typical or expected developmental changes across age groups will help establish frameworks for intervention in clinical populations. This systematic review aimed to determine how different somatosensory modalities develop across childhood into adolescence to use as a point of reference for children at risk of somatosensory impairment. METHODS Searches of five electronic databases were undertaken through EBSCO-host (MEDLINE, CINAHL, PsycINFO, SPORTDiscus and ERIC) for studies measuring at least one somatosensory modality in typically developing individuals between birth and 18 years and analysed by age. Characteristics of studies were collected including country of origin, sample size, demographics and outcome measure used. Quality assessment and data extraction were performed by two independent reviewers. RESULTS Twenty three cross-sectional studies were included from a total of 188 articles retrieved: 8 examined aspects of touch, 5 proprioception and 10 haptic ability. Variability of study designs and variation in assessment tools precluded any formal meta-analysis. CONCLUSIONS Somatosensation matures through childhood into adolescence; however, the present review found the pattern of somatosensory development varied depending on the assessment tool used and the aspect of somatosensation being measured, making it difficult to describe typical performance. There is a need for comprehensive assessment batteries to measure the somatosensation, including touch, proprioception and haptic ability, of children at risk of somatosensory impairment to aid in the development of effective interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Taylor
- Faculty of Computing Health and Science, Edith Cowan University, Perth, Australia. .,School of Occupational Therapy and Social work, Curtin University, Perth, Australia. .,Department of Paediatric Rehabilitation, Princess Margaret Hospital for Children, Perth, Australia.
| | - B McLean
- School of Paediatrics and Child Health, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia.,Department of Paediatric Rehabilitation, Princess Margaret Hospital for Children, Perth, Australia
| | - T Falkmer
- School of Occupational Therapy and Social work, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| | - L Carey
- Neurorehabilitation and Recovery, The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia.,Occupational Therapy, School of Allied Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - S Girdler
- School of Occupational Therapy and Social work, Curtin University, Perth, Australia.,Population Sciences, Telethon Kids Institute, Perth, Australia
| | - C Elliott
- School of Occupational Therapy and Social work, Curtin University, Perth, Australia.,Department of Paediatric Rehabilitation, Princess Margaret Hospital for Children, Perth, Australia
| | - E Blair
- Population Sciences, Telethon Kids Institute, Perth, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Ego C, Yüksel D, Orban de Xivry JJ, Lefèvre P. Development of internal models and predictive abilities for visual tracking during childhood. J Neurophysiol 2016; 115:301-9. [PMID: 26510757 PMCID: PMC4760460 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00534.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2015] [Accepted: 10/28/2015] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
The prediction of the consequences of our own actions through internal models is an essential component of motor control. Previous studies showed improvement of anticipatory behaviors with age for grasping, drawing, and postural control. Since these actions require visual and proprioceptive feedback, these improvements might reflect both the development of internal models and the feedback control. In contrast, visual tracking of a temporarily invisible target gives specific markers of prediction and internal models for eye movements. Therefore, we recorded eye movements in 50 children (aged 5-19 yr) and in 10 adults, who were asked to pursue a visual target that is temporarily blanked. Results show that the youngest children (5-7 yr) have a general oculomotor behavior in this task, qualitatively similar to the one observed in adults. However, the overall performance of older subjects in terms of accuracy at target reappearance and variability in their behavior was much better than the youngest children. This late maturation of predictive mechanisms with age was reflected into the development of the accuracy of the internal models governing the synergy between the saccadic and pursuit systems with age. Altogether, we hypothesize that the maturation of the interaction between smooth pursuit and saccades that relies on internal models of the eye and target displacement is related to the continuous maturation of the cerebellum.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Ego
- Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Demet Yüksel
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium; Ophthalmology Department, Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, Brussels, Belgium; and
| | - Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry
- Department of Kinesiology, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Philippe Lefèvre
- Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium;
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Lee CM, Bo J. Error Argumentation Enhance Adaptability in Adults With Low Motor Ability. J Mot Behav 2015; 48:297-308. [DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2015.1092937] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
28
|
Davies BL, Gehringer JE, Kurz MJ. Age-related differences in the motor planning of a lower leg target matching task. Hum Mov Sci 2015; 44:299-306. [PMID: 26519904 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2015] [Revised: 10/09/2015] [Accepted: 10/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
While the development and execution of upper extremity motor plans have been well explored, little is known about how individuals plan and execute rapid, goal-directed motor tasks with the lower extremities. Furthermore, the amount of time needed to integrate the proper amount of visual and proprioceptive feedback before being able to accurately execute a goal-directed movement is not well understood; especially in children. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to initially interrogate how the amount of motor planning time provided to a child before movement execution may influence the preparation and execution of a lower leg goal-directed movement. The results displayed that the amount of pre-movement motor planning time provided may influence the reaction time and accuracy of a goal directed leg movement. All subjects in the study had longer reaction times and less accurate movements when no pre-movement motor planning time was provided. In addition, the children had slower reaction times, slower movements, and less accurate movements than the adults for all the presented targets and motor planning times. These results highlight that children may require more time to successfully plan a goal directed movement with the lower extremity. This suggests that children may potentially have less robust internal models than adults for these types of motor skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brenda L Davies
- Department of Physical Therapy, Munroe-Meyer Institute for Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - James E Gehringer
- Department of Physical Therapy, Munroe-Meyer Institute for Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Max J Kurz
- Department of Physical Therapy, Munroe-Meyer Institute for Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Kagerer FA, Clark JE. Development of kinesthetic-motor and auditory-motor representations in school-aged children. Exp Brain Res 2015; 233:2181-94. [PMID: 25912609 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4288-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2014] [Accepted: 04/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
In two experiments using a center-out task, we investigated kinesthetic-motor and auditory-motor integrations in 5- to 12-year-old children and young adults. In experiment 1, participants moved a pen on a digitizing tablet from a starting position to one of three targets (visuo-motor condition), and then to one of four targets without visual feedback of the movement. In both conditions, we found that with increasing age, the children moved faster and straighter, and became less variable in their feedforward control. Higher control demands for movements toward the contralateral side were reflected in longer movement times and decreased spatial accuracy across all age groups. When feedforward control relies predominantly on kinesthesia, 7- to 10-year-old children were more variable, indicating difficulties in switching between feedforward and feedback control efficiently during that age. An inverse age progression was found for directional endpoint error; larger errors increasing with age likely reflect stronger functional lateralization for the dominant hand. In experiment 2, the same visuo-motor condition was followed by an auditory-motor condition in which participants had to move to acoustic targets (either white band or one-third octave noise). Since in the latter directional cues come exclusively from transcallosally mediated interaural time differences, we hypothesized that auditory-motor representations would show age effects. The results did not show a clear age effect, suggesting that corpus callosum functionality is sufficient in children to allow them to form accurate auditory-motor maps already at a young age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian A Kagerer
- Department of Kinesiology, Michigan State University, 308 W Circle Drive, East Lansing, MI, 48824, USA,
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Kagerer FA, Clark JE. Development of interactions between sensorimotor representations in school-aged children. Hum Mov Sci 2014; 34:164-77. [PMID: 24636697 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2011] [Revised: 02/09/2014] [Accepted: 02/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Reliable sensory-motor integration is a pre-requisite for optimal movement control; the functionality of this integration changes during development. Previous research has shown that motor performance of school-age children is characterized by higher variability, particularly under conditions where vision is not available, and movement planning and control is largely based on kinesthetic input. The purpose of the current study was to determine the characteristics of how kinesthetic-motor internal representations interact with visuo-motor representations during development. To this end, we induced a visuo-motor adaptation in 59 children, ranging from 5 to 12years of age, as well as in a group of adults, and measured initial directional error (IDE) and endpoint error (EPE) during a subsequent condition where visual feedback was not available, and participants had to rely on kinesthetic input. Our results show that older children (age range 9-12years) de-adapted significantly more than younger children (age range 5-8years) over the course of 36 trials in the absence of vision, suggesting that the kinesthetic-motor internal representation in the older children was utilized more efficiently to guide hand movements, and was comparable to the performance of the adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian A Kagerer
- Dept. of Kinesiology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA.
| | - Jane E Clark
- Dept. of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Motor commands in children interfere with their haptic perception of objects. Exp Brain Res 2012; 223:149-57. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3248-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2011] [Accepted: 08/24/2012] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
32
|
The effect of removing visual information on reach control in young children. Exp Brain Res 2012; 222:291-302. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-3216-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2012] [Accepted: 07/29/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
33
|
King BR, Oliveira MA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Clark JE. Development of state estimation explains improvements in sensorimotor performance across childhood. J Neurophysiol 2012; 107:3040-9. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00932.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous developmental research examining sensorimotor control of the arm in school-age children has demonstrated age-related improvements in movement kinematics. However, the mechanisms that underlie these age-related improvements are still unclear. This study hypothesized that changes in sensorimotor performance across childhood can be attributed, in part, to the development of state estimation, defined as estimates computed by the central nervous system, which specify both current and future hand positions and velocities (i.e., hand “state”). Two behavioral experiments were conducted, in which 6- to 12-year-old children and young adults executed goal-directed arm movements. Results from Experiment 1 revealed that young children (i.e., ∼6–8 years) have less precise proprioceptive feedback for static (i.e., stationary) hand state estimation compared with older children (i.e., ∼10–12 years), resulting in increased variability of target-directed reaching movements. Experiment 2 demonstrated that young children rely on delayed and unreliable state estimates during the execution of goal-directed hand movements (i.e., dynamic state estimation), resulting in both increased movement errors and directional variability. Collectively, these results suggest that improvements in sensorimotor behavior across childhood can be attributed, at least partially, to the development of both static and dynamic state estimation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bradley R. King
- Cognitive Motor Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology
| | | | - Jose L. Contreras-Vidal
- Cognitive Motor Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology
- Graduate Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science; and
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland
| | - Jane E. Clark
- Cognitive Motor Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology
- Graduate Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science; and
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Multisensory adaptation of spatial-to-motor transformations in children with developmental coordination disorder. Exp Brain Res 2011; 212:257-65. [PMID: 21584627 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2722-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2011] [Accepted: 05/02/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Recent research has demonstrated that adaptation to a visuomotor distortion systematically influenced movements to auditory targets in adults and typically developing (TD) children, suggesting that the adaptation of spatial-to-motor transformations for reaching movements is multisensory (i.e., generalizable across sensory modalities). The multisensory characteristics of these transformations in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) have not been examined. Given that previous research has demonstrated that children with DCD have deficits in sensorimotor integration, these children may also have impairments in the formation of multisensory spatial-to-motor transformations for target-directed arm movements. To investigate this hypothesis, children with and without DCD executed discrete arm movements to visual and acoustic targets prior to and following exposure to an abrupt visual feedback rotation. Results demonstrated that the magnitudes of the visual aftereffects were equivalent in the TD children and the children with DCD, indicating that both groups of children adapted similarly to the visuomotor perturbation. Moreover, the influence of visuomotor adaptation on auditory-motor performance was similar in the two groups of children. This suggests that the multisensory processes underlying adaptation of spatial-to-motor transformations are similar in children with DCD and TD children.
Collapse
|
35
|
Chen YP, Keen R, Rosander K, von Hofsten C. Movement planning reflects skill level and age changes in toddlers. Child Dev 2011; 81:1846-58. [PMID: 21077868 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01514.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Kinematic measures of children's reaching were found to reflect stable differences in skill level for planning for future actions. Thirty-five toddlers (18-21 months) were engaged in building block towers (precise task) and in placing blocks into an open container (imprecise task). Sixteen children were retested on the same tasks a year later. Longer deceleration as the hand approached the block for pickup was found in the tower task compared with the imprecise task, indicating planning for the second movement. More skillful toddlers who could build high towers had a longer deceleration phase when placing blocks on the tower than toddlers who built low towers. Kinematic differences between the groups remained a year later when all children could build high towers.
Collapse
|
36
|
Pangelinan MM, Kagerer FA, Momen B, Hatfield BD, Clark JE. Electrocortical dynamics reflect age-related differences in movement kinematics among children and adults. Cereb Cortex 2010; 21:737-47. [PMID: 20805237 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhq162] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Previous neuroimaging and behavioral studies demonstrated structural and functional changes in the motor system across childhood. However, it is unclear what functionally relevant electrocortical processes underlie developmental differences in motor planning and control during multijoint, goal-directed movements. The current study characterized age-related differences in electrocortical processes during the performance of discrete aiming movements in children and adults. Electroencephalography and movement kinematics were recorded from 3 groups of participants (n = 15 each): young children (mean 6.7 years), older children (mean 10.2 years), and adults (mean 22.1 years). Age-related differences were evident in the electroencephalographic (EEG) signals. First, young children exhibited less movement-related activity in task-relevant motor areas compared with adults (movement-related cortical potentials). Second, young children exhibited greater activation (less alpha power) of the frontal areas and less activation of the parietal areas as compared with the other groups. At the behavioral level, young children made slower and jerkier movements, with less consistent directional planning compared with older children and adults. Significant correlations were also found between EEG and movement kinematic measures. Taken together, the results of this study provide evidence that age-related differences in the quality of motor planning and performance are reflected in the differences in electrocortical dynamics among children and adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa M Pangelinan
- Cognitive Motor Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742-2611, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
King BR, Pangelinan MM, Kagerer FA, Clark JE. Improvements in proprioceptive functioning influence multisensory-motor integration in 7- to 13-year-old children. Neurosci Lett 2010; 483:36-40. [PMID: 20674678 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2010] [Revised: 07/21/2010] [Accepted: 07/21/2010] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Accurate and efficient sensorimotor behavior depends on precise localization of the body in space, which may be estimated using multiple sensory modalities (i.e., vision and proprioception). Although age-related differences in multisensory-motor integration across childhood have been previously reported, the extent to which age-related changes in unimodal functioning affect multisensory-motor integration is unclear. The purpose of the current study was to address this knowledge gap. Thirty-seven 7- to 13-year-old children moved their dominant hand in a target localization task to visual, proprioceptive, and concurrent visual and proprioceptive stimuli. During a subsequent experimental phase, we introduced a perturbation that placed the concurrent visual and proprioceptive stimuli in conflicting locations (incongruent condition) to determine the relative contributions of vision and proprioception to the multisensory estimate of target position. Results revealed age-related differences in the localization of incongruent stimuli in which the visual estimate of target position contributed more to the multisensory estimate in the younger children whereas the proprioceptive estimate was up-weighted in the older children. Moreover, above and beyond the effects of age, differences in proprioceptive functioning systematically influenced the relative contributions of vision and proprioception to the multisensory estimate during the incongruent trials. Specifically, improvements in proprioceptive functioning resulted in an up-weighting of proprioception, suggesting that the central nervous system of school-aged children utilizes information about unimodal functioning to integrate redundant sensorimotor inputs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bradley R King
- Cognitive Motor Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, 2351 SPH Building, College Park, MD 20742-2611, United States.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Mackrous I, Proteau L. Developmental aspects of pluriarticular movement control. Exp Brain Res 2010; 204:21-32. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2287-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2009] [Accepted: 05/03/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
39
|
Richter S, Brenner E, Karnath HO. Movement orientation is related to mental rotation in childhood. Dev Neuropsychol 2009; 34:284-95. [PMID: 19437204 DOI: 10.1080/87565640902801858] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The present study asked whether the ability to mentally rotate animal pictures was associated with orientation errors of aiming movements in 7- to 10-year-old children and adults. Mental rotation involves a mental change of the encoded picture's orientation, and one has to determine movement vector orientation in motor programming. In the children, slower rotations were related to higher absolute orientation errors, and less correct responses with movements rightward of the target. No significant correlations were found in adults. This result suggests that in children, motor control and mental rotation may be related through orientation specification processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefanie Richter
- Section Neuropsychology, Center of Neurology, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tuebingen, Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
van Roon D, Caeyenberghs K, Swinnen SP, Smits-Engelsman BCM. Development of Feedforward Control in a Dynamic Manual Tracking Task. Child Dev 2008; 79:852-65. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01163.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|