1
|
Velasquez MA, Winston JL, Sur S, Yurgil K, Upman AE, Wroblewski SR, Huddle A, Colombo PJ. Music training is related to late ERP modulation and enhanced performance during Simon task but not Stroop task. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 18:1384179. [PMID: 38711801 PMCID: PMC11070544 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1384179] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2024] [Accepted: 04/08/2024] [Indexed: 05/08/2024] Open
Abstract
Increasing evidence suggests that music training correlates with better performance in tasks measuring executive function components including inhibitory control, working memory and selective attention. The Stroop and Simon tasks measure responses to congruent and incongruent information reflecting cognitive conflict resolution. However, there are more reports of a music-training advantage in the Simon than the Stroop task. Reports indicate that these tasks may differ in the timing of conflict resolution: the Stroop task might involve early sensory stage conflict resolution, while the Simon task may do so at a later motor output planning stage. We hypothesize that musical experience relates to conflict resolution at the late motor output stage rather than the early sensory stage. Behavioral responses, and event-related potentials (ERP) were measured in participants with varying musical experience during these tasks. It was hypothesized that musical experience correlates with better performance in the Simon but not the Stroop task, reflected in ERP components in the later stage of motor output processing in the Simon task. Participants were classified into high- and low-music training groups based on the Goldsmith Musical Sophistication Index. Electrical brain activity was recorded while they completed visual Stroop and Simon tasks. The high-music training group outperformed the low-music training group on the Simon, but not the Stroop task. Mean amplitude difference (incongruent-congruent trials) was greater for the high-music training group at N100 for midline central (Cz) and posterior (Pz) sites in the Simon task and midline central (Cz) and frontal (Fz) sites in the Stroop task, and at N450 at Cz and Pz in the Simon task. N450 difference peaks occurred earlier in the high-music training group at Pz. Differences between the groups at N100 indicate that music training may be related to better sensory discrimination. These differences were not related to better behavioral performance. Differences in N450 responses between the groups, particularly in regions encompassing the motor and parietal cortices, suggest a role of music training in action selection during response conflict situations. Overall, this supports the hypothesis that music training selectively enhances cognitive conflict resolution during late motor output planning stages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jenna L. Winston
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Loyola University New Orleans, New Orleans, LA, United States
| | - Sandeepa Sur
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| | - Kate Yurgil
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Loyola University New Orleans, New Orleans, LA, United States
| | - Anna E. Upman
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Loyola University New Orleans, New Orleans, LA, United States
| | | | - Annabelle Huddle
- Department of Psychology, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, United States
| | - Paul J. Colombo
- Department of Psychology, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, United States
- Brain Institute, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Wojciechowski J, Jurewicz K, Dzianok P, Antonova I, Paluch K, Wolak T, Kublik E. Common and distinct BOLD correlates of Simon and flanker conflicts which can(not) be reduced to time-on-task effects. Hum Brain Mapp 2024; 45:e26549. [PMID: 38224538 PMCID: PMC10777776 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26549] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2023] [Revised: 10/25/2023] [Accepted: 11/16/2023] [Indexed: 01/17/2024] Open
Abstract
The ability to identify and resolve conflicts between standard, well-trained behaviors and behaviors required by the current context is an essential feature of cognitive control. To date, no consensus has been reached on the brain mechanisms involved in exerting such control: while some studies identified diverse patterns of activity across different conflicts, other studies reported common resources across conflict tasks or even across simple tasks devoid of the conflict component. The latter reports attributed the entire activity observed in the presence of conflict to longer time spent on the task (i.e., to the so-called time-on-task effects). Here, we used an extended Multi-Source Interference Task (MSIT) which combines Simon and flanker types of interference to determine shared and conflict-specific mechanisms of conflict resolution in fMRI and their separability from the time-on-task effects. Large portions of the activity in the dorsal attention network and decreases of activity in the default mode network were shared across the tasks and scaled in parallel with increasing reaction times. Importantly, the activity in the sensory and sensorimotor cortices, as well as in the posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) - a key region implicated in conflict processing - could not be exhaustively explained by the time-on-task effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jakub Wojciechowski
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Bioimaging Research CenterInstitute of Physiology and Pathology of HearingWarsawPoland
| | - Katarzyna Jurewicz
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Department of PhysiologyFaculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, McGill UniversityMontrealQuebecCanada
| | - Patrycja Dzianok
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| | - Ingrida Antonova
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Laboratory of NeuroinformaticsNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| | - Katarzyna Paluch
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
- Laboratory of Neurophysiology of MindCenter of Excellence for Neural Plasticity and Brain Disorders: BRAINCITY, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| | - Tomasz Wolak
- Bioimaging Research CenterInstitute of Physiology and Pathology of HearingWarsawPoland
| | - Ewa Kublik
- Neurobiology of Emotions LaboratoryNencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of SciencesWarsawPoland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Manelis A, Hu H, Miceli R, Satz S, Schwalbe M. Neural correlates of the sound facilitation effect in the modified Simon task in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2023; 15:1207707. [PMID: 37644962 PMCID: PMC10461020 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1207707] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2023] [Accepted: 07/31/2023] [Indexed: 08/31/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction The ability to resolve interference declines with age and is attributed to neurodegeneration and reduced cognitive function and mental alertness in older adults. Our previous study revealed that task-irrelevant but environmentally meaningful sounds improve performance on the modified Simon task in older adults. However, little is known about neural correlates of this sound facilitation effect. Methods Twenty right-handed older adults [mean age = 72 (SD = 4), 11 female] participated in the fMRI study. They performed the modified Simon task in which the arrows were presented either in the locations matching the arrow direction (congruent trials) or in the locations mismatching the arrow direction (incongruent trials). A total of 50% of all trials were accompanied by task-irrelevant but environmentally meaningful sounds. Results Participants were faster on the trials with concurrent sounds, independently of whether trials were congruent or incongruent. The sound effect was associated with activation in the distributed network of auditory, posterior parietal, frontal, and limbic brain regions. The magnitude of the behavioral facilitation effect due to sound was associated with the changes in activation of the bilateral auditory cortex, cuneal cortex, and occipital fusiform gyrus, precuneus, left superior parietal lobule (SPL) for No Sound vs. Sound trials. These changes were associated with the corresponding changes in reaction time (RT). Older adults with a recent history of falls showed greater activation in the left SPL than those without falls history. Conclusion Our findings are consistent with the dedifferentiation hypothesis of cognitive aging. The facilitatory effect of sound could be achieved through recruitment of excessive neural resources, which allows older adults to increase attention and mental alertness during task performance. Considering that the SPL is critical for integration of multisensory information, individuals with slower task responses and those with a history of falls may need to recruit this region more actively than individuals with faster responses and those without a fall history to overcome increased difficulty with interference resolution. Future studies should examine the relationship among activation in the SPL, the effect of sound, and falls history in the individuals who are at heightened risk of falls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Manelis
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Hang Hu
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Rachel Miceli
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Skye Satz
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Marie Schwalbe
- University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Cardinale EM, Bezek J, Morales S, Filippi C, Smith AR, Haller S, Valadez EA, Harrewijn A, Phillips D, Chronis-Tuscano A, Brotman MA, Fox NA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, Kircanski K. Cross-sectional and Longitudinal Associations of Anxiety and Irritability With Adolescents' Neural Responses to Cognitive Conflict. BIOLOGICAL PSYCHIATRY. COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROIMAGING 2023; 8:436-444. [PMID: 35358745 PMCID: PMC9764223 DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.03.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2021] [Revised: 03/14/2022] [Accepted: 03/15/2022] [Indexed: 04/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Psychiatric symptoms are commonly comorbid in childhood. The ability to disentangle unique and shared correlates of comorbid symptoms facilitates personalized medicine. Cognitive control is implicated broadly in psychopathology, including in pediatric disorders characterized by anxiety and irritability. To disentangle cognitive control correlates of anxiety versus irritability, the current study leveraged both cross-sectional and longitudinal data from early childhood into adolescence. METHODS For this study, 89 participants were recruited from a large longitudinal research study on early-life temperament to investigate associations of developmental trajectories of anxiety and irritability symptoms (from ages 2 to 15) as well as associations of anxiety and irritability symptoms measured cross-sectionally at age 15 with neural substrates of conflict and error processing assessed at age 15 using the flanker task. RESULTS Results of whole-brain multivariate linear models revealed that anxiety at age 15 was uniquely associated with decreased neural response to conflict across multiple regions implicated in attentional control and conflict adaptation. Conversely, irritability at age 15 was uniquely associated with increased neural response to conflict in regions implicated in response inhibition. Developmental trajectories of anxiety and irritability interacted in relation to neural responses to both error and conflict. CONCLUSIONS Our findings suggest that neural correlates of conflict processing may relate uniquely to anxiety and irritability. Continued cross-symptom research on the neural correlates of cognitive control could stimulate advances in individualized treatment for anxiety and irritability during child and adolescent development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jessica Bezek
- National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
| | - Santiago Morales
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland; Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
| | | | | | - Simone Haller
- National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
| | - Emilio A Valadez
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland
| | - Anita Harrewijn
- National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland; Department of Clinical Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | - Andrea Chronis-Tuscano
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland
| | | | - Nathan A Fox
- Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland
| | - Daniel S Pine
- National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Liu H, Guo Z, Jiang Y, Schwieter JW, Wang F. Neural circuits underlying language control and modality control in bilinguals: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2023; 178:108430. [PMID: 36460081 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108430] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2022] [Revised: 11/02/2022] [Accepted: 11/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Human communication not only involves the need to switch between the modalities of speaking and listening, but for bilinguals, it can also involve switching between languages. It is unknown as to whether modality and language switching share underlying control mechanisms or whether one type of switching affects control processes involved in the other. The present study uses behavioral and fMRI measures to examine neural circuits of control during communicative situations that required Chinese-English bilinguals to switch between modalities and their two languages according to associated color cues. The results showed that for both language and modality control, similar brain regions were recruited during speech production and comprehension. For modality control, the specific control processes partly depended on the corresponding modality. Finally, switching between modalities appears to exert more influence on language control in production compared to comprehension. These findings offer a first detailed characterization of the neural bases involved in control mechanisms in bilingual communication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Huanhuan Liu
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, China.
| | - Zibin Guo
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, China
| | - Yishan Jiang
- Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China; Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Province, Dalian, China
| | - John W Schwieter
- Language Acquisition, Cognition, And Multilingualism Laboratory / Bilingualism Matters @ Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Canada; Department of Linguistics and Languages, McMaster University, Hamilton, Canada
| | - Fenqi Wang
- Department of Linguistics, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Nelson A, Malmberg Gavelin H, Andersson M, Josefsson M, Eskilsson T, Slunga Järvholm L, Stigsdotter Neely A, Boraxbekk CJ. Subjective cognitive complaints and its associations to response inhibition and neural activation in patients with stress-related exhaustion disorder. Stress 2023; 26:2188092. [PMID: 36883330 DOI: 10.1080/10253890.2023.2188092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Stress-related exhaustion is associated with cognitive deficits, measured subjectively using questionnaires targeting everyday slips and failures or more objectively as performance on cognitive tests. Yet, only weak associations between subjective and objective cognitive measures in this group has been presented, theorized to reflect recruitment of compensational resources during cognitive testing. This explorative study investigated how subjectively reported symptoms of cognitive functioning and burnout levels relate to performance as well as neural activation during a response inhibition task. To this end, 56 patients diagnosed with stress-related exhaustion disorder (ED; ICD-10 code F43.8A) completed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) using a Flanker paradigm. In order to investigate associations between neural activity and subjective cognitive complaints (SCCs) and burnout, respectively, scores on the Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) and the Shirom-Melamed Burnout Questionnaire (SMBQ) were added as covariates of interest to a general linear model at the whole-brain level. In agreement with previous research, the results showed that SCCs and burnout levels were largely unrelated to task performance. Moreover, we did not see any correlations between these self-report measures and altered neural activity in frontal brain regions. Instead, we observed an association between the PRMQ and increased neural activity in an occipitally situated cluster. We propose that this finding may reflect compensational processes at the level of basic visual attention which could go unnoticed in cognitive testing but still be reflected in the experience of deficits in everyday cognitive functioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andreas Nelson
- Department of Social and Psychological studies, Karlstad University, Karlstad, Sweden
- Department of Anaesthesiology, Central Hospital of Karlstad, Karlstad, Sweden
| | | | - Micael Andersson
- Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging (UFBI), Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
- Department of Integrative Medical Biology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
| | - Maria Josefsson
- Centre for Demographic and Ageing Research (CEDAR), Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
- Department of Statistics, USBE, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
| | - Therese Eskilsson
- Department of Public Health and Clinical Medicine, Section for Sustainable Health, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
- Department of Community Medicine and Rehabilitation, Physiotherapy, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
| | - Lisbeth Slunga Järvholm
- Department of Public Health and Clinical Medicine, Section for Sustainable Health, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
| | - Anna Stigsdotter Neely
- Department of Social and Psychological studies, Karlstad University, Karlstad, Sweden
- Department of Social Sciences, Technology and Arts; Department of Health, Education and Technology, Luleå University of Technology, Luleå, Sweden
| | - Carl-Johan Boraxbekk
- Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging (UFBI), Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
- Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance, Centre for Functional and Diagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen University Hospital, Hvidovre, Denmark
- Institute for Clinical Medicine, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
- Department of Radiation Sciences, Diagnostic Radiology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
- Institute of Sports Medicine Copenhagen (ISMC) and Department of Neurology, Copenhagen University Hospital Bispebjerg, Copenhagen, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Duprez J, Tabbal J, Hassan M, Modolo J, Kabbara A, Mheich A, Drapier S, Vérin M, Sauleau P, Wendling F, Benquet P, Houvenaghel JF. Spatio-temporal dynamics of large-scale electrophysiological networks during cognitive action control in healthy controls and Parkinson's disease patients. Neuroimage 2022; 258:119331. [PMID: 35660459 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2021] [Revised: 05/16/2022] [Accepted: 05/23/2022] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Among the cognitive symptoms that are associated with Parkinson's disease (PD), alterations in cognitive action control (CAC) are commonly reported in patients. CAC enables the suppression of an automatic action, in favor of a goal-directed one. The implementation of CAC is time-resolved and arguably associated with dynamic changes in functional brain networks. However, the electrophysiological functional networks involved, their dynamic changes, and how these changes are affected by PD, still remain unknown. In this study, to address this gap of knowledge, 10 PD patients and 10 healthy controls (HC) underwent a Simon task while high-density electroencephalography (HD-EEG) was recorded. Source-level dynamic connectivity matrices were estimated using the phase-locking value in the beta (12-25 Hz) and gamma (30-45 Hz) frequency bands. Temporal independent component analyses were used as a dimension reduction tool to isolate the task-related brain network states. Typical microstate metrics were quantified to investigate the presence of these states at the subject-level. Our results first confirmed that PD patients experienced difficulties in inhibiting automatic responses during the task. At the group-level, we found three functional network states in the beta band that involved fronto-temporal, temporo-cingulate and fronto-frontal connections with typical CAC-related prefrontal and cingulate nodes (e.g., inferior frontal cortex). The presence of these networks did not differ between PD patients and HC when analyzing microstates metrics, and no robust correlations with behavior were found. In the gamma band, five networks were found, including one fronto-temporal network that was identical to the one found in the beta band. These networks also included CAC-related nodes previously identified in different neuroimaging modalities. Similarly to the beta networks, no subject-level differences were found between PD patients and HC. Interestingly, in both frequency bands, the dominant network at the subject-level was never the one that was the most durably modulated by the task. Altogether, this study identified the dynamic functional brain networks observed during CAC, but did not highlight PD-related changes in these networks that might explain behavioral changes. Although other new methods might be needed to investigate the presence of task-related networks at the subject-level, this study still highlights that task-based dynamic functional connectivity is a promising approach in understanding the cognitive dysfunctions observed in PD and beyond.
Collapse
Key Words
- Cognitive control
- DIFFIT, Difference in data fitting
- DLPFC, Dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex
- EEG, Electroencephalography
- FC, Functional connectivity
- Functional connectivity
- HC, Healthy controls
- HD-EEG, High-density EEG
- ICA, Independent component analysis
- IFC, Inferior frontal cortex
- MEG, Magnetoencephalography
- Networks, Dynamics
- PD, Parkinson's disease
- PLV, Phase locking value
- Parkinson's disease Abbreviations CAC, Cognitive action control
- ROIS, Regions of interest
- RT, Reaction time
- Simon task
- dBNS, Dynamic brain network state
- dFC, Dynamic functional connectivity
- fMRI, Functional magnetic resonance imaging
- high density EEG
- pre-SMA, Pre-supplementary motor area
- tICA, Temporal ICA
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joan Duprez
- Univ Rennes, LTSI - U1099, F-35000 Rennes, France
| | - Judie Tabbal
- Univ Rennes, LTSI - U1099, F-35000 Rennes, France; Azm Center for Research in Biotechnology and Its Applications, EDST, Lebanese University, Beirut, Lebanon
| | - Mahmoud Hassan
- MINDig, F-35000 Rennes, France; School of Engineering, Reykjavik University, Iceland
| | | | | | | | - Sophie Drapier
- CIC INSERM 1414, Rennes, France; Neurology Department, Pontchaillou Hospital, Rennes University Hospital, France
| | - Marc Vérin
- Neurology Department, Pontchaillou Hospital, Rennes University Hospital, France; Behavioral and Basal Ganglia' Research Unit, University of Rennes 1-Rennes University Hospital, France
| | - Paul Sauleau
- Behavioral and Basal Ganglia' Research Unit, University of Rennes 1-Rennes University Hospital, France; Neurophysiology department, Rennes University Hospital, France
| | | | | | - Jean-François Houvenaghel
- Neurology Department, Pontchaillou Hospital, Rennes University Hospital, France; Behavioral and Basal Ganglia' Research Unit, University of Rennes 1-Rennes University Hospital, France
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Rosenberg J, Dong Q, Florin E, Sripad P, Boers F, Reske M, Shah NJ, Dammers J. Conflict processing networks: A directional analysis of stimulus-response compatibilities using MEG. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0247408. [PMID: 33630915 PMCID: PMC7906351 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2020] [Accepted: 02/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The suppression of distracting information in order to focus on an actual cognitive goal is a key feature of executive functions. The use of brain imaging methods to investigate the underlying neurobiological brain activations that occur during conflict processing have demonstrated a strong involvement of the fronto-parietal attention network (FPAN). Surprisingly, the directional interconnections, their time courses and activations at different frequency bands remain to be elucidated, and thus, this constitutes the focus of this study. The shared information flow between brain areas of the FPAN is provided for frequency bands ranging from the theta to the lower gamma band (4–40 Hz). We employed an adaptation of the Simon task utilizing Magnetoencephalography (MEG). Granger causality was applied to investigate interconnections between the active brain regions, as well as their directionality. Following stimulus onset, the middle frontal precentral cortex and superior parietal cortex were significantly activated during conflict processing in a time window of between 300 to 600ms. Important differences in causality were found across frequency bands between processing of conflicting stimuli in the left as compared to the right visual hemifield. The exchange of information from and to the FPAN was most prominent in the beta band. Moreover, the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula represented key areas for conflict monitoring, either by receiving input from other areas of the FPAN or by generating output themselves. This indicates that the salience network is at least partly involved in processing conflict information. The present study provides detailed insights into the underlying neural mechanisms of the FPAN, especially regarding its temporal characteristics and directional interconnections.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Rosenberg
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
- Department of Neurology, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
- JARA-Brain, Translational Medicine, Aachen, Germany
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-11, JARA, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
| | - Qunxi Dong
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
- Ubiquitous Awareness and Intelligent Solutions Lab, Lanzhou University, Lanzhou, China
| | - Esther Florin
- Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
| | - Praveen Sripad
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
| | - Frank Boers
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
| | - Martina Reske
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
- JARA-Brain, Translational Medicine, Aachen, Germany
| | - N. Jon Shah
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
- Department of Neurology, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
- JARA-Brain, Translational Medicine, Aachen, Germany
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-11, JARA, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
| | - Jürgen Dammers
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, INM-4, Forschungszentrum Jülich, Jülich, Germany
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
The neurocognitive underpinnings of the Simon effect: An integrative review of current research. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2020; 20:1133-1172. [PMID: 33025513 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-020-00836-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/13/2020] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
For as long as half a century the Simon task - in which participants respond to a nonspatial stimulus feature while ignoring its position - has represented a very popular tool to study a variety of cognitive functions, such as attention, cognitive control, and response preparation processes. In particular, the task generates two theoretically interesting effects: the Simon effect proper and the sequential modulations of this effect. In the present study, we review the main theoretical explanations of both kinds of effects and the available neuroscientific studies that investigated the neural underpinnings of the cognitive processes underlying the Simon effect proper and its sequential modulation using electroencephalogram (EEG) and event-related brain potentials (ERP), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Then, we relate the neurophysiological findings to the main theoretical accounts and evaluate their validity and empirical plausibility, including general implications related to processing interference and cognitive control. Overall, neurophysiological research supports claims that stimulus location triggers the creation of a spatial code, which activates a spatially compatible response that, in incompatible conditions, interferes with the response based on the task instructions. Integration of stimulus-response features plays a major role in the occurrence of the Simon effect (which is manifested in the selection of the response) and its modulation by sequential congruency effects. Additional neural mechanisms are involved in supporting the correct and inhibiting the incorrect response.
Collapse
|
10
|
Wu T, Chen C, Spagna A, Wu X, Mackie M, Russell‐Giller S, Xu P, Luo Y, Liu X, Hof PR, Fan J. The functional anatomy of cognitive control: A domain‐general brain network for uncertainty processing. J Comp Neurol 2020; 528:1265-1292. [DOI: 10.1002/cne.24804] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2019] [Revised: 10/12/2019] [Accepted: 10/22/2019] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Tingting Wu
- Department of Psychology, Queens CollegeThe City University of New York Queens New York
| | - Caiqi Chen
- Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, School of PsychologySouth China Normal University Guangzhou China
| | - Alfredo Spagna
- Department of PsychologyColumbia University in the City of New York New York New York
| | - Xia Wu
- Faculty of PsychologyTianjin Normal University Tianjin China
| | - Melissa‐Ann Mackie
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral SciencesNorthwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine Chicago Illinois
| | - Shira Russell‐Giller
- Department of Psychology, Queens CollegeThe City University of New York Queens New York
| | - Pengfei Xu
- Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Center for Brain Disorders and Cognitive NeuroscienceShenzhen University Shenzhen China
| | - Yue‐jia Luo
- Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Center for Brain Disorders and Cognitive NeuroscienceShenzhen University Shenzhen China
| | - Xun Liu
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of PsychologyUniversity of Chinese Academy of Sciences Beijing China
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of Chinese Academy of Sciences Beijing China
| | - Patrick R. Hof
- Nash Family Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain InstituteIcahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai New York New York
| | - Jin Fan
- Department of Psychology, Queens CollegeThe City University of New York Queens New York
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Pineda-Pardo JA, Obeso I, Guida P, Dileone M, Strange BA, Obeso JA, Oliviero A, Foffani G. Static magnetic field stimulation of the supplementary motor area modulates resting-state activity and motor behavior. Commun Biol 2019; 2:397. [PMID: 31701026 PMCID: PMC6823375 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-019-0643-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2019] [Accepted: 09/10/2019] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Focal application of a strong static magnetic field over the human scalp induces measurable local changes in brain function. Whether it also induces distant effects across the brain and how these local and distant effects collectively affect motor behavior remains unclear. Here we applied transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) over the supplementary motor area (SMA) in healthy subjects. At a behavioral level, tSMS increased the time to initiate movement while decreasing errors in choice reaction-time tasks. At a functional level, tSMS increased SMA resting-state fMRI activity and bilateral functional connectivity between the SMA and both the paracentral lobule and the lateral frontotemporal cortex, including the inferior frontal gyrus. These results suggest that tSMS over the SMA can induce behavioral aftereffects associated with modulation of both local and distant functionally-connected cortical circuits involved in the control of speed-accuracy tradeoffs, thus offering a promising protocol for cognitive and clinical research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- José A. Pineda-Pardo
- CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Universidad CEU-San Pablo, Madrid, Spain
| | - Ignacio Obeso
- CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Universidad CEU-San Pablo, Madrid, Spain
| | - Pasqualina Guida
- CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Universidad CEU-San Pablo, Madrid, Spain
| | - Michele Dileone
- CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Universidad CEU-San Pablo, Madrid, Spain
| | - Bryan A. Strange
- Laboratory for Clinical Neuroscience, CTB, Universidad Politecnica de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
- Department of Neuroimaging, Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centre, Reina Sofia-CIEN Foundation, Madrid, Spain
| | - José A. Obeso
- CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Universidad CEU-San Pablo, Madrid, Spain
- CIBERNED, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
| | | | - Guglielmo Foffani
- CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Universidad CEU-San Pablo, Madrid, Spain
- Hospital Nacional de Parapléjicos, Toledo, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Li H, Liu N, Li Y, Weidner R, Fink GR, Chen Q. The Simon Effect Based on Allocentric and Egocentric Reference Frame: Common and Specific Neural Correlates. Sci Rep 2019; 9:13727. [PMID: 31551429 PMCID: PMC6760495 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-49990-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2018] [Accepted: 09/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
An object's location can be represented either relative to an observer's body effectors (egocentric reference frame) or relative to another external object (allocentric reference frame). In non-spatial tasks, an object's task-irrelevant egocentric position conflicts with the side of a task-relevant manual response, which defines the classical Simon effect. Growing evidence suggests that the Simon effect occurs not only based on conflicting positions within the egocentric but also within the allocentric reference frame. Although neural mechanisms underlying the egocentric Simon effect have been extensively researched, neural mechanisms underlying the allocentric Simon effect and their potential interaction with those underlying its egocentric variant remain to be explored. In this fMRI study, spatial congruency between the task-irrelevant egocentric and allocentric target positions and the task-relevant response hand was orthogonally manipulated. Behaviorally, a significant Simon effect was observed for both reference frames. Neurally, three sub-regions in the frontoparietal network were involved in different aspects of the Simon effect, depending on the source of the task-irrelevant object locations. The right precentral gyrus, extending to the right SMA, was generally activated by Simon conflicts, irrespective of the spatial reference frame involved, and showed no additive activity to Simon conflicts. In contrast, the right postcentral gyrus was specifically involved in Simon conflicts induced by task-irrelevant allocentric, rather than egocentric, representations. Furthermore, a right lateral frontoparietal network showed increased neural activity whenever the egocentric and allocentric target locations were incongruent, indicating its functional role as a mismatch detector that monitors the discrepancy concerning allocentric and egocentric object locations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hui Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Nan Liu
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - You Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Ralph Weidner
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Center Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany
| | - Gereon R Fink
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Center Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital Cologne, 50937, Cologne, Germany
| | - Qi Chen
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China.
- Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, P.R. China.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Yang X, Li H, Lin N, Zhang X, Wang Y, Zhang Y, Zhang Q, Zuo X, Yang Y. Uncovering cortical activations of discourse comprehension and their overlaps with common large-scale neural networks. Neuroimage 2019; 203:116200. [PMID: 31536803 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2019] [Revised: 09/11/2019] [Accepted: 09/15/2019] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
We conducted a meta-analysis of 78 task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (1976 total participants) to reveal underlying brain activations and their overlap with large-scale neural networks in the brain during general discourse comprehension and its sub-processes. We found that discourse comprehension involved a neural system consisting of widely distributed brain regions that comprised not only the bilateral perisylvian language zones, but also regions in the superior and medial frontal cortex and the medial temporal lobe. Moreover, this neural system can be categorized into several sub-systems representing various sub-processes of discourse comprehension, with the left inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus serving as core regions across all sub-processes. At a large-scale network level, we found that discourse comprehension relied most heavily on the default network, particularly on its dorsal medial subsystem. The pattern associated with large-scale network cooperation varied according to the respective sub-processes required. Our results reveal the functional dissociation within the discourse comprehension neural system and highlight the flexible involvements of large-scale networks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- XiaoHong Yang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - HuiJie Li
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - Nan Lin
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - XiuPing Zhang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - YinShan Wang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - Ying Zhang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - Qian Zhang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China
| | - XiNian Zuo
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China.
| | - YuFang Yang
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100101, China; Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, 100049, China.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Vissers ME, Ridderinkhof KR, Cohen MX, Slagter HA. Oscillatory Mechanisms of Response Conflict Elicited by Color and Motion Direction: An Individual Differences Approach. J Cogn Neurosci 2018; 30:468-481. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Goal-directed behavior requires control over automatic behavior, for example, when goal-irrelevant information from the environment captures an inappropriate response and conflicts with the correct, goal-relevant action. Neural oscillations in the theta band (∼6 Hz) measured at midfrontal electrodes are thought to form an important substrate of the detection and subsequent resolution of response conflict. Here, we examined the extent to which response conflict and associated theta-band activity depend on the visual stimulus feature dimension that triggers the conflict. We used a feature-based Simon task to manipulate conflict by motion direction and stimulus color. Analyses were focused on individual differences in behavioral response conflict elicited across different stimulus dimensions and their relationship to conflict-related midfrontal theta. We first confirmed the presence of response conflict elicited by task-irrelevant motion and stimulus color, demonstrating the usefulness of our modified version of the Simon task to assess different sensory origins of response conflict. Despite titrating overall task performance, we observed large individual differences in the behavioral manifestations of response conflict elicited by the different visual dimensions. These behavioral conflict effects were mirrored in a dimension-specific relationship with conflict-related midfrontal theta power, such that, for each dimension, individual midfrontal theta power was generally higher when experienced response conflict was high. Finally, exploratory analyses of interregional functional connectivity suggested a role for phase synchronization between frontal and parietal scalp sites in modulating experienced conflict when color was the task-relevant visual dimension. Highlighting the importance of an individual differences approach in cognitive neuroscience, these results reveal large individual differences in experienced response conflict depending on the source of visual interference, which are predicted by conflict-related midfrontal theta power.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Michael X. Cohen
- Radboud University Nijmegen
- Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Aisenberg D, Sapir A, Close A, Henik A, d'Avossa G. Right anterior cerebellum BOLD responses reflect age related changes in Simon task sequential effects. Neuropsychologia 2017; 109:155-164. [PMID: 29233718 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2017] [Revised: 12/06/2017] [Accepted: 12/07/2017] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Participants are slower to report a feature, such as color, when the target appears on the side opposite the instructed response, than when the target appears on the same side. This finding suggests that target location, even when task-irrelevant, interferes with response selection. This effect is magnified in older adults. Lengthening the inter-trial interval, however, suffices to normalize the congruency effect in older adults, by re-establishing young-like sequential effects (Aisenberg et al., 2014). We examined the neurological correlates of age related changes by comparing BOLD signals in young and old participants performing a visual version of the Simon task. Participants reported the color of a peripheral target, by a left or right-hand keypress. Generally, BOLD responses were greater following incongruent than congruent targets. Also, they were delayed and of smaller amplitude in old than young participants. BOLD responses in visual and motor regions were also affected by the congruency of the previous target, suggesting that sequential effects may reflect remapping of stimulus location onto the hand used to make a response. Crucially, young participants showed larger BOLD responses in right anterior cerebellum to incongruent targets, when the previous target was congruent, but smaller BOLD responses to incongruent targets when the previous target was incongruent. Old participants, however, showed larger BOLD responses to congruent than incongruent targets, irrespective of the previous target congruency. We conclude that aging may interfere with the trial by trial updating of the mapping between the task-irrelevant target location and response, which takes place during the inter-trial interval in the cerebellum and underlays sequential effects in a Simon task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Aisenberg
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel; Department of clinical Psychology - Gerontology, Ruppin Academic center, Emek Hefer, Israel.
| | - A Sapir
- School of Psychology and Wolfson Center of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience, Bangor University, Wales, UK
| | - A Close
- School of Psychology and Wolfson Center of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience, Bangor University, Wales, UK
| | - A Henik
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - G d'Avossa
- School of Psychology and Wolfson Center of Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience, Bangor University, Wales, UK
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Liu N, Li H, Su W, Chen Q. Common and specific neural correlates underlying the spatial congruency effect induced by the egocentric and allocentric reference frame. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38:2112-2127. [PMID: 28054740 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23508] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2016] [Revised: 12/16/2016] [Accepted: 12/20/2016] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
The spatial location of an object can be represented in two frames of reference: egocentric (relative to the observer's body or body parts) and allocentric (relative to another object independent of the observer). The object positions relative to the two frames can be either congruent (e.g., both left or both right) or incongruent (e.g., one left and one right). Most of the previous studies, however, did not discriminate between the two types of spatial conflicts. To investigate the common and specific neural mechanisms underlying the spatial congruency effect induced by the two reference frames, we adopted a 3 (type of task: allocentric, egocentric, and color) × 2 (spatial congruency: congruent vs. incongruent) within-subject design in this fMRI study. The spatial congruency effect in the allocentric task was induced by the task-irrelevant egocentric representations, and vice versa in the egocentric task. The nonspatial color task was introduced to control for the differences in bottom-up stimuli between the congruent and incongruent conditions. Behaviorally, significant spatial congruency effect was revealed in both the egocentric and allocentric task. Neurally, the dorsal-medial visuoparietal stream was commonly involved in the spatial congruency effect induced by the task-irrelevant egocentric and allocentric representations. The right superior parietal cortex and the right precentral gyrus were specifically involved in the spatial congruency effect induced by the irrelevant egocentric and allocentric representations, respectively. Taken together, these results suggested that different subregions in the parieto-frontal network played different functional roles in the spatial interaction between the egocentric and allocentric reference frame. Hum Brain Mapp 38:2112-2127, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nan Liu
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Hui Li
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Wen Su
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Qi Chen
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China.,Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China.,Epilepsy Center, Guangdong Sanjiu Brain Hospital, Guangzhou, 510510, China
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Xu M, Xu G, Yang Y. Neural Systems Underlying Emotional and Non-emotional Interference Processing: An ALE Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Front Behav Neurosci 2016; 10:220. [PMID: 27895564 PMCID: PMC5109402 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2016] [Accepted: 10/25/2016] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Understanding how the nature of interference might influence the recruitments of the neural systems is considered as the key to understanding cognitive control. Although, interference processing in the emotional domain has recently attracted great interest, the question of whether there are separable neural patterns for emotional and non-emotional interference processing remains open. Here, we performed an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of 78 neuroimaging experiments, and examined common and distinct neural systems for emotional and non-emotional interference processing. We examined brain activation in three domains of interference processing: emotional verbal interference in the face-word conflict task, non-emotional verbal interference in the color-word Stroop task, and non-emotional spatial interference in the Simon, SRC and Flanker tasks. Our results show that the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was recruited for both emotional and non-emotional interference. In addition, the right anterior insula, presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA), and right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) were activated by interference processing across both emotional and non-emotional domains. In light of these results, we propose that the anterior insular cortex may serve to integrate information from different dimensions and work together with the dorsal ACC to detect and monitor conflicts, whereas pre-SMA and right IFG may be recruited to inhibit inappropriate responses. In contrast, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) showed different degrees of activation and distinct lateralization patterns for different processing domains, which suggests that these regions may implement cognitive control based on the specific task requirements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Min Xu
- Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Biomedical Engineering, Shenzhen University Health Science CenterShenzhen, China; Center for Neuroimaging, Shenzhen Institute of NeuroscienceShenzhen, China; Guangdong Key Laboratory of Biomedical Information Detection and Ultrasound Imaging, Shenzhen UniversityShenzhen, China
| | - Guiping Xu
- Department of Psychology, Guangdong University of Education Guangzhou, China
| | - Yang Yang
- Center for Neuroimaging, Shenzhen Institute of NeuroscienceShenzhen, China; Department of Linguistics, School of Humanities, The University of Hong KongHong Kong, China
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Shang Q, Fu H, Qiu W, Ma Q. Event-related lateralized readiness potential correlates of the emotion-priming Simon effect. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:2123-32. [PMID: 26993492 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4614-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Accepted: 02/26/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
The Simon effect indicates that the reaction time (RT) is shorter when the stimulus and response locations are congruent than when they are not. This study used a priming-target paradigm to explore the emotion-priming Simon effect with event-related potential techniques. The technique of residue iteration decomposition was employed to analyze the lateralized readiness potential (LRP) component, which contributed to disentangling the overlap between LRP and N2 central contralateral in the Simon task with horizontal stimulus-response arrangements. The behavioral result revealed significant Simon effect in RT. In the neural process, the Simon effect was reflected by both the stimulus-locked LRP (S-LRP) and the response-locked LRP (R-LRP), with the incongruent condition showing longer onset latency, larger Gratton-dip, and smaller negative-going deflection of S-LRP and smaller negative-going deflection of R-LRP. These findings suggest that the interference of irrelevant location information is located at the perceptual-encoding (indicated by S-LRP) and response-execution stages (indicated by R-LRP), providing evidence for both the perceptual-interference and response-interference accounts. However, the further linear regression result signaled that the Simon effect might be more closely related to the response-execution stage than the perceptual-encoding stage. In addition, the influence of emotion on the Simon effect was salient only in the incongruent condition, showing longer onset latency of S-LRP and larger Gratton-dip of R-LRP in the negative emotion-priming condition than in the neutral emotion-priming condition, which revealed that the emotional interference effect arose from the stages of perceptual encoding and early response execution only when the locations of a stimulus and the corresponding response were incongruent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qian Shang
- Management School, Hangzhou Dianzi University, Hangzhou, People's Republic of China
| | - Huijian Fu
- School of Management, Zhejiang University, 38# Zheda Road, Hangzhou, 310027, People's Republic of China.,Neuromanagement Lab, Zhejiang University, 38# Zheda Road, Hangzhou, 310027, People's Republic of China
| | - Wenwei Qiu
- School of Management, Zhejiang University, 38# Zheda Road, Hangzhou, 310027, People's Republic of China.,Neuromanagement Lab, Zhejiang University, 38# Zheda Road, Hangzhou, 310027, People's Republic of China
| | - Qingguo Ma
- School of Management, Zhejiang University, 38# Zheda Road, Hangzhou, 310027, People's Republic of China. .,Neuromanagement Lab, Zhejiang University, 38# Zheda Road, Hangzhou, 310027, People's Republic of China.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Classen C, Kibele A. Action induction due to visual perception of linear motion in depth. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2015; 81:131-142. [PMID: 26586291 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0724-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2015] [Accepted: 11/08/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Visually perceived motion can affect observers' motor control in such a way that an intended action can be activated automatically when it contains similar spatial features. So far, effects have been mostly demonstrated with simple displays where objects were moving in a two-dimensional plane. However, almost all actions we perform and visually perceive in everyday life are much more complex and take place in three-dimensional space. The purpose of this study was to examine action inductions due to visual perception of motion in depth. Therefore, we conducted two Simon experiments where subjects were presented with video displays of a sphere (simple displays, experiment 1) and a real person (complex displays, experiment 2) moving in depth. In both experiments, motion direction towards and away from the observer served as task irrelevant information whereas a color change in the video served as relevant information to choose the correct response (close or far positioned response key). The results show that subjects reacted faster when motion direction of the dynamic stimulus was corresponding to the spatial position of the demanded response. In conclusion, this direction-based Simon effect is modulated by spatial position information, higher sensitivity of our visual system for looming objects, and a high salience of objects being on a collision course.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia Classen
- Institute for Sports and Sport Science, University of Kassel, Damaschkestraße 25, 34121, Kassel, Germany.
| | - Armin Kibele
- Institute for Sports and Sport Science, University of Kassel, Damaschkestraße 25, 34121, Kassel, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Scott NM, Leuthold A, Sera MD, Georgopoulos AP. Differential neural activity patterns for spatial relations in humans: a MEG study. Exp Brain Res 2015; 234:429-41. [PMID: 26514809 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4467-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2015] [Accepted: 10/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Children learn the words for above-below relations earlier than for left-right relations, despite treating these equally well in a simple visual categorization task. Even as adults--conflicts in congruency, such as when a stimulus is depicted in a spatially incongruent manner with respect to salient global cues--can be challenging. Here we investigated the neural correlates of encoding and maintaining in working memory above-below and left-right relational planes in 12 adults using magnetoencephalography in order to discover whether above-below relations are represented by the brain differently than left-right relations. Adults performed perfectly on the task behaviorally, so any differences in neural activity were attributed to the stimuli's cognitive attributes. In comparing above-below to left-right relations during stimulus encoding, we found the greatest differences in neural activity in areas associated with space and movement. In comparing congruent to incongruent trials, we found the greatest differential activity in premotor areas. For both contrasts, brain areas involved in the encoding phase were also involved in the maintenance phase, which provides evidence that those brain areas are particularly important in representing the relational planes or congruency types throughout the trial. When comparing neural activity associated with the relational planes during working memory, additional right posterior areas were implicated, whereas the congruent-incongruent contrast implicated additional bilateral frontal and temporal areas. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis left-right relations are represented differently than above-below relations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole M Scott
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Arthur Leuthold
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota, 321 Church Street, Minneapolis, MN, 55455, USA
| | - Maria D Sera
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
- Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Apostolos P Georgopoulos
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA.
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota, 321 Church Street, Minneapolis, MN, 55455, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Action induction by visual perception of rotational motion. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2015; 80:785-804. [PMID: 26259847 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0693-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2014] [Accepted: 07/22/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
A basic process in the planning of everyday actions involves the integration of visually perceived movement characteristics. Such processes of information integration often occur automatically. The aim of the present study was to examine whether the visual perception of spatial characteristics of a rotational motion (rotation direction) can induce a spatially compatible action. Four reaction time experiments were conducted to analyze the effect of perceiving task irrelevant rotational motions of simple geometric figures as well as of gymnasts on a horizontal bar while responding to color changes in these objects. The results show that the participants react faster when the directional information of a rotational motion is compatible with the spatial characteristics of an intended action. The degree of complexity of the perceived event does not play a role in this effect. The spatial features of the used biological motion were salient enough to elicit a motion based Simon effect. However, in the cognitive processing of the visual stimulus, the critical criterion is not the direction of rotation, but rather the relative direction of motion (direction of motion above or below the center of rotation). Nevertheless, this conclusion is tainted with reservations since it is only fully supported by the response behavior of female participants.
Collapse
|
22
|
Neural correlates of the reverse Simon effect in the Hedge and Marsh task. Neuropsychologia 2015; 75:119-31. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/31/2014] [Revised: 05/03/2015] [Accepted: 05/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
23
|
Cieslik EC, Mueller VI, Eickhoff CR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB. Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: evidence from neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 48:22-34. [PMID: 25446951 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 223] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2014] [Revised: 10/08/2014] [Accepted: 11/02/2014] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
The supervisory attentional system has been proposed to mediate non-routine, goal-oriented behaviour by guiding the selection and maintenance of the goal-relevant task schema. Here, we aimed to delineate the brain regions that mediate these high-level control processes via neuroimaging meta-analysis. In particular, we investigated the core neural correlates of a wide range of tasks requiring supervisory control for the suppression of a routine action in favour of another, non-routine one. Our sample comprised n=173 experiments employing go/no-go, stop-signal, Stroop or spatial interference tasks. Consistent convergence across all four paradigm classes was restricted to right anterior insula and inferior frontal junction, with anterior midcingulate cortex and pre-supplementary motor area being consistently involved in all but the go/no-go task. Taken together with lesion studies in patients, our findings suggest that the controlled activation and maintenance of adequate task schemata relies, across paradigms, on a right-dominant midcingulo-insular-inferior frontal core network. This also implies that the role of other prefrontal and parietal regions may be less domain-general than previously thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Edna C Cieslik
- Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine-University, Universitätsstraße 1, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany; Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-1) Research Centre Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Straße, 52428 Jülich, Germany.
| | - Veronika I Mueller
- Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine-University, Universitätsstraße 1, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany; Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-1) Research Centre Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Straße, 52428 Jülich, Germany
| | - Claudia R Eickhoff
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-1) Research Centre Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Straße, 52428 Jülich, Germany; Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, RWTH Aachen, University, Pauwelsstraße 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Robert Langner
- Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine-University, Universitätsstraße 1, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany; Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-1) Research Centre Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Straße, 52428 Jülich, Germany
| | - Simon B Eickhoff
- Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine-University, Universitätsstraße 1, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany; Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-1) Research Centre Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Straße, 52428 Jülich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Neural correlates of the Simon effect modulated by practice with spatial mapping. Neuropsychologia 2014; 63:72-84. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2014] [Revised: 08/11/2014] [Accepted: 08/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
25
|
AbdulSabur NY, Xu Y, Liu S, Chow HM, Baxter M, Carson J, Braun AR. Neural correlates and network connectivity underlying narrative production and comprehension: A combined fMRI and PET study. Cortex 2014; 57:107-27. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2013] [Revised: 05/14/2013] [Accepted: 01/27/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
26
|
Jiang J, Heller K, Egner T. Bayesian modeling of flexible cognitive control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 46 Pt 1:30-43. [PMID: 24929218 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2013] [Revised: 03/31/2014] [Accepted: 06/03/2014] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
"Cognitive control" describes endogenous guidance of behavior in situations where routine stimulus-response associations are suboptimal for achieving a desired goal. The computational and neural mechanisms underlying this capacity remain poorly understood. We examine recent advances stemming from the application of a Bayesian learner perspective that provides optimal prediction for control processes. In reviewing the application of Bayesian models to cognitive control, we note that an important limitation in current models is a lack of a plausible mechanism for the flexible adjustment of control over conflict levels changing at varying temporal scales. We then show that flexible cognitive control can be achieved by a Bayesian model with a volatility-driven learning mechanism that modulates dynamically the relative dependence on recent and remote experiences in its prediction of future control demand. We conclude that the emergent Bayesian perspective on computational mechanisms of cognitive control holds considerable promise, especially if future studies can identify neural substrates of the variables encoded by these models, and determine the nature (Bayesian or otherwise) of their neural implementation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiefeng Jiang
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, United States; Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, United States
| | - Katherine Heller
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, United States; Department of Statistical Science, Duke University, United States
| | - Tobias Egner
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, United States; Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Space positional and motion SRC effects: A comparison with the use of reaction time distribution analysis. Adv Cogn Psychol 2013; 9:202-15. [PMID: 24605178 PMCID: PMC3902833 DOI: 10.2478/v10053-008-0146-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2013] [Accepted: 08/26/2013] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
The analysis of reaction time (RT) distributions has become a recognized standard in studies on the stimulus response correspondence (SRC) effect as it allows exploring how this effect changes as a function of response speed. In this study, we compared the spatial SRC effect (the classic Simon effect) with the motion SRC effect using RT distribution analysis. Four experiments were conducted, in which we manipulated factors of space position and motion for stimulus and response, in order to obtain a clear distinction between positional SRC and motion SRC. Results showed that these two types of SRC effects differ in their RT distribution functions as the space positional SRC effect showed a decreasing function, while the motion SRC showed an increasing function. This suggests that different types of codes underlie these two SRC effects. Potential mechanisms and processes are discussed.
Collapse
|
28
|
Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiol Aging 2013; 34:2183-93. [PMID: 23591131 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 95] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2012] [Revised: 03/04/2013] [Accepted: 03/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
29
|
Obeso I, Robles N, Marrón EM, Redolar-Ripoll D. Dissociating the Role of the pre-SMA in Response Inhibition and Switching: A Combined Online and Offline TMS Approach. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:150. [PMID: 23616761 PMCID: PMC3629293 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00150] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2013] [Accepted: 04/04/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) is considered to be a key node in the cognitive control of actions that require rapid updating, inhibition, or switching, as well as working memory. It is now recognized that the pre-SMA is part of a "cognitive control" network involving the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and subcortical regions, such as the striatum and subthalamic nucleus. However, two important questions remain to be addressed. First, it is not clear if the main role of the pre-SMA in cognitive control lies in inhibition or switching of actions. From imaging evidence, the right pre-SMA is consistently recruited during inhibition and switching, but the extent to which it participates specifically in either of these processes is unknown. Secondly, the pre-SMA may perform inhibition and switching alone or as part of a larger brain network. The present study used online and offline transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to dissociate the roles of pre-SMA in cognitive control, but also to investigate the potential contribution of connectivity between the pre-SMA and IFG. We applied continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS) over the right IFG before participants performed a stop switching task while receiving single TMS pulses over the right pre-SMA. The results were compared to a sham cTBS session and pulses applied over the vertex region. Significant worsening of inhibition as well as response adaptation during inhibition was found when applying pulses over the pre-SMA. However, no such worsening was observed in switch trials. Additionally, after cTBS over the IFG, inhibition was also delayed, suggesting its critical necessity in stopping of actions. The results reveal a key contribution of the pre-SMA in inhibition and could suggest a dissociative role in the switching of actions. These findings indicate there is an essential union between IFG and pre-SMA during inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ignacio Obeso
- Cognitive Neuro-Lab, Cognitive Neuroscience and Information Technologies Research Program, IN3, Open University of CataloniaBarcelona, Spain
- Reward and Decision Making Group, Cognitive Neuroscience Center, CNRS, Lyon 1 UniversityLyon, France
| | - Noemí Robles
- Cognitive Neuro-Lab, Cognitive Neuroscience and Information Technologies Research Program, IN3, Open University of CataloniaBarcelona, Spain
| | - Elena M. Marrón
- Cognitive Neuro-Lab, Cognitive Neuroscience and Information Technologies Research Program, IN3, Open University of CataloniaBarcelona, Spain
| | - Diego Redolar-Ripoll
- Cognitive Neuro-Lab, Cognitive Neuroscience and Information Technologies Research Program, IN3, Open University of CataloniaBarcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Cohen MX, Ridderinkhof KR. EEG source reconstruction reveals frontal-parietal dynamics of spatial conflict processing. PLoS One 2013; 8:e57293. [PMID: 23451201 PMCID: PMC3581478 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 90] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2012] [Accepted: 01/20/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive control requires the suppression of distracting information in order to focus on task-relevant information. We applied EEG source reconstruction via time-frequency linear constrained minimum variance beamforming to help elucidate the neural mechanisms involved in spatial conflict processing. Human subjects performed a Simon task, in which conflict was induced by incongruence between spatial location and response hand. We found an early (∼200 ms post-stimulus) conflict modulation in stimulus-contralateral parietal gamma (30–50 Hz), followed by a later alpha-band (8–12 Hz) conflict modulation, suggesting an early detection of spatial conflict and inhibition of spatial location processing. Inter-regional connectivity analyses assessed via cross-frequency coupling of theta (4–8 Hz), alpha, and gamma power revealed conflict-induced shifts in cortical network interactions: Congruent trials (relative to incongruent trials) had stronger coupling between frontal theta and stimulus-contrahemifield parietal alpha/gamma power, whereas incongruent trials had increased theta coupling between medial frontal and lateral frontal regions. These findings shed new light into the large-scale network dynamics of spatial conflict processing, and how those networks are shaped by oscillatory interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael X Cohen
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2012; 12:241-68. [PMID: 22282036 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1093] [Impact Index Per Article: 84.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
Classic cognitive theory conceptualizes executive functions as involving multiple specific domains, including initiation, inhibition, working memory, flexibility, planning, and vigilance. Lesion and neuroimaging experiments over the past two decades have suggested that both common and unique processes contribute to executive functions during higher cognition. It has been suggested that a superordinate fronto-cingulo-parietal network supporting cognitive control may also underlie a range of distinct executive functions. To test this hypothesis in the largest sample to date, we used quantitative meta-analytic methods to analyze 193 functional neuroimaging studies of 2,832 healthy individuals, ages 18-60, in which performance on executive function measures was contrasted with an active control condition. A common pattern of activation was observed in the prefrontal, dorsal anterior cingulate, and parietal cortices across executive function domains, supporting the idea that executive functions are supported by a superordinate cognitive control network. However, domain-specific analyses showed some variation in the recruitment of anterior prefrontal cortex, anterior and midcingulate regions, and unique subcortical regions such as the basal ganglia and cerebellum. These results are consistent with the existence of a superordinate cognitive control network in the brain, involving dorsolateral prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and parietal cortices, that supports a broad range of executive functions.
Collapse
|
32
|
The Simon effect modulates N2cc and LRP but not the N2pc component. Int J Psychophysiol 2012; 84:120-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.01.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2011] [Revised: 01/20/2012] [Accepted: 01/22/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
33
|
Yotsumoto Y, Seitz AR, Shimojo S, Sakagami M, Watanabe T, Sasaki Y. Performance Dip in motor response induced by task-irrelevant weaker coherent visual motion signals. Cereb Cortex 2011; 22:1887-93. [PMID: 21940704 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhr270] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The Performance Dip is a newly characterized behavioral phenomenon, where, paradoxically, a weaker task-irrelevant visual stimulus causes larger disturbances on the accuracy of a main letter identification task than a stronger stimulus does. Understanding mechanisms of the Performance Dip may provide insight into unconsciousness behavior. Here, we investigated the generalization of the Performance Dip. Specifically, we tested whether the Performance Dip occurs in a motion-related Simon task, and if so, whether the Performance Dip involves the same brain region, that is, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), previously implicated in the Performance Dip, or the supplementary motor area (SMA) and pre-SMA, implicated in a motion-related Simon Task. Subjects made manual directional responses according to the color of stochastic moving dots while ignoring the global direction of moving dots, which could be either congruent or incongruent to the response appropriate to the main task. We found that weak incongruent task-irrelevant stimuli caused a Performance Dip, in which the SMA and pre-SMA, rather than DLPFC, played critical roles. Our results suggest a possible common brain mechanism across different neural circuits, in which weak, but not strong, task-irrelevant information is free from inhibition and intrudes into neural circuits relevant to the main task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuko Yotsumoto
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, 149 13th Street, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Object affordance and spatial-compatibility effects in Parkinson's disease. Cortex 2011; 47:332-41. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.01.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2009] [Revised: 12/17/2009] [Accepted: 01/20/2010] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
35
|
Frühholz S, Godde B, Finke M, Herrmann M. Spatio-temporal brain dynamics in a combined stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict task. Neuroimage 2011; 54:622-34. [PMID: 20691791 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2010] [Revised: 07/09/2010] [Accepted: 07/30/2010] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Sascha Frühholz
- Department of Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Bremen University, Bremen, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Knyazeva MG, Carmeli C, Fornari E, Meuli R, Small M, Frackowiak RS, Maeder P. Binding under conflict conditions: state-space analysis of multivariate EEG synchronization. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 23:2363-75. [PMID: 20946055 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Real-world objects are often endowed with features that violate Gestalt principles. In our experiment, we examined the neural correlates of binding under conflict conditions in terms of the binding-by-synchronization hypothesis. We presented an ambiguous stimulus ("diamond illusion") to 12 observers. The display consisted of four oblique gratings drifting within circular apertures. Its interpretation fluctuates between bound ("diamond") and unbound (component gratings) percepts. To model a situation in which Gestalt-driven analysis contradicts the perceptually explicit bound interpretation, we modified the original diamond (OD) stimulus by speeding up one grating. Using OD and modified diamond (MD) stimuli, we managed to dissociate the neural correlates of Gestalt-related (OD vs. MD) and perception-related (bound vs. unbound) factors. Their interaction was expected to reveal the neural networks synchronized specifically in the conflict situation. The synchronization topography of EEG was analyzed with the multivariate S-estimator technique. We found that good Gestalt (OD vs. MD) was associated with a higher posterior synchronization in the beta-gamma band. The effect of perception manifested itself as reciprocal modulations over the posterior and anterior regions (theta/beta-gamma bands). Specifically, higher posterior and lower anterior synchronization supported the bound percept, and the opposite was true for the unbound percept. The interaction showed that binding under challenging perceptual conditions is sustained by enhanced parietal synchronization. We argue that this distributed pattern of synchronization relates to the processes of multistage integration ranging from early grouping operations in the visual areas to maintaining representations in the frontal networks of sensory memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria G Knyazeva
- Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV) and University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Plomp G, Leeuwen CV, Ioannides AA. Functional specialization and dynamic resource allocation in visual cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 2010; 31:1-13. [PMID: 19621367 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20840] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
We studied the spatiotemporal characteristics of cortical activity in early visual areas and the fusiform gyri (FG) by means of magnetoencephalography (MEG). Subjects performed a visual classification task, in which letters and visually similar pseudoletters were presented in different surrounds and under different task demands. The stimuli appeared in a cued half of the visual field (VF). We observed prestimulus effects on amplitudes in V1 and Cuneus relating to VF and task demands, suggesting a combination of active anticipation and specialized routing of activity in visual processing. Amplitudes in the right FG between 150 and 350 ms after stimulus onset reflected task demands, while those in the left FG between 300 and 400 ms showed selectivity for graphemes. The contrasting stimulus-evoked effects in the right and left FG show that the former area is sensitive to task demands irrespective of stimulus content, whereas the left FG is sensitive to stimulus content irrespectively of task demand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gijs Plomp
- Brain Science Institute, RIKEN, Wako-shi, Japan.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Dorsal and ventral stream mediated visual processing in genetic subtypes of Prader–Willi syndrome. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47:2367-73. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2008] [Revised: 09/26/2008] [Accepted: 09/29/2008] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
39
|
Lamar M, Cutter WJ, Rubia K, Brammer M, Daly EM, Craig MC, Cleare AJ, Murphy DG. 5-HT, prefrontal function and aging: fMRI of inhibition and acute tryptophan depletion. Neurobiol Aging 2009; 30:1135-46. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.09.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2007] [Revised: 09/20/2007] [Accepted: 09/29/2007] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
40
|
Wittfoth M, Schardt DM, Fahle M, Herrmann M. How the brain resolves high conflict situations: Double conflict involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 2009; 44:1201-9. [PMID: 18951983 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2008] [Revised: 09/12/2008] [Accepted: 09/18/2008] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
|
41
|
Forstmann BU, van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR. Neural mechanisms, temporal dynamics, and individual differences in interference control. J Cogn Neurosci 2008; 20:1854-65. [PMID: 18370596 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 149] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) methods may help in understanding processes of response capture and response inhibition in conflict tasks, such as the Simon task. However, data-driven approaches thus far have not yielded consistent insights into these processes. Here, a theory-driven approach is introduced that capitalizes on individual differences in the processes of central interest. Based on the so-called activation-suppression model, specific behavioral parameters for each individual derived from reaction time (RT) distribution analysis were computed and entered into model-based fMRI analyses. These parameters correspond closely to the processes of inappropriate location-driven response activation (capture) and the subsequent inhibition of this activation as detailed by the model. Data from 24 participants revealed activation in the pre-supplementary motor area, which covaried with the RT distribution measure of response capture. Activation in the right inferior frontal cortex was found to covary with the RT distribution measure of response inhibition. These results, which are consistent against the backdrop of the larger literature on cognitive control, could have been derived neither from the standard data-driven fMRI approach, nor from inspecting overall mean RT alone.
Collapse
|
42
|
Anderson IM, McKie S, Elliott R, Williams SR, Deakin JFW. Assessing human 5-HT function in vivo with pharmacoMRI. Neuropharmacology 2008; 55:1029-37. [PMID: 18621068 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2008] [Revised: 06/14/2008] [Accepted: 06/17/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
A number of novel ways of using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to visualise the action of drugs on animal and human brain (pharmacoMRI or phMRI) are becoming established tools in translational psychopharmacology. Using drugs with known pharmacology it is possible to investigate how neurotransmitter systems are involved in neural systems engaged by other processes, such as cognitive challenge (modulation phMRI) or to examine the acute effects of the drug itself in the brain (challenge phMRI). In this article we discuss the principles behind phMRI and review studies investigating the effect of serotonin (5-HT) manipulations. 5-HT modulation phMRI studies show the involvement of 5-HT in a broad range of neural processes ranging from motor function through 'cold' cognition, such as memory and response inhibition, to emotional processing. We highlight findings in brain areas that show some consistency or complementarity across studies, such as the ventrolateral orbitofrontal cortex where modulation by 5-HT is task-specific, and the amygdala in emotional processing where 5-HT is predominantly inhibitory. 5-HT challenge phMRI is promising but as yet few studies have been carried out. New ways of analysing phMRI data include connectivity analysis which holds the promise of going beyond identifying isolated areas of activation/modulation to understanding functional circuits and their neurochemistry. 5-HT phMRI now needs to be taken into patient populations and methods of investigating treatment effects need to be developed. If this is successful then phMRI will provide a genuinely exciting opportunity for the rapid development of better treatments for psychiatric conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I M Anderson
- Neuroscience and Psychiatry Unit, The University of Manchester, Room G907, Stopford Building, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT, United Kingdom.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Two Simon tasks with different sources of conflict: An ERP study of motion- and location-based compatibility effects. Biol Psychol 2008; 78:246-52. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2007] [Revised: 01/31/2008] [Accepted: 03/13/2008] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
44
|
Wittfoth M, Küstermann E, Fahle M, Herrmann M. The influence of response conflict on error processing: evidence from event-related fMRI. Brain Res 2007; 1194:118-29. [PMID: 18177843 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.067] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2007] [Revised: 11/26/2007] [Accepted: 11/28/2007] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The ability to detect errors is a crucial prerequisite for the appropriate adjustment of behavior to future situations. In the present event-related fMRI study, we provide evidence for the existence of different error-related networks within the human brain using a Simon task based on coherent motion perception. While errors related to incompatible trials were mainly associated with activation of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) and the precuneus/posterior cingulate, errors related to trials without pre-response conflict showed specific activation in the right inferior parietal cortex. Despite this functional dissociation of brain networks, conjunction analysis revealed common clusters of activation in the medial wall (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and medial superior frontal cortex (msFC)), and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus/insula, consistent with earlier reports of error-related BOLD-signal increases. The present data support the view that despite of an overlapping core system of error processing, additional brain areas come into play depending on the existence or absence of cognitive conflict.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthias Wittfoth
- Center for Advanced Imaging--CAI Bremen, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Nachev P, Wydell H, O'neill K, Husain M, Kennard C. The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action. Neuroimage 2007; 36 Suppl 2:T155-63. [PMID: 17499162 PMCID: PMC2648723 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 278] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2006] [Accepted: 03/20/2007] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Although regions within the medial frontal cortex are known to be active during voluntary movements their precise role remains unclear. Here we combine functional imaging localisation with psychophysics to demonstrate a strikingly selective contralesional impairment in the ability to inhibit ongoing movement plans in a patient with a rare lesion involving the right pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), but sparing the supplementary motor area (SMA). We find no corresponding delay in simple reaction times, and show that the inhibitory deficit is sensitive to the presence of competition between responses. The findings demonstrate that the pre-SMA plays a critical role in exerting control over voluntary actions in situations of response conflict. We discuss these findings in the context of a unified framework of pre-SMA function, and explore the degree to which extant data on this region can be explained by this function alone.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Parashkev Nachev
- Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Imperial College London, St Dunstan's Road, London W6 8RP, UK.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Martinez-Trujillo JC, Cheyne D, Gaetz W, Simine E, Tsotsos JK. Activation of Area MT/V5 and the Right Inferior Parietal Cortex during the Discrimination of Transient Direction Changes in Translational Motion. Cereb Cortex 2006; 17:1733-9. [PMID: 17012375 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhl084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
The perception of changes in the direction of objects that translate in space is an important function of our visual system. Here we investigate the brain electrical phenomena underlying such a function by using a combination of magnetoencephalography (MEG) and magnetic resonance imaging. We recorded MEG-evoked responses in 9 healthy human subjects while they discriminated the direction of a transient change in a translationally moving random dot pattern presented either to the right or to the left of a central fixation point. We found that responses reached their maximum in 2 main regions corresponding to motion processing area middle temporal (MT)/V5 contralateral to the stimulated visual field, and to the right inferior parietal lobe (rIPL). The activation latencies were very similar in both regions ( approximately 135 ms) following the direction change onset. Our findings suggest that area MT/V5 provides the strongest sensory signal in response to changes in the direction of translational motion, whereas area rIPL may be involved either in the sensory processing of transient motion signals or in the processing of signals related to orienting of attention.
Collapse
|