1
|
Smith DV, Sharp CJ, Dachs A, Wyngaarden J, Sazhin D, Yang Y, Kos M, Tropea T, Kohli I, Clithero JA, Olson I, Giovannetti T, Fareri D, Jarcho JM. Social reward and nonsocial reward processing across the adult lifespan: An interim multi-echo fMRI and diffusion dataset. Data Brief 2024; 56:110810. [PMID: 39252767 PMCID: PMC11381464 DOI: 10.1016/j.dib.2024.110810] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2024] [Revised: 07/26/2024] [Accepted: 08/02/2024] [Indexed: 09/11/2024] Open
Abstract
Social relationships change across the lifespan as social networks narrow and motivational priorities shift. These changes may affect, or reflect, differences in how older adults make decisions related to processing social and non-social rewards. While we have shown initial evidence that older adults have a blunted response to some features of social reward, further work in larger samples is needed to replicate our results and probe the extent to which age-related differences translate to real world consequences, such as financial exploitation. To address this gap, we are conducting a 5-year study funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIH R01-AG067011). Over the course of the funding period (2021-2026), this study seeks to: 1) characterize neural responses to social rewards across adulthood; 2) relate those responses to risk for financial exploitation and sociodemographic factors tied to risk; and 3) examine changes in risk for financial exploitation over time in healthy and vulnerable groups of older adults. This paper describes the preliminary release of data for the larger study. Adults (N = 114; 40 male / 70 female / 4 other or non-binary; 21-80 years of age M = 42.78, SD = 17.13) were recruited from the community to undergo multi-echo fMRI while completing tasks that measure brain function during social reward and decision making. Tasks probe neural response to social reward (e.g., peer vs. monetary feedback) and social context and closeness (e.g., sharing a monetary reward with a friend compared to a stranger). Neural response to social decision making is probed via economic trust and ultimatum games. Functional data are complimented by a T1 weighted anatomical scan and multi-shell diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) to enable tractography and assess neurite orientation dispersion and density. Overall, this dataset has extensive potential for re-use, including leveraging multimodal neuroimaging data, within subject measures of fMRI data from different tasks - data features that are rarely seen in an adult lifespan dataset. Finally, the functional data will allow for developmentally sensitive cross-sectional analyses of differences in brain response to nuanced differences in reward contexts and outcomes (e.g., monetary vs. social; sharing winnings with a friend vs. stranger; stranger vs. computer).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | - Yi Yang
- Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| | - Melanie Kos
- Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| | - Tia Tropea
- Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Lim AC, Weissberger GH, Axelrod J, Mosqueda L, Nguyen AL, Fenton L, Noriega D, Erdman CE, Han SD. Neuropsychological profile associated with financial exploitation vulnerability in older adults without dementia. Clin Neuropsychol 2024:1-17. [PMID: 39060956 DOI: 10.1080/13854046.2024.2378526] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2023] [Accepted: 07/06/2024] [Indexed: 07/28/2024]
Abstract
Objective: Reports of financial exploitation have steadily increased among older adults. Few studies have examined neuropsychological profiles for individuals vulnerable to financial exploitation, and existing studies have focused on susceptibility to scams, one specific type of financial exploitation. The current study therefore examines whether a general measure of financial exploitation vulnerability is associated with neuropsychological performance in a community sample. Methods: A sample (n = 116) of adults aged 50 or older without dementia completed a laboratory visit that measures physical and psychological functioning and a neuropsychological assessment, the Uniform Data Set-3 (UDS-3) and California Verbal Learning Test-II. Results: After covarying for demographics, current medical problems, financial literacy, and a global cognition screen, financial exploitation vulnerability was negatively associated with scores on the Multilingual Naming Test, Craft Story Recall and Delayed Recall, California Verbal Learning Test-II Delayed Recall and Recognition Discriminability, Phonemic Fluency, and Trails B. Financial exploitation vulnerability was not associated with performance on Digit Span, Semantic Fluency, Benson Complex Figure Recall, or Trails A. Conclusions: Among older adults without dementia, individuals at higher risk for financial exploitation demonstrated worse verbal memory, confrontation naming, phonemic fluency, and set-shifting. These tests are generally sensitive to Default Mode Network functioning and Alzheimer's Disease neuropathology. Longitudinal studies in more impaired samples are warranted to further corroborate and elucidate these relationships.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron C Lim
- Department of Psychology, USC Dornsife College of Letters, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Gali H Weissberger
- Department of Social and Health Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| | - Jenna Axelrod
- Department of Psychology, USC Dornsife College of Letters, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Laura Mosqueda
- Department of Family Medicine, USC Keck School of Medicine, Alhambra, CA, USA
| | - Annie L Nguyen
- Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health & Human Longevity Science, University of California San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Laura Fenton
- Department of Psychology, USC Dornsife College of Letters, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Daisy Noriega
- Department of Psychology, USC Dornsife College of Letters, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Camille E Erdman
- Department of Psychology, USC Dornsife College of Letters, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - S Duke Han
- Department of Psychology, USC Dornsife College of Letters, Los Angeles, CA, USA
- Department of Family Medicine, USC Keck School of Medicine, Alhambra, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Chen Y, He H, Ding Y, Tao W, Guan Q, Krueger F. Connectome-based prediction of decreased trust propensity in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: A resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 2024; 292:120605. [PMID: 38615705 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2024.120605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2023] [Revised: 03/19/2024] [Accepted: 04/08/2024] [Indexed: 04/16/2024] Open
Abstract
Trust propensity (TP) relies more on social than economic rationality to transform the perceived probability of betrayal into positive reciprocity expectations in older adults with normal cognition. While deficits in social rationality have been observed in older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), there is limited research on TP and its associated resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) mechanisms in this population. To measure TP and related psychological functions (affect, motivation, executive cognition, and social cognition), MCI (n = 42) and normal healthy control (NHC, n = 115) groups completed a one-shot trust game and additional assessments of related psychological functions. RSFC associated with TP was analyzed using connectome-based predictive modeling (CPM) and lesion simulations. Our behavioral results showed that the MCI group trusted less (i.e., had lower TP) than the NHC group, with lower TP associated with higher sensitivity to the probability of betrayal in the MCI group. In the MCI group, only negative CPM models (RSFC negatively correlated with TP) significantly predicted TP, with a high salience network (SN) contribution. In contrast, in the NHC group, positive CPM models (RSFC positively correlated with TP) significantly predicted TP, with a high contribution from the default mode network (DMN). In addition, the total network strength of the NHC-specific positive network was lower in the MCI group than in the NHC group. Our findings demonstrated a decrease in TP in the MCI group compared to the NHC group, which is associated with deficits in social rationality (social cognition, associated with DMN) and increased sensitivity to betrayal (affect, associated with SN) in a trust dilemma. In conclusion, our study contributes to understanding MCI-related alterations in trust and their underlying neural mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yiqi Chen
- School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China; Department of Psychology, University of Mannheim, Mannheim 68131, Germany
| | - Hao He
- School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China; Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science-Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions, Shenzhen 518055, China
| | - Yiyang Ding
- School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
| | - Wuhai Tao
- School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China; Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science-Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions, Shenzhen 518055, China.
| | - Qing Guan
- School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China; Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science-Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions, Shenzhen 518055, China.
| | - Frank Krueger
- Department of Psychology, University of Mannheim, Mannheim 68131, Germany; School of Systems Biology, George Mason University, Fair, VA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Smith DV, Ludwig RM, Dennison JB, Reeck C, Fareri DS. An fMRI Dataset on Social Reward Processing and Decision Making in Younger and Older Adults. Sci Data 2024; 11:158. [PMID: 38302470 PMCID: PMC10834522 DOI: 10.1038/s41597-024-02931-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2023] [Accepted: 01/08/2024] [Indexed: 02/03/2024] Open
Abstract
Behavioural and neuroimaging research has shown that older adults are less sensitive to financial losses compared to younger adults. Yet relatively less is known about age-related differences in social decisions and social reward processing. As part of a pilot study, we collected behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from 50 participants (Younger: N = 26, ages 18-34 years; Older: N = 24, ages 63-80 years) who completed three tasks in the scanner: an economic trust game as the investor with three partners (computer, stranger, friend) as the investee; a card-guessing task with monetary gains and losses shared with three partners (computer, stranger, friend); and an ultimatum game as responder to three anonymous proposers (computer, age-similar adults, age-dissimilar adults). We also collected B0 field maps and high-resolution structural images (T1-weighted and T2-weighted images). These data could be reused to answer questions about moment-to-moment variability in fMRI signal, representational similarity between tasks, and brain structure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Rita M Ludwig
- Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
- University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Jeffrey B Dennison
- Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
- University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Harris LT. The Neuroscience of Human and Artificial Intelligence Presence. Annu Rev Psychol 2024; 75:433-466. [PMID: 37906951 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-013123-123421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/02/2023]
Abstract
Two decades of social neuroscience and neuroeconomics research illustrate the brain mechanisms that are engaged when people consider human beings, often in comparison to considering artificial intelligence (AI) as a nonhuman control. AI as an experimental control preserves agency and facilitates social interactions but lacks a human presence, providing insight into brain mechanisms that are engaged by human presence and the presence of AI. Here, I review this literature to determine how the brain instantiates human and AI presence across social perception and decision-making paradigms commonly used to realize a social context. People behave toward humans differently than they do toward AI. Moreover, brain regions more engaged by humans compared to AI extend beyond the social cognition brain network to all parts of the brain, and the brain sometimes is engaged more by AI than by humans. Finally, I discuss gaps in the literature, limitations in current neuroscience approaches, and how an understanding of the brain correlates of human and AI presence can inform social science in the wild.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lasana T Harris
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom;
- Alan Turing Institute, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Zaff O, Wyngaarden JB, Dennison JB, Sazhin D, Chein J, McCloskey M, Alloy LB, Jarcho JM, Smith DV, Fareri DS. Social Context and Reward Sensitivity Enhance Corticostriatal Function during Experiences of Shared Rewards. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.10.19.562908. [PMID: 37905048 PMCID: PMC10614966 DOI: 10.1101/2023.10.19.562908] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/02/2023]
Abstract
Although prior research has demonstrated enhanced striatal response when sharing rewards with close social connections, less is known about how individual differences affect ventral striatal (VS) activation and connectivity when experiencing rewards within social contexts. Given that self-reported reward sensitivity and level of substance use have been associated with differences in VS activation, we set out to investigate whether these factors would be independently associated with enhancements to neural reward responses within social contexts. In this pre-registered study, participants (N=45) underwent fMRI while playing a card guessing game in which correct or incorrect guesses resulted in monetary gains and losses that were shared evenly with either a close friend, stranger (confederate), or non-human partner. Consistent with our prior work, we found increased VS activation when sharing rewards with a socially close peer as opposed to an out-of-network stranger. As self-reported reward sensitivity increased, the difference in VS response to rewards shared with friends and strangers decreased. We also found enhanced connectivity between the VS and temporoparietal junction when sharing rewards with close friends as opposed to strangers. Finally, exploratory analyses revealed that as reward sensitivity and sub-clinical substance use increase, the difference in VS connectivity with the right fusiform face area increases as a function of social context. These findings demonstrate that responsivity to the context of close friends may be tied to individual reward sensitivity or sub-clinical substance use habits; together these factors may inform predictions of risk for future mental health disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ori Zaff
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - James B. Wyngaarden
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Jeffrey B. Dennison
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Daniel Sazhin
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Jason Chein
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Michael McCloskey
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Lauren B. Alloy
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Johanna M. Jarcho
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - David V. Smith
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Dominic S. Fareri
- Derner School of Psychology, Adelphi University, Garden City, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Grady CL, Rieck JR, Baracchini G, DeSouza B. Relation of resting brain signal variability to cognitive and socioemotional measures in an adult lifespan sample. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2023; 18:nsad044. [PMID: 37698268 PMCID: PMC10508322 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2022] [Revised: 07/09/2023] [Accepted: 09/06/2023] [Indexed: 09/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Temporal variability of the fMRI-derived blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal during cognitive tasks shows important associations with individual differences in age and performance. Less is known about relations between spontaneous BOLD variability measured at rest and relatively stable cognitive measures, such as IQ or socioemotional function. Here, we examined associations among resting BOLD variability, cognitive/socioemotional scores from the NIH Toolbox and optimal time of day for alertness (chronotype) in a sample of 157 adults from 20 to 86 years of age. To investigate individual differences in these associations independently of age, we regressed age out from both behavioral and BOLD variability scores. We hypothesized that greater BOLD variability would be related to higher fluid cognition scores, more positive scores on socioemotional scales and a morningness chronotype. Consistent with this idea, we found positive correlations between resting BOLD variability, positive socioemotional scores (e.g. self-efficacy) and morning chronotype, as well as negative correlations between variability and negative emotional scores (e.g. loneliness). Unexpectedly, we found negative correlations between BOLD variability and fluid cognition. These results suggest that greater resting brain signal variability facilitates optimal socioemotional function and characterizes those with morning-type circadian rhythms, but individuals with greater fluid cognition may be more likely to show less temporal variability in spontaneous measures of BOLD activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cheryl L Grady
- Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario M6A 2E1, Canada
- Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada
| | - Jenny R Rieck
- Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario M6A 2E1, Canada
| | - Giulia Baracchini
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Montréal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montréal, Quebec H3A 0G4, Canada
| | - Brennan DeSouza
- Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario M6A 2E1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Pehlivanoglu D, Lin T, Lighthall NR, Heemskerk A, Harber A, Wilson RC, Turner GR, Spreng RN, Ebner NC. Facial Trustworthiness Perception Across the Adult Life Span. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2023; 78:434-444. [PMID: 36242775 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbac166] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2022] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Trust is crucial for successful social interaction across the life span. Perceiver age, facial age, and facial emotion have been shown to influence trustworthiness perception, but the complex interplay between these perceiver and facial characteristics has not been examined. METHOD Adopting an adult life-span developmental approach, 199 adults (aged 22-78 years) rated the trustworthiness of faces that systematically varied in age (young, middle-aged, and older) and emotion (neutral, happy, sad, fearful, angry, and disgusted) from the FACES Lifespan Database. RESULTS The study yielded three key results. First, on an aggregated level, facial trustworthiness perception did not differ by perceiver age. Second, all perceivers rated young faces as the most trustworthy, and middle-aged and older (but not young) perceivers rated older faces as least trustworthy. Third, facial emotions signaling threat (fear, anger, and disgust) relative to neutral, happy, and sad expressions moderated age effects on facial trustworthiness perception. DISCUSSION Findings from this study highlight the impact of perceiver and facial characteristics on facial trustworthiness perception in adulthood and aging and have potential to inform first impression formation, with effects on trait attributions and behavior. This publication also provides normative data on perceived facial trustworthiness for the FACES Lifespan Database.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Didem Pehlivanoglu
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| | - Tian Lin
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| | - Nichole R Lighthall
- Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, Florida, USA
| | - Amber Heemskerk
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| | - Alexandria Harber
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| | - Robert C Wilson
- Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, USA
| | - Gary R Turner
- Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada
| | - R Nathan Spreng
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Natalie C Ebner
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA.,Institute on Aging, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| |
Collapse
|