1
|
Ren X, Mann E, Wilhelm RA, Stewart JL, Kuplicki R, Edwards LS, Guinjoan SM, Park H, Aupperle RL, Victor TA, Paulus MP, White EJ, Tsuchiyagaito A. The burden of brooding on neural error processing: The role of repetitive negative thinking in major depressive disorder with and without comorbid anxiety disorders. J Affect Disord 2024; 369:S0165-0327(24)01639-2. [PMID: 39326584 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2024.09.151] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2024] [Revised: 09/18/2024] [Accepted: 09/21/2024] [Indexed: 09/28/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Repetitive negative thinking (RNT), particularly its brooding aspect, is a prominent feature in Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) with and without comorbid anxiety. Error processing, an adaptive cognitive operation, seems to be impaired in individuals with exaggerated RNT. This study measured a post-error neural response, error-related negativity (ERN), during an inhibitory task to examine the mechanism underlying the relationship between RNT and faulty error processing. METHODS We examined current MDD patients with (n = 61) and without comorbid anxiety disorders (COM; n = 38), propensity-matched into High- or Low-RNT groups according to Ruminative Response Scale Brooding subscale scores. Using 32-channel electroencephalography (EEG) during a stop-signal task, we measured baseline-corrected ERN amplitude at FCz 0-100 ms after an incorrect response. A between-subjects ANOVA was conducted with group (High RNT, Low RNT) and comorbidity (MDD, COM) as factors. RESULTS A significant group-by-comorbidity interaction (η2 = 0.07) was found, with MDD participants exhibiting high RNT revealing smaller (more positive) ERN amplitudes compared to their COM counterparts with high RNT (d = 0.77) and MDD participants with low RNT (d = 0.92). CONCLUSIONS Non-anxious individuals with MDD and high RNT showed blunted post-error neural responses, potentially indicating a diminished adaptive neural mechanism for recognizing and correcting errors. However, the presence of comorbid anxiety disorders in individuals with high RNT appears to counteract this reduction, potentially through an enhanced neural response to errors, thereby maintaining a higher level of error-processing activity. Further understanding of these relationships is essential for developing targeted interventions for MDD, with particular focus on the detrimental impact of brooding RNT.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xi Ren
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA.
| | - Eric Mann
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | | | - Jennifer L Stewart
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA; Oxley College of Health & Natural Sciences, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | | | | | | | - Heekyeong Park
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA; University of North Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
| | - Robin L Aupperle
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA; Oxley College of Health & Natural Sciences, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | | | - Martin P Paulus
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA; Oxley College of Health & Natural Sciences, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | - Evan J White
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA; Oxley College of Health & Natural Sciences, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | - Aki Tsuchiyagaito
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA; Oxley College of Health & Natural Sciences, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA; Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Varriale V, De Pascalis V, van der Molen MW. Post-error slowing is associated with intelligence. INTELLIGENCE 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2021.101599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
3
|
Dissociation of Medial Frontal β-Bursts and Executive Control. J Neurosci 2020; 40:9272-9282. [PMID: 33097634 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2072-20.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2020] [Revised: 10/06/2020] [Accepted: 10/08/2020] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
The neural mechanisms of executive and motor control concern both basic researchers and clinicians. In human studies, preparation and cancellation of movements are accompanied by changes in the β-frequency band (15-29 Hz) of electroencephalogram (EEG). Previous studies with human participants performing stop signal (countermanding) tasks have described reduced frequency of transient β-bursts over sensorimotor cortical areas before movement initiation and increased β-bursting over medial frontal areas with movement cancellation. This modulation has been interpreted as contributing to the trial-by-trial control of behavior. We performed identical analyses of EEG recorded over the frontal lobe of macaque monkeys (one male, one female) performing a saccade countermanding task. While we replicate the occurrence and modulation of β-bursts associated with initiation and cancellation of saccades, we found that β-bursts occur too infrequently to account for the observed stopping behavior. We also found β-bursts were more common after errors, but their incidence was unrelated to response time (RT) adaptation. These results demonstrate the homology of this EEG signature between humans and macaques but raise questions about the current interpretation of β band functional significance.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The finding of increased β-bursting over medial frontal cortex with movement cancellation in humans is difficult to reconcile with the finding of modulation too late to contribute to movement cancellation in medial frontal cortex of macaque monkeys. To obtain comparable measurement scales, we recorded electroencephalogram (EEG) over medial frontal cortex of macaques performing a stop signal (countermanding) task. We replicated the occurrence and modulation of β-bursts associated with the cancellation of movements, but we found that β-bursts occur too infrequently to account for observed stopping behavior. Unfortunately, this finding raises doubts whether β-bursts can be a causal mechanism of response inhibition, which impacts future applications in devices such as brain-machine interfaces.
Collapse
|
4
|
Senderecka M, Kossowska M, Sekerdej M, Szewczyk J. Religious fundamentalism is associated with hyperactive performance monitoring: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous responses. Biol Psychol 2018; 140:96-107. [PMID: 30552949 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2017] [Revised: 10/26/2018] [Accepted: 12/11/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The aim of the current study was to examine whether action monitoring is associated with religious fundamentalism. Participants performed a stop-signal task that required response inhibition to a simple auditory tone. The level of their religious fundamentalism was measured on a scale. Analysis with mixed-effects linear models revealed significantly larger error-related negativity, correct-related negativity, and post-error positivity components in individuals scoring higher on religious fundamentalism, pointing to their increased engagement in response monitoring. However, it was not accompanied by improved behavioral performance. The electrophysiological results of our study suggest that individuals high in religious fundamentalism engage more in monitoring for conflict between outcomes of their actions and standards of correct behavior. Our findings also point to a possible association between a fundamentalist mindset and higher levels of negative affect, uncertainty, anxiety, and distress, as measured by response-related brain activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Senderecka
- Institute of Philosophy, Jagiellonian University, Grodzka 52, 31-044 Kraków, Poland.
| | - Małgorzata Kossowska
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Ingardena 6, 30-060 Kraków, Poland
| | - Maciej Sekerdej
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Ingardena 6, 30-060 Kraków, Poland
| | - Jakub Szewczyk
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Ingardena 6, 30-060 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Senderecka M, Ociepka M, Matyjek M, Kroczek B. Post-error Brain Activity Correlates With Incidental Memory for Negative Words. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:178. [PMID: 29867408 PMCID: PMC5951961 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2018] [Accepted: 04/13/2018] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study had three main objectives. First, we aimed to evaluate whether short-duration affective states induced by negative and positive words can lead to increased error-monitoring activity relative to a neutral task condition. Second, we intended to determine whether such an enhancement is limited to words of specific valence or is a general response to arousing material. Third, we wanted to assess whether post-error brain activity is associated with incidental memory for negative and/or positive words. Participants performed an emotional stop-signal task that required response inhibition to negative, positive or neutral nouns while EEG was recorded. Immediately after the completion of the task, they were instructed to recall as many of the presented words as they could in an unexpected free recall test. We observed significantly greater brain activity in the error-positivity (Pe) time window in both negative and positive trials. The error-related negativity amplitudes were comparable in both the neutral and emotional arousing trials, regardless of their valence. Regarding behavior, increased processing of emotional words was reflected in better incidental recall. Importantly, the memory performance for negative words was positively correlated with the Pe amplitude, particularly in the negative condition. The source localization analysis revealed that the subsequent memory recall for negative words was associated with widespread bilateral brain activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and in the medial frontal gyrus, which was registered in the Pe time window during negative trials. The present study has several important conclusions. First, it indicates that the emotional enhancement of error monitoring, as reflected by the Pe amplitude, may be induced by stimuli with symbolic, ontogenetically learned emotional significance. Second, it indicates that the emotion-related enhancement of the Pe occurs across both negative and positive conditions, thus it is preferentially driven by the arousal content of an affective stimuli. Third, our findings suggest that enhanced error monitoring and facilitated recall of negative words may both reflect responsivity to negative events. More speculatively, they can also indicate that post-error activity of the medial prefrontal cortex may selectively support encoding for negative stimuli and contribute to their privileged access to memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Michał Ociepka
- Institute of Computer Science and Computational Mathematics, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Magdalena Matyjek
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Bartłomiej Kroczek
- Institute of Computer Science and Computational Mathematics, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Beuk J, Beninger RJ, Mechefske E, Paré M. Contextual response time adaptation in the countermanding performance of rats. Neuroscience 2016; 337:200-217. [PMID: 27646289 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2016] [Revised: 08/15/2016] [Accepted: 09/07/2016] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Humans and non-human primates are known to lengthen their response time (RT) to a go signal when they occasionally must cancel their responses following a stop signal in a countermanding task as well as to adjust their RT adaptively on a trial-by-trial basis. Less is clear regarding the adaptive RT adjustments in the countermanding performance of rodents. To investigate this question, male Wistar rats (N=12) were trained with food reward to press a lever directly below an illuminated light (go signal), but to countermand the lever press subsequent to a tone (stop signal) presented infrequently (25% of trials) at variable delays. Rats were then tested in a standard responding task (0% stop trials) or a countermanding task with a 10-s or 1-s TO interval following errors. Rats exhibited significant RT lengthening in the countermanding task, compared with the standard responding task, and RT shortening following consecutive correct go trials. They also show RT lengthening following both error trials in the standard responding task and unrewarded, non-canceled stop trials in the countermanding task. RT lengthening following erroneous stop trials was observed in sessions with a 10-s TO interval, but not with a 1-s TO interval. Analyses of RT distributions suggest that RT lengthening results largely from reduced sensitivity to the go signal, but also from reduced readiness. These findings indicate that rats exert control in the countermanding task by lengthening RT in anticipation of stop trials to avoid long, unrewarded TO intervals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Beuk
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
| | - Richard J Beninger
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada; Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
| | - Elysia Mechefske
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
| | - Martin Paré
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada; Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada; Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Fonken YM, Rieger JW, Tzvi E, Crone NE, Chang E, Parvizi J, Knight RT, Krämer UM. Frontal and motor cortex contributions to response inhibition: evidence from electrocorticography. J Neurophysiol 2016; 115:2224-36. [PMID: 26864760 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00708.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2015] [Accepted: 02/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Changes in the environment require rapid modification or inhibition of ongoing behavior. We used the stop-signal paradigm and intracranial recordings to investigate response preparation, inhibition, and monitoring of task-relevant information. Electrocorticographic data were recorded in eight patients with electrodes covering frontal, temporal, and parietal cortex, and time-frequency analysis was used to examine power differences in the beta (13-30 Hz) and high-gamma bands (60-180 Hz). Over motor cortex, beta power decreased, and high-gamma power increased during motor preparation for both go trials (Go) and unsuccessful stops (US). For successful stops (SS), beta increased, and high-gamma was reduced, indexing the cancellation of the prepared response. In the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), stop signals elicited a transient high-gamma increase. The MFG response occurred before the estimated stop-signal reaction time but did not distinguish between SS and US trials, likely signaling attention to the salient stop stimulus. A postresponse high-gamma increase in MFG was stronger for US compared with SS and absent in Go, supporting a role in behavior monitoring. These results provide evidence for differential contributions of frontal subregions to response inhibition, including motor preparation and inhibitory control in motor cortex and cognitive control and action evaluation in lateral prefrontal cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yvonne M Fonken
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California
| | - Jochem W Rieger
- Department of Psychology, University of Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
| | - Elinor Tzvi
- Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany
| | - Nathan E Crone
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Edward Chang
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of California at San Francisco, San Francisco, California
| | - Josef Parvizi
- Department of Neurology, Stanford School of Medicine, Stanford, California
| | - Robert T Knight
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California; Department of Psychology, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California; and
| | - Ulrike M Krämer
- Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany; Institute of Psychology II, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Chang A, Chen CC, Li HH, Li CSR. Event-related potentials for post-error and post-conflict slowing. PLoS One 2014; 9:e99909. [PMID: 24932780 PMCID: PMC4059667 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099909] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2014] [Accepted: 05/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
In a reaction time task, people typically slow down following an error or conflict, each called post-error slowing (PES) and post-conflict slowing (PCS). Despite many studies of the cognitive mechanisms, the neural responses of PES and PCS continue to be debated. In this study, we combined high-density array EEG and a stop-signal task to examine event-related potentials of PES and PCS in sixteen young adult participants. The results showed that the amplitude of N2 is greater during PES but not PCS. In contrast, the peak latency of N2 is longer for PCS but not PES. Furthermore, error-positivity (Pe) but not error-related negativity (ERN) was greater in the stop error trials preceding PES than non-PES trials, suggesting that PES is related to participants' awareness of the error. Together, these findings extend earlier work of cognitive control by specifying the neural correlates of PES and PCS in the stop signal task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew Chang
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Chien-Chung Chen
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
- Center for Neurobiology and Cognitive Science, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
- * E-mail:
| | - Hsin-Hung Li
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Chiang-Shan R. Li
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Neurobiology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Solbakk AK, Funderud I, Løvstad M, Endestad T, Meling T, Lindgren M, Knight RT, Krämer UM. Impact of orbitofrontal lesions on electrophysiological signals in a stop signal task. J Cogn Neurosci 2014; 26:1528-45. [PMID: 24392904 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00561] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
Behavioral inhibition and performance monitoring are critical cognitive functions supported by distributed neural networks including the pFC. We examined neurophysiological correlates of motor response inhibition and action monitoring in patients with focal orbitofrontal (OFC) lesions (n = 12) after resection of a primary intracranial tumor or contusion because of traumatic brain injury. Healthy participants served as controls (n = 14). Participants performed a visual stop signal task. We analyzed behavioral performance as well as event-related brain potentials and oscillations. Inhibition difficulty was adjusted individually to yield an equal amount of successful inhibitions across participants. RTs of patients and controls did not differ significantly in go trials or in failed stop trials, and no differences were observed in estimated stop signal RT. However, electrophysiological response patterns during task performance distinguished the groups. Patients with OFC lesions had enhanced P3 amplitudes to congruent condition go signals and to stop signals. In stop trials, patients had attenuated N2 and error-related negativity, but enhanced error positivity. Patients also showed enhanced and prolonged post-error beta band increases for stop errors. This effect was particularly evident in patients whose lesion extended to the subgenual cingulate cortex. In summary, although response inhibition was not impaired, the diminished stop N2 and ERN support a critical role of the OFC in action monitoring. Moreover, the increased stop P3, error positivity, and post-error beta response indicate that OFC injury affected action outcome evaluation and support the notion that the OFC is relevant for the processing of abstract reinforcers such as performing correctly in the task.
Collapse
|