1
|
van Hoef R, Lynott D, Connell L. Timed picture naming norms for 800 photographs of 200 objects in English. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:6655-6672. [PMID: 38504079 PMCID: PMC11362483 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02380-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/21/2024] [Indexed: 03/21/2024]
Abstract
The present study presents picture-naming norms for a large set of 800 high-quality photographs of 200 natural objects and artefacts spanning a range of categories, with four unique images per object. Participants were asked to provide a single, most appropriate name for each image seen. We report recognition latencies for each image, and several normed variables for the provided names: agreement, H-statistic (i.e. level of naming uncertainty), Zipf word frequency and word length. Rather than simply focusing on a single name per image (i.e. the modal or most common name), analysis of recognition latencies showed that it is important to consider the diversity of labels that participants may ascribe to each pictured object. The norms therefore provide a list of candidate labels per image with weighted measures of word length and frequency per image that incorporate all provided names, as well as modal measures based on the most common name only.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rens van Hoef
- Department of Psychology, Fylde College, Lancaster University, Bailrigg, Lancaster, LA1 4YF, UK.
| | - Dermot Lynott
- Department of Psychology, Maynooth University, Maynooth, Co. Kildare, Ireland
| | - Louise Connell
- Department of Psychology, Fylde College, Lancaster University, Bailrigg, Lancaster, LA1 4YF, UK
- Department of Psychology, Maynooth University, Maynooth, Co. Kildare, Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Zhong J, Huang W, Kang K, Duñabeitia JA, Pliatsikas C, Zhang H. Standardizing norms for 1286 colored pictures in Cantonese. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:6318-6331. [PMID: 38379116 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02362-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/05/2024] [Indexed: 02/22/2024]
Abstract
This study established psycholinguistic norms in Cantonese for a set of 1286 colored pictures sourced from several picture databases, including 750 colored line drawings from MultiPic (Duñabeitia et al., 2018) and 536 photographs selected for McRae et al. (2005) concepts. The pictures underwent rigorous normalization processes. We provided picture characteristics including name and concept agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, and frequency of modal responses. Through correlational analyses, we observed strong interrelationships among these variables. We also compared the current Cantonese norming to other languages and demonstrated similarity and variations among different languages. Additionally, we embraced the multilingual diversity within the current sample, and found that higher Cantonese proficiency but lower non-native language proficiency were associated with better spoken picture naming. Last but not least, we validated the predictive power of normed variables calculated from typed responses to spoken picture naming, and the consistency between typed and spoken responses. The present norming provides a timely and valuable alternative for researchers in the field of psycholinguistics, especially those studying Cantonese production and lexical retrieval. All raw data, analysis scripts, and final norming results are available online as psycholinguistic norms for Cantonese in the following link at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/dz9j6/?view_only=a452d8a56c92430b9dedf21ac26b1bc1 .
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jing Zhong
- Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
| | - Weike Huang
- Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
| | - Keyi Kang
- Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China
| | - Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
- UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| | - Christos Pliatsikas
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
- School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
| | - Haoyun Zhang
- Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China.
- Department of Psychology, University of Macau, Taipa, Macau SAR, China.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Frugarello P, Rusconi E, Job R. The label-feedback effect is influenced by target category in visual search. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0306736. [PMID: 39088399 PMCID: PMC11293709 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0306736] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2024] [Accepted: 06/22/2024] [Indexed: 08/03/2024] Open
Abstract
The label-feedback hypothesis states that language can modulate visual processing. In particular, hearing or reading aloud target names (labels) speeds up performance in visual search tasks by facilitating target detection and such advantage is often measured against a condition where the target name is shown visually (i.e. via the same modality as the search task). The current study conceptually complements and expands previous investigations. The effect of a multimodal label presentation (i.e., an audio+visual, AV, priming label) in a visual search task is compared to that of a multimodal (i.e. white noise+visual, NV, label) and two unimodal (i.e. audio, A, label or visual, V, label) control conditions. The name of a category (i.e. a label at the superordinate level) is used as a cue, instead of the more commonly used target name (a basic level label), with targets belonging to one of three categories: garments, improper weapons, and proper weapons. These categories vary for their structure, improper weapons being an ad hoc category (i.e. context-dependent), unlike proper weapons and garments. The preregistered analysis shows an overall facilitation of visual search performance in the AV condition compared to the NV condition, confirming that the label-feedback effect may not be explained away by the effects of multimodal stimulation only and that it extends to superordinate labels. Moreover, exploratory analyses show that such facilitation is driven by the garments and proper weapons categories, rather than improper weapons. Thus, the superordinate label-feedback effect is modulated by the structural properties of a category. These findings are consistent with the idea that the AV condition prompts an "up-regulation" of the label, a requirement for enhancing the label's beneficial effects, but not when the label refers to an ad hoc category. They also highlight the peculiar status of the category of improper weapons and set it apart from that of proper weapons.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paolo Frugarello
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto (Trento), Italy
- Centre of Security and Crime Sciences, University of Trento – University of Verona, Trento, Italy
| | - Elena Rusconi
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto (Trento), Italy
- Centre of Security and Crime Sciences, University of Trento – University of Verona, Trento, Italy
| | - Remo Job
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto (Trento), Italy
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Bellin I, Iob E, De Pellegrin S, Navarrete E. Phonological neighbourhood effects in Italian speech production: Evidence from healthy and neurologically impaired populations. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2024:1-19. [PMID: 38950198 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2024.2360127] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2023] [Accepted: 05/21/2024] [Indexed: 07/03/2024]
Abstract
In two speech production studies conducted in Italian, we investigated the impact of phonological neighbourhood properties such as the neighbourhood density and the mean frequency of the neighbours on speech processing. Two populations of healthy (Study 1) and neurologically impaired (Study 2) individuals were tested. We employed multi-regression methods to analyse naming latencies in Study 1 and accuracy rates in Study 2 while controlling for various psycholinguistic predictors. In Study 1, pictures with words from high-density neighbourhoods were named faster than those from low-density neighbourhoods. Additionally, words with high-frequency neighbours were named faster in Study 1 and yielded higher accuracy rates in Study 2. The results suggest facilitatory effects of both the phonological neighbourhood density and frequency neighbourhood variables. Furthermore, we observed interactions between these two phonological neighbourhood variables and name agreement and repetition. Specifically, the facilitation effect was more pronounced for pictures with lower name agreement and during the initial presentation of the pictures. These findings are discussed in the context of previous literature and within the framework of interactive models of speech production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene Bellin
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
- Padova Neuroscience Center, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Erica Iob
- UOC Neurologia, DIDAS Medicina dei Sistemi, Azienda Ospedaliera, Università Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Serena De Pellegrin
- UOC Neurologia, DIDAS Medicina dei Sistemi, Azienda Ospedaliera, Università Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Eduardo Navarrete
- Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Adams J, Sherman SM, Williams HL. Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart in photograph form: The Keele Photo Stimulus Set (KPSS). Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:3861-3872. [PMID: 38332413 PMCID: PMC11133023 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02351-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/30/2024] [Indexed: 02/10/2024]
Abstract
Over the last 40 years, object recognition studies have moved from using simple line drawings, to more detailed illustrations, to more ecologically valid photographic representations. Researchers now have access to various stimuli sets, however, existing sets lack the ability to independently manipulate item format, as the concepts depicted are unique to the set they derive from. To enable such comparisons, Rossion and Pourtois (2004) revisited Snodgrass and Vanderwart's (1980) line drawings and digitally re-drew the objects, adding texture and shading. In the current study, we took this further and created a set of stimuli that showcase the same objects in photographic form. We selected six photographs of each object (three color/three grayscale) and collected normative data and RTs. Naming accuracy and agreement was high for all photographs and appeared to steadily increase with format distinctiveness. In contrast to previous data patterns for drawings, naming agreement (H values) did not differ between grey and color photographs, nor did familiarity ratings. However, grey photographs received significantly lower mental imagery agreement and visual complexity scores than color photographs. This suggests that, in comparison to drawings, the ecological nature of photographs may facilitate deeper critical evaluation of whether they offer a good match to a mental representation. Color may therefore play a more vital role in photographs than in drawings, aiding participants in judging the match with their mental representation. This new photographic stimulus set and corresponding normative data provide valuable materials for a wide range of experimental studies of object recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jamie Adams
- School of Psychology, Dorothy Hodgkin Building, Keele University, Keele, Staffordshire, ST5 5BG, UK
| | - Susan M Sherman
- School of Psychology, Dorothy Hodgkin Building, Keele University, Keele, Staffordshire, ST5 5BG, UK
| | - Helen L Williams
- School of Psychology, Dorothy Hodgkin Building, Keele University, Keele, Staffordshire, ST5 5BG, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Morkovina O, Manukyan P, Sharapkova A. Picture naming test through the prism of cognitive neuroscience and linguistics: adapting the test for cerebellar tumor survivors-or pouring new wine in old sacks? Front Psychol 2024; 15:1332391. [PMID: 38566942 PMCID: PMC10985186 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1332391] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2023] [Accepted: 02/20/2024] [Indexed: 04/04/2024] Open
Abstract
A picture naming test (PNT) has long been regarded as an integral part of neuropsychological assessment. In current research and clinical practice, it serves a variety of purposes. PNTs are used to assess the severity of speech impairment in aphasia, monitor possible cognitive decline in aging patients with or without age-related neurodegenerative disorders, track language development in children and map eloquent brain areas to be spared during surgery. In research settings, picture naming tests provide an insight into the process of lexical retrieval in monolingual and bilingual speakers. However, while numerous advances have occurred in linguistics and neuroscience since the classic, most widespread PNTs were developed, few of them have found their way into test design. Consequently, despite the popularity of PNTs in clinical and research practice, their relevance and objectivity remain questionable. The present study provides an overview of literature where relevant criticisms and concerns have been expressed over the recent decades. It aims to determine whether there is a significant gap between conventional test design and the current understanding of the mechanisms underlying lexical retrieval by focusing on the parameters that have been experimentally proven to influence picture naming. We discuss here the implications of these findings for improving and facilitating test design within the picture naming paradigm. Subsequently, we highlight the importance of designing specialized tests with a particular target group in mind, so that test variables could be selected for cerebellar tumor survivors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olga Morkovina
- Laboratory of Diagnostics and Advancing Cognitive Functions, Research Institute for Brain Development and Peak Performance, RUDN University, Moscow, Russia
- Department of English, Faculty of Computational Mathematics and Cybernetics, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Piruza Manukyan
- Laboratory of Diagnostics and Advancing Cognitive Functions, Research Institute for Brain Development and Peak Performance, RUDN University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Anastasia Sharapkova
- Laboratory of Diagnostics and Advancing Cognitive Functions, Research Institute for Brain Development and Peak Performance, RUDN University, Moscow, Russia
- Department of English Linguistics, Faculty of Philology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
The growing interdisciplinary research field of psycholinguistics is in constant need of new and up-to-date tools which will allow researchers to answer complex questions, but also expand on languages other than English, which dominates the field. One type of such tools are picture datasets which provide naming norms for everyday objects. However, existing databases tend to be small in terms of the number of items they include, and have also been normed in a limited number of languages, despite the recent boom in multilingualism research. In this paper we present the Multilingual Picture (Multipic) database, containing naming norms and familiarity scores for 500 coloured pictures, in thirty-two languages or language varieties from around the world. The data was validated with standard methods that have been used for existing picture datasets. This is the first dataset to provide naming norms, and translation equivalents, for such a variety of languages; as such, it will be of particular value to psycholinguists and other interested researchers. The dataset has been made freely available.
Collapse
|
8
|
Li D, Yu YY, Hu N, Zhang M, Liu L, Fan LM, Ruan SS, Wang F. A Color-Picture Version of Boston Naming Test Outperformed the Black-and-White Version in Discriminating Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment and Mild Alzheimer's Disease. Front Neurol 2022; 13:884460. [PMID: 35547369 PMCID: PMC9082938 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2022.884460] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2022] [Accepted: 03/24/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Despite the ubiquity of the Boston naming test (BNT) in clinical practice and research, concerns have been expressed about its poor quality pictures, insufficient psychometric properties, and cultural bias in non-English language backgrounds. We modified the black-and-white BNT with a set of color pictures since color effects have been suggested to improve naming accuracy in the visual naming test. This study aimed to examine and compare the reliability and validity of the color-picture version of BNT (CP-BNT) and the black-and-white version of BNT (BW-BNT) to differentiate amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) or mild Alzheimer's disease (AD) from the cognitive normals. This study included two subgroups, and each subgroup had 101 normal controls, 51 aMCI, and 52 mild AD. One subgroup undertook BW-BNT and the other conducted CP-BNT. The reliability, convergent and discriminant validity, and the diagnostic accuracy of two versions of BNT were evaluated. The CP-BNT showed a greater area under the curve (AUC) than the BW-BNT for aMCI (80.3 vs.s 69.4%) and mild AD (93.5 vs. 77.6%). The CP-BNT also demonstrated better convergent validity with CDR global scores and better reliability (Cronbach's coefficient 0.66 for the CP-BNT vs. 0.55 for the BW-BNT). At the optimal cutoff value of spontaneous naming, the CP-BNT demonstrated improved sensitivity and specificity for differentiating mild AD from NC with a higher positive predictive value, negative predictive value, and lower false-positive rate. Compared with BW-BNT, CP-BNT is a more reliable and valid test to assess cognitive and naming impairment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dan Li
- Innovation Center for Neurological Disorders, Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Yue-Yi Yu
- Innovation Center for Neurological Disorders, Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Nan Hu
- Discipline of Pediatrics & Child Health, School of Clinical Medicine, UNSW Medicine & Health, UNSW Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Min Zhang
- Department of Radiology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Li Liu
- Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Li-Mei Fan
- Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Shi-Shuang Ruan
- Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Fen Wang
- Innovation Center for Neurological Disorders, Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Martínez N, Matute H, Goikoetxea E. PicPsy: A new bank of 106 photographs and line drawings with written naming norms for Spanish-speaking children and adults. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0238976. [PMID: 32925930 PMCID: PMC7489540 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238976] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Accepted: 08/24/2020] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The use of pictures as experimental stimuli is a frequent practice in psychological and educational research. In addition, picture-naming task allows the study of different cognitive processes such as perception, attention, memory and language. Line drawings have been widely used in research to date but it has begun to be highlighted the need for more ecological stimuli such as photographs. However, normative data of a photographic set has not been published yet for use with children. We present PicPsy, a new standardized bank of photographs and matched line drawing. We collected written picture-naming norms for name agreement, unknown responses, alternative names, familiarity and visual complexity. A total of 118 native Spanish-speaking children in grades 3-4 participated in the study. For comparison purposes, 89 adults were also included in the study. Child and adult performance was highly correlated, but we found significant age group differences in all variables examined except for visual complexity. Researchers and teachers could benefit from using the new standardized bank reported here which is published under public domain license. The data and materials for this research are available at the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/nyf3t/.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Naroa Martínez
- Departamento de Fundamentos y Métodos de la Psicología, Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain
| | - Helena Matute
- Departamento de Fundamentos y Métodos de la Psicología, Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain
| | - Edurne Goikoetxea
- Departamento de Fundamentos y Métodos de la Psicología, Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Greco M, Canal P, Bambini V, Moro A. Modulating "Surprise" with Syntax: A Study on Negative Sentences and Eye-Movement Recording. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2020; 49:415-434. [PMID: 32036569 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-020-09691-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
This work focuses on a particular case of negative sentences, the Surprise Negation sentences (SNEGs). SNEGs belong to the class of expletive negation sentences, i.e., they are affirmative in meaning but involve a clausal negation. A clear example is offered by Italian: 'Enonmi è scesa dal treno Maria?!' (let. 'and not CLITIC.to_me is got off-the train Mary' = 'The surprise was that Maria got off the train!'). From a theoretical point of view, the interpretation of SNEGs as affirmative can be derived from their specific syntactic and semantic structure. Here we offer an implementation of the visual world paradigm to test how SNEGs are interpreted. Participants listened to affirmative, negative or expletive negative clauses while four objects (two relevant-either expected or unexpected-and two unrelated) were shown on the screen and their eye movements were recorded. Growth Curve Analysis showed that the fixation patterns to the relevant objects were very similar for affirmative and expletive negative sentences, while striking differences were observed between negative and affirmative sentences. These results showed that negation does play a different role in the mental representation of a sentence, depending on its syntactic derivation. Moreover, we also found that, compared to affirmative sentences, SNEGs require higher processing efforts due to both their syntactic complexity and pragmatic integration, with slower response time and lower accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matteo Greco
- Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax (NETS), Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS Pavia (University School for Advanced Studies IUSS Pavia), Piazza della Vittoria 15, 27100, Pavia, Italy.
| | - Paolo Canal
- Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax (NETS), Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS Pavia (University School for Advanced Studies IUSS Pavia), Piazza della Vittoria 15, 27100, Pavia, Italy
| | - Valentina Bambini
- Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax (NETS), Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS Pavia (University School for Advanced Studies IUSS Pavia), Piazza della Vittoria 15, 27100, Pavia, Italy
| | - Andrea Moro
- Center for Neurocognition, Epistemology and Theoretical Syntax (NETS), Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS Pavia (University School for Advanced Studies IUSS Pavia), Piazza della Vittoria 15, 27100, Pavia, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Ni L, Liu Y, Yu W, Fu X. The China Image Set (CIS): A New Set of 551 Colored Photos With Chinese Norms for 12 Psycholinguistic Variables. Front Psychol 2019; 10:2631. [PMID: 31866888 PMCID: PMC6905411 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02631] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2019] [Accepted: 11/07/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Normative image sets are widely used in memory, perception, and language studies. Following the pioneering work of Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980), a number of normalized image sets with various language norms have been created. However, original image sets that are carefully selected to accommodate Chinese culture and language are still in short supply. In the present study, we provided the China Image Set (CIS), a new set of photo stimuli with Chinese norms. The CIS consists of 551 high-quality colored photo stimuli that cover 21 categories and are normalized on 12 different variables, including name agreement, category agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, object manipulability, manipulation experience, color diagnosticity, shape diagnosticity, image variability, age of acquisition, image agreement, and within-category typicality. Of the 12 variables, shape diagnosticity and manipulation experience with the object depicted in a stimulus are the two newly introduced and normalized variables. Multiple regression analysis reveals that name agreement, age of acquisition, image agreement, shape diagnosticity, and image variability are the most robust determinants of picture naming latency. Our normative dataset of the high-quality photo stimuli offers an ecologically more valid tool to study object recognition and language processing within Chinese culture than has previously been available.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Long Ni
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China.,Department of Psychology, University of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Ye Liu
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China.,Department of Psychology, University of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Wenyuan Yu
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China.,Department of Psychology, University of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Xiaolan Fu
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China.,Department of Psychology, University of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Mitteer DR, Luczynski KC, McKeown CA, Cohrs VL. A comparison of teaching tacts with and without background stimuli on acquisition and generality. BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTIONS 2019. [DOI: 10.1002/bin.1702] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel R. Mitteer
- Munroe‐Meyer InstituteUniversity of Nebraska Medical Center Omaha Nebraska
| | - Kevin C. Luczynski
- Munroe‐Meyer InstituteUniversity of Nebraska Medical Center Omaha Nebraska
| | - Ciobha A. McKeown
- Munroe‐Meyer InstituteUniversity of Nebraska Medical Center Omaha Nebraska
| | - Victoria L. Cohrs
- Munroe‐Meyer InstituteUniversity of Nebraska Medical Center Omaha Nebraska
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Fernandes NL, Pandeirada JNS, Nairne JS. Presenting new stimuli to study emotion: Development and validation of the Objects-on-Hands Picture Database. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0219615. [PMID: 31339959 PMCID: PMC6656347 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2018] [Accepted: 06/27/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
A long-standing goal shared by researchers has been to design optimal experimental procedures, including the selection of appropriate stimuli. Pictures are commonly used in different research fields. However, until recently, researchers have relied mostly on line-drawings, which can have poor ecological validity. We developed a set of high quality standardized photographs of objects from six different categories, recorded under two camera viewpoints, and five presentation conditions (on its own, held by clean hands, and by hands covered with different substances: sauce, chocolate and mud). These various staging conditions can be used to induce different emotional states while maintaining the object of interest constant. We first report normative data on the objects’ name agreement and familiarity collected from North American and Portuguese participants. Results showed high name agreement and familiarity in both samples. Next, arousal, disgust and valence ratings were collected for the stimuli under either an emotional-activating or a neutral context. Subjective ratings varied according to the staging condition and the context, confirming that the same items can effectively be used in different emotional conditions. This database allows researchers to select more ecologically-valid stimuli according to their research purposes while considering several variables of interest and avoiding item-selection problems commonly present when comparing responses to neutral and emotional items.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natália Lisandra Fernandes
- CINTESIS, UA, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
- William James Center for Research, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
- * E-mail:
| | - Josefa N. S. Pandeirada
- CINTESIS, UA, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
- William James Center for Research, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
- Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America
| | - James S. Nairne
- Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Giganti F, Gavazzi G, Righi S, Rossi A, Caprilli S, Giovannelli F, Toni S, Rebai M, Viggiano MP. Priming effect in children with Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus. Child Neuropsychol 2019; 26:100-112. [PMID: 31111792 DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2019.1617260] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies have evidenced cognitive difficulties across various domains in Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus (T1DM) children, but the implicit memory system has not yet been systematically explored.Taking into account that the interplay between memory and perception may be modulated by the semantic category of the stimuli and their salience, we explored explicit and implicit memory using both object and food stimuli to verify whether for T1DM children there is a feebleness in performing the function of memory as a function of the stimuli used.Eighteen T1DM children and 47 healthy children performed an explicit recognition task in which they were requested to judge whether the presented image had already been shown ("old") or not ("new") and an identification priming task in which they were asked to name new and old pictures presented at nine ascending levels of spatial filtering.Results did not reveal any differences between controls and T1DM children in the explicit memory recognition task, whereas some differences between the two groups were found in the identification priming task. In T1DM children, the priming effect was observed only for food images.The dissociation between implicit and explicit memory observed in children with diabetes seems to be modulated by the category of the stimuli, and these results underscore the relevance of taking into account this variable when exploring cognitive functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- F Giganti
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research, Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - G Gavazzi
- Diagnostic and Nuclear Research Institute, IRCCS SDN, Napoli, Italy
| | - S Righi
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research, Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - A Rossi
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research, Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - S Caprilli
- Istituto di Psicoanalisi - ISIPSE, Rome, Italy
| | - F Giovannelli
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research, Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - S Toni
- Pediatric Diabetologic Unit, AOU Meyer, Florence, Italy
| | - M Rebai
- CRFDP, Normandie Université, Rouen, France
| | - M P Viggiano
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research, Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
The use of immersive virtual reality as a research tool is rapidly increasing in numerous scientific disciplines. By combining ecological validity with strict experimental control, immersive virtual reality provides the potential to develop and test scientific theories in rich environments that closely resemble everyday settings. This article introduces the first standardized database of colored three-dimensional (3-D) objects that can be used in virtual reality and augmented reality research and applications. The 147 objects have been normed for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, and corresponding lexical characteristics of the modal object names. The availability of standardized 3-D objects for virtual reality research is important, because reaching valid theoretical conclusions hinges critically on the use of well-controlled experimental stimuli. Sharing standardized 3-D objects across different virtual reality labs will allow for science to move forward more quickly.
Collapse
|
16
|
Navarrete E, Arcara G, Mondini S, Penolazzi B. Italian norms and naming latencies for 357 high quality color images. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0209524. [PMID: 30794543 PMCID: PMC6386297 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2018] [Accepted: 02/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In the domain of cognitive studies on the lexico-semantic representational system, one of the most important means of ensuring effective experimental designs is using ecological stimulus sets accompanied by normative data on the most relevant variables affecting the processing of their items. In the context of image sets, color photographs are particularly suited to this purpose as they reduce the difficulty of visual decoding processes that may emerge with traditional image sets of line drawings. This is especially so in clinical populations. In this study we provide Italian norms for a set of 357 high quality image-items belonging to 23 semantic subcategories from the Moreno-Martínez and Montoro database. Data from several variables affecting image processing were collected from a sample of 255 Italian-speaking participants: age of acquisition, familiarity, lexical frequency, manipulability, name agreement, typicality and visual complexity. Lexical frequency data were derived from the CoLFIS corpus. Furthermore, we collected data on image oral naming latencies to explore how the variance in these latencies could be explained by these critical variables. Multiple regression analyses on the naming latencies show classical psycholinguistic phenomena, such as the effects of age of acquisition and name agreement. In addition, manipulability was also a significant predictor. The described Italian normative data and naming latencies are available for download as supplementary material.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eduardo Navarrete
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | | - Sara Mondini
- Department of General Psychology, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
- Human Inspired Technologies Research Centre-HIT, Padova, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Ferrucci R, Mrakic-Sposta S, Gardini S, Ruggiero F, Vergari M, Mameli F, Arighi A, Spallazzi M, Barocco F, Michelini G, Pietroboni AM, Ghezzi L, Fumagalli GG, D'Urso G, Caffarra P, Scarpini E, Priori A, Marceglia S. Behavioral and Neurophysiological Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) in Fronto-Temporal Dementia. Front Behav Neurosci 2018; 12:235. [PMID: 30420799 PMCID: PMC6215856 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2018] [Accepted: 09/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Fronto-temporal dementia (FTD) is the clinical-diagnostic term that is now preferred to describe patients with a range of progressive dementia syndromes associated with focal atrophy of the frontal and anterior temporal cerebral regions. Currently available FTD medications have been used to control behavioral symptoms, even though they are ineffective in some patients, expensive and may induce adverse effects. Alternative therapeutic approaches are worth pursuing, such as non-invasive brain stimulation with transcranial direct current (tDCS). tDCS has been demonstrated to influence neuronal excitability and reported to enhance cognitive performance in dementia. The aim of this study was to investigate whether applying Anodal tDCS (2 mA intensity, 20 min) over the fronto-temporal cortex bilaterally in five consecutive daily sessions would improve cognitive performance and behavior symptoms in FTD patients, also considering the neuromodulatory effect of stimulation on cortical electrical activity measured through EEG. We recruited 13 patients with FTD and we tested the effect of Anodal and Sham (i.e., placebo) tDCS in two separate experimental sessions. In each session, at baseline (T0), after 5 consecutive days (T1), after 1 week (T2), and after 4 weeks (T3) from the end of the treatment, cognitive and behavioral functions were tested. EEG (21 electrodes, 10-20 international system) was recorded for 5 min with eyes closed at the same time points in nine patients. The present findings showed that Anodal tDCS applied bilaterally over the fronto-temporal cortex significantly improves (1) neuropsychiatric symptoms (as measured by the neuropsychiatric inventory, NPI) in FTD patients immediately after tDCS treatment, and (2) simple visual reaction times (sVRTs) up to 1 month after tDCS treatment. These cognitive improvements significantly correlate with the time course of the slow EEG oscillations (delta and theta bands) measured at the same time points. Even though further studies on larger samples are needed, these findings support the effectiveness of Anodal tDCS over the fronto-temporal regions in FTD on attentional processes that might be correlated to a normalized EEG low-frequency pattern.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roberta Ferrucci
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- “Aldo Ravelli” Research Center for Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics, University of Milan Medical School, Milan, Italy
- III Neurological Clinic, San Paolo Hospital, Milan, Italy
| | - Simona Mrakic-Sposta
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- National Council of Research, Institute of Bioimaging and Molecular Physiology, Segrate, Italy
| | - Simona Gardini
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Fabiana Ruggiero
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
| | - Maurizio Vergari
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
| | - Francesca Mameli
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
| | - Andrea Arighi
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, “Dino Ferrari” center, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
| | - Marco Spallazzi
- Dementia Unit, Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Federica Barocco
- Dementia Unit, Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | | | - Anna Margherita Pietroboni
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, “Dino Ferrari” center, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
| | - Laura Ghezzi
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, “Dino Ferrari” center, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
| | - Giorgio Giulio Fumagalli
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, “Dino Ferrari” center, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
- Department of Neurosciences, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health (NEUROFARBA), University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | | | - Paolo Caffarra
- Dementia Unit, Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria of Parma, Parma, Italy
- Center for Cognitive Disorders and Dementia, AUSL of Parma, Parma, Italy
| | - Elio Scarpini
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, “Dino Ferrari” center, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
| | - Alberto Priori
- “Aldo Ravelli” Research Center for Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics, University of Milan Medical School, Milan, Italy
- III Neurological Clinic, San Paolo Hospital, Milan, Italy
| | - Sara Marceglia
- Fondazione Ca' Granda, IRCCS Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
- Department of Engineering and Architecture, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Clarke AJB, Ludington JD. Thai Norms for Name, Image, and Category Agreement, Object Familiarity, Visual Complexity, Manipulability, and Age of Acquisition for 480 Color Photographic Objects. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2018; 47:607-626. [PMID: 29222768 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-017-9544-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Normative databases containing psycholinguistic variables are commonly used to aid stimulus selection for investigations into language and other cognitive processes. Norms exist for many languages, but not for Thai. The aim of the present research, therefore, was to obtain Thai normative data for the BOSS, a set of 480 high resolution color photographic images of real objects (Brodeur et al. in PLoS ONE 5(5), 2010. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010773 ). Norms were provided by 584 Thai university students on eight dimensions: name agreement, object familiarity, visual complexity, category agreement, image agreement, two types of manipulability (graspability and mimeability), and age of acquisition. The results revealed comparatively similar levels of name agreement to Brodeur et al. especially when unfamiliar items were factored out. The pattern of intercorrelations among the Thai psycholinguistic norms was comparable to previous studies and our cross-linguistic correlations were robust for the same set of pictures in English and French. Conjointly, the findings extend the relevancy of the BOSS to Thailand, supporting this photographic resource for investigations of language and other cognitive processes in monolingual, multilingual, and brain-impaired populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A J Benjamin Clarke
- Department of English and Linguistics, Thammasat University, Khlong Luang, Pathum Thani, Thailand.
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Picture perfect: A stimulus set of 225 pairs of matched clipart and photographic images normed by Mechanical Turk and laboratory participants. Behav Res Methods 2018. [PMID: 29520634 PMCID: PMC6267513 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-018-1028-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
The present study provides normative measures for a new stimulus set of images consisting of 225 everyday objects, each depicted both as a photograph and a matched clipart image generated directly from the photograph (450 images total). The clipart images preserve the same scale, shape, orientation, and general color features as the corresponding photographs. Various norms (modal name and verb agreement measures, picture–name agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, and image agreement) were collected separately for each image type and in two different contexts: online (using Mechanical Turk) and in the laboratory. We discuss similarities and differences in the normative measures according to both image type and experimental context. The full set of norms is provided in the supplemental materials.
Collapse
|
20
|
Duñabeitia JA, Crepaldi D, Meyer AS, New B, Pliatsikas C, Smolka E, Brysbaert M. MultiPic: A standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six European languages. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2018; 71:808-816. [PMID: 28326995 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1310261] [Citation(s) in RCA: 89] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Numerous studies in psychology, cognitive neuroscience and psycholinguistics have used pictures of objects as stimulus materials. Currently, authors engaged in cross-linguistic work or wishing to run parallel studies at multiple sites where different languages are spoken must rely on rather small sets of black-and-white or colored line drawings. These sets are increasingly experienced as being too limited. Therefore, we constructed a new set of 750 colored pictures of concrete concepts. This set, MultiPic, constitutes a new valuable tool for cognitive scientists investigating language, visual perception, memory and/or attention in monolingual or multilingual populations. Importantly, the MultiPic databank has been normed in six different European languages (British English, Spanish, French, Dutch, Italian and German). All stimuli and norms are freely available at http://www.bcbl.eu/databases/multipic .
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Davide Crepaldi
- 2 Milan Center for Neuroscience, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy.,3 International School for Advanced Studies (SISSA), Trieste, Italy
| | - Antje S Meyer
- 4 Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Boris New
- 5 LPNC, Université de Savoie, Chambéry, France.,6 LPNC, CNRS, Grenoble, France
| | - Christos Pliatsikas
- 7 School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
| | - Eva Smolka
- 8 University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Zhou D, Chen Q. Color Image Norms in Mandarin Chinese. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1880. [PMID: 29118730 PMCID: PMC5660975 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01880] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2017] [Accepted: 10/10/2017] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study comprises two parts, an object picture naming task and rating tasks, and reports naming latencies and norms for 435 color images in Mandarin Chinese. These norms include name agreement (%), H-value, concept agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, age of acquisition (AOA) based on adult ratings, object agreement, viewpoint agreement, word frequency, and word length. We examined correlations between the norms and explored the internal structure among these correlative variables by a factor analysis. Four factors were extracted, which accounted for 74.86% of the total variance. These data were analyzed to identify variables with significant contributions to naming latencies using multiple regression analysis, including norms of name agreement (%), familiarity, word frequency, concept agreement, AOA, and object agreement. These variables explained 54.70% of the total variance of naming latencies. This work presents a new set of photo stimuli and a large set of normalized variables. We expect that this study will provide useful materials for further researches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dandan Zhou
- School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
- Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Qi Chen
- School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
- Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
In the present study, we report naming latencies and norms for 327 photos of objects in Dutch. We provide norms for eight psycholinguistic variables: age of acquisition, familiarity, imageability, image agreement, objective and subjective visual complexity, word frequency, word length in syllables and letters, and name agreement. Furthermore, multiple regression analyses revealed that the significant predictors of photo-naming latencies were name agreement, word frequency, imageability, and image agreement. The naming latencies, norms, and stimuli are provided as supplemental materials.
Collapse
|
23
|
Pisoni A, Cerciello M, Cattaneo Z, Papagno C. Phonological facilitation in picture naming: When and where? A tDCS study. Neuroscience 2017; 352:106-121. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.03.043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2016] [Revised: 03/24/2017] [Accepted: 03/25/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
24
|
Brodeur MB, O’Sullivan M, Crone L. The impact of image format and normative variables on episodic memory. COGENT PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1080/23311908.2017.1328869] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Mathieu B. Brodeur
- Department of Psychiatry, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, FBC Pavilion, 6875 Boul. LaSalle, Verdun, Québec, Canada H4H 1R3
| | - Mary O’Sullivan
- Department of Kinesiology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Lauren Crone
- Department of Neuroscience, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Facilitation and interference in naming: A consequence of the same learning process? Cognition 2017; 165:61-72. [PMID: 28501548 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2016] [Revised: 04/25/2017] [Accepted: 04/30/2017] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Our success with naming depends on what we have named previously, a phenomenon thought to reflect learning processes. Repeatedly producing the same name facilitates language production (i.e., repetition priming), whereas producing semantically related names hinders subsequent performance (i.e., semantic interference). Semantic interference is found whether naming categorically related items once (continuous naming) or multiple times (blocked cyclic naming). A computational model suggests that the same learning mechanism responsible for facilitation in repetition creates semantic interference in categorical naming (Oppenheim, Dell, & Schwartz, 2010). Accordingly, we tested the predictions that variability in semantic interference is correlated across categorical naming tasks and is caused by learning, as measured by two repetition priming tasks (picture-picture repetition priming, Exp. 1; definition-picture repetition priming, Exp. 2, e.g., Wheeldon & Monsell, 1992). In Experiment 1 (77 subjects) semantic interference and repetition priming effects were robust, but the results revealed no relationship between semantic interference effects across contexts. Critically, learning (picture-picture repetition priming) did not predict semantic interference effects in either task. We replicated these results in Experiment 2 (81 subjects), finding no relationship between semantic interference effects across tasks or between semantic interference effects and learning (definition-picture repetition priming). We conclude that the changes underlying facilitatory and interfering effects inherent to lexical access are the result of distinct learning processes where multiple mechanisms contribute to semantic interference in naming.
Collapse
|
26
|
Giganti F, Aisa B, Arzilli C, Viggiano MP, Cerasuolo M, Conte F, Ficca G. Priming recognition in good sleepers and in insomniacs. J Sleep Res 2017; 26:345-352. [PMID: 28251718 DOI: 10.1111/jsr.12511] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2016] [Accepted: 01/16/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Insomniacs often report memory and concentration problems, but these complaints have not been consistently supported by performance measurements. Furthermore, while the majority of studies have addressed explicit memory, few have investigated the implicit domain, and very little is known concerning other types of implicit memory besides procedural memory, such as priming. Thus, the purpose of the study was to investigate priming effect for visual stimuli in insomniacs and good sleepers. Twenty-three insomniacs and 20 good sleepers performed a visual priming task in which they were asked to name new and old pictures presented at nine ascending levels of spatial filtering. Both neutral and sleep-related stimuli were used, as previous research evidenced an attentional bias for sleep-related stimuli. Visual priming effect was observed in both groups, suggesting that poor sleep quality does not affect this type of implicit memory. However, the identification process in insomniacs is influenced by the nature of the stimulus to identify: insomniacs recognized both new and old sleep-related stimuli at lower spatial frequencies compared with good sleepers. The tendency to selectively attend to sleep-related stimuli may influence top-down processes occurring during identification of filtering stimuli, by determining a pre-allocation of attentional resources and facilitating identification processes even when sensorial information is scant. Differences in the identification processes of sleep-related stimuli compared with neutral ones should be carefully taken into account as possible pre-clinical markers of insomnia in poor sleepers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fiorenza Giganti
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - Beatrice Aisa
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - Cinzia Arzilli
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | - Maria Pia Viggiano
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
| | | | - Francesca Conte
- Department of Psychology, University of Naples II, Caserta, Italy
| | - Gianluca Ficca
- Department of Psychology, University of Naples II, Caserta, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Giovannelli F, Giganti F, Righi S, Peru A, Borgheresi A, Zaccara G, Viggiano M, Cincotta M. Audio–visual integration effect in lateral occipital cortex during an object recognition task: An interference pilot study. Brain Stimul 2016; 9:574-6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.02.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2015] [Revised: 02/10/2016] [Accepted: 02/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
|
28
|
Harvey DY, Schnur TT. Different Loci of Semantic Interference in Picture Naming vs. Word-Picture Matching Tasks. Front Psychol 2016; 7:710. [PMID: 27242621 PMCID: PMC4865493 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00710] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2014] [Accepted: 04/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Naming pictures and matching words to pictures belonging to the same semantic category impairs performance relative to when stimuli come from different semantic categories (i.e., semantic interference). Despite similar semantic interference phenomena in both picture naming and word-picture matching tasks, the locus of interference has been attributed to different levels of the language system - lexical in naming and semantic in word-picture matching. Although both tasks involve access to shared semantic representations, the extent to which interference originates and/or has its locus at a shared level remains unclear, as these effects are often investigated in isolation. We manipulated semantic context in cyclical picture naming and word-picture matching tasks, and tested whether factors tapping semantic-level (generalization of interference to novel category items) and lexical-level processes (interactions with lexical frequency) affected the magnitude of interference, while also assessing whether interference occurs at a shared processing level(s) (transfer of interference across tasks). We found that semantic interference in naming was sensitive to both semantic- and lexical-level processes (i.e., larger interference for novel vs. old and low- vs. high-frequency stimuli), consistent with a semantically mediated lexical locus. Interference in word-picture matching exhibited stable interference for old and novel stimuli and did not interact with lexical frequency. Further, interference transferred from word-picture matching to naming. Together, these experiments provide evidence to suggest that semantic interference in both tasks originates at a shared processing stage (presumably at the semantic level), but that it exerts its effect at different loci when naming pictures vs. matching words to pictures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Denise Y. Harvey
- Department of Neurology, University of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphia, PA, USA
- Moss Rehabilitation Research InstituteElkins Park, PA, USA
| | - Tatiana T. Schnur
- Baylor College of Medicine, Department of NeurosurgeryHouston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Rappaport SJ, Riddoch MJ, Chechlacz M, Humphreys GW. Unconscious Familiarity-based Color-Form Binding: Evidence from Visual Extinction. J Cogn Neurosci 2015; 28:501-16. [PMID: 26679213 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
There is good evidence that early visual processing involves the coding of different features in independent brain regions. A major question, then, is how we see the world in an integrated manner, in which the different features are "bound" together. A standard account of this has been that feature binding depends on attention to the stimulus, which enables only the relevant features to be linked together [Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136, 1980]. Here we test this influential idea by examining whether, in patients showing visual extinction, the processing of otherwise unconscious (extinguished) stimuli is modulated by presenting objects in their correct (familiar) color. Correctly colored objects showed reduced extinction when they had a learned color, and this color matched across the ipsi- and contralesional items (red strawberry + red tomato). In contrast, there was no reduction in extinction under the same conditions when the stimuli were colored incorrectly (blue strawberry + blue tomato; Experiment 1). The result was not due to the speeded identification of a correctly colored ipsilesional item, as there was no benefit from having correctly colored objects in different colors (red strawberry + yellow lemon; Experiment 2). There was also no benefit to extinction from presenting the correct colors in the background of each item (Experiment 3). The data suggest that learned color-form binding can reduce extinction even when color is irrelevant for the task. The result is consistent with preattentive binding of color and shape for familiar stimuli.
Collapse
|
30
|
de Groot F, Koelewijn T, Huettig F, Olivers CN. A stimulus set of words and pictures matched for visual and semantic similarity. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1101119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
31
|
Barca L, Benedetti F, Pezzulo G. The effects of phonological similarity on the semantic categorisation of pictorial and lexical stimuli: evidence from continuous behavioural measures. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1101117] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
32
|
Catricalà E, Della Rosa PA, Parisi L, Zippo AG, Borsa VM, Iadanza A, Castiglioni I, Falini A, Cappa SF. Functional correlates of preserved naming performance in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neuropsychologia 2015; 76:136-52. [PMID: 25578430 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2014] [Revised: 01/04/2015] [Accepted: 01/05/2015] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Abstract
Naming abilities are typically preserved in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI), a condition associated with increased risk of progression to Alzheimer's disease (AD). We compared the functional correlates of covert picture naming and word reading between a group of aMCI subjects and matched controls. Unimpaired picture naming performance was associated with more extensive activations, in particular involving the parietal lobes, in the aMCI group. In addition, in the condition associated with higher processing demands (blocks of categorically homogeneous items, living items), increased activity was observed in the aMCI group, in particular in the left fusiform gyrus. Graph analysis provided further evidence of increased modularity and reduced integration for the homogenous sets in the aMCI group. The functional modifications associated with preserved performance may reflect, in the case of more demanding tasks, compensatory mechanisms for the subclinical involvement of semantic processing areas by AD pathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eleonora Catricalà
- Institute for Advanced Study IUSS Pavia, Palazzo del Broletto - Piazza della Vittoria n.15, 27100, Pavia, Italy.
| | - Pasquale A Della Rosa
- Institute of Molecular Bioimaging and Physiology, National Research Council (IBFM-CNR), Milan, Italy
| | - Laura Parisi
- Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy
| | - Antonio G Zippo
- Institute of Molecular Bioimaging and Physiology, National Research Council (IBFM-CNR), Milan, Italy
| | - Virginia M Borsa
- Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy
| | - Antonella Iadanza
- Department of Neuroradiology and CERMAC, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy
| | - Isabella Castiglioni
- Institute of Molecular Bioimaging and Physiology, National Research Council (IBFM-CNR), Milan, Italy
| | - Andrea Falini
- Department of Neuroradiology and CERMAC, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy
| | - Stefano F Cappa
- Institute for Advanced Study IUSS Pavia, Palazzo del Broletto - Piazza della Vittoria n.15, 27100, Pavia, Italy; Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Moreno-Martínez FJ, Rodríguez-Rojo IC. The Nombela 2.0 semantic battery: an updated Spanish instrument for the study of semantic processing. Neurocase 2015; 21:773-85. [PMID: 25645383 DOI: 10.1080/13554794.2015.1006644] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
In this study, the Nombela 2.0 semantic battery is presented. This is a new version of its earlier precedent: the battery Nombela (I), in an attempt to improve it (dealing with ceiling effects) and reducing the application time by decreasing the number of tasks. The battery is constructed on a common set of 98 stimuli, including both living and nonliving semantic domains. It consists of five tasks designed to explore category specificity by tapping semantic production and comprehension, using both visual and verbal input. All of the items were rated according to Spanish norms, as stated in a previous study of our group, and all of the tasks were matched across domain on six nuisance variables. The present study has two goals: (i) to make available the updated version (2.0) of the Nombela semantic memory battery and (ii) to characterize and compare the neuropsychological profiles of two different patient groups: mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease, with regard to normal controls.
Collapse
|
34
|
Catricalà E, Della Rosa PA, Plebani V, Vigliocco G, Cappa SF. Abstract and concrete categories? Evidences from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia 2014; 64:271-81. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2013] [Revised: 08/10/2014] [Accepted: 09/24/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
35
|
Brodeur MB, Guérard K, Bouras M. Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS) phase II: 930 new normative photos. PLoS One 2014; 9:e106953. [PMID: 25211489 PMCID: PMC4161371 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106953] [Citation(s) in RCA: 225] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2014] [Accepted: 08/03/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Researchers have only recently started to take advantage of the developments in technology and communication for sharing data and documents. However, the exchange of experimental material has not taken advantage of this progress yet. In order to facilitate access to experimental material, the Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS) project was created as a free standardized set of visual stimuli accessible to all researchers, through a normative database. The BOSS is currently the largest existing photo bank providing norms for more than 15 dimensions (e.g. familiarity, visual complexity, manipulability, etc.), making the BOSS an extremely useful research tool and a mean to homogenize scientific data worldwide. The first phase of the BOSS was completed in 2010, and contained 538 normative photos. The second phase of the BOSS project presented in this article, builds on the previous phase by adding 930 new normative photo stimuli. New categories of concepts were introduced, including animals, building infrastructures, body parts, and vehicles and the number of photos in other categories was increased. All new photos of the BOSS were normalized relative to their name, familiarity, visual complexity, object agreement, viewpoint agreement, and manipulability. The availability of these norms is a precious asset that should be considered for characterizing the stimuli as a function of the requirements of research and for controlling for potential confounding effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mathieu B. Brodeur
- Douglas Mental Health University Institute and Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montréal (Québec), Canada
- * E-mail:
| | - Katherine Guérard
- Department of Psychology, Université de Moncton, Moncton (New Brunswick), Canada
| | - Maria Bouras
- Department of Education, University of Sheffield, Sheffield (South Yorkshire), United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Giganti F, Viggiano MP. How semantic category modulates preschool children's visual memory. Child Neuropsychol 2014; 21:849-55. [PMID: 25089556 DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2014.945406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The dynamic interplay between perception and memory has been explored in preschool children by presenting filtered stimuli regarding animals and artifacts. The identification of filtered images was markedly influenced by both prior exposure and the semantic nature of the stimuli. The identification of animals required less physical information than artifacts did. Our results corroborate the notion that the human attention system evolves to reliably develop definite category-specific selection criteria by which living entities are monitored in different ways.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fiorenza Giganti
- a Department of Neurosciences, Psychology, Drug Research , Child Health University of Florence , Florence , Italy
| | - Maria Pia Viggiano
- a Department of Neurosciences, Psychology, Drug Research , Child Health University of Florence , Florence , Italy.,b Pediatric Psychology Service , Children's Hospital A. Meyer-University of Florence , Florence , Italy
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Giganti F, Arzilli C, Conte F, Toselli M, Viggiano MP, Ficca G. The effect of a daytime nap on priming and recognition tasks in preschool children. Sleep 2014; 37:1087-93. [PMID: 24882903 DOI: 10.5665/sleep.3766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022] Open
Abstract
STUDY OBJECTIVES The beneficial effect of sleep on memory consolidation is widely accepted in the adult population and has recently been shown in children. However, the few available data almost exclusively refer to school-aged children. Here we explore the effect of a daytime nap on memory consolidation in a sample of preschool children. DESIGN Subjects performed both a figures recognition task and a priming task, in order to differentiate effects on explicit and implicit memory. SETTING Nursery school. PARTICIPANTS Twenty-three children (mean age: 52.6 ± 8 mo; 13 males) participated in the study. INTERVENTION After a study phase in which children had to name 40 pictures of objects and animals, each subject either took an actigraphically monitored nap or stayed awake. At retest, children were administered both an implicit and an explicit memory task. MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS The implicit memory task consisted of naming 40 pictures presented at eight ascending levels of spatial filtering. The explicit memory task consisted of judging 40 pictures as old or new. The number of correct answers at the explicit recognition task was significantly higher in the nap compared to the wake condition, whereas priming effects did not differ between conditions. CONCLUSIONS A positive role of sleep in explicit memory consolidation, similar to the one observed in the adult, was detected in our sample of preschool children. In contrast, our data suggest that implicit perceptual learning, involved in priming tasks, does not benefit from sleep.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fiorenza Giganti
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Italy
| | - Cinzia Arzilli
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Italy
| | | | - Monica Toselli
- Department of Education and Psychology, University of Florence, Italy
| | - Maria Pia Viggiano
- Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Italy
| | - Gianluca Ficca
- Department of Psychology, University of Naples II, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Manipulating cues in involuntary autobiographical memory: Verbal cues are more effective than pictorial cues. Mem Cognit 2014; 42:1076-85. [DOI: 10.3758/s13421-014-0420-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
39
|
Abstract
Although many visual stimulus databases exist, none has data on item similarity levels for multiple items of each kind of stimulus. We present such data for 50 sets of grayscale object photographs. Similarity measures between pictures in each set (e.g., 25 different buttons) were collected using a similarity-sorting method (Goldstone, Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 26(4):381-386, 1994). A validation experiment used data from 1 picture set and compared responses from standard pairwise measures. This showed close agreement. The similarity-sorting measures were then standardized across picture sets, using pairwise ratings. Finally, the standardized similarity distances were validated in a recognition memory experiment; false alarms increased when targets and foils were more similar. These data will facilitate memory and perception research that needs to make comparisons between stimuli with a range of known target-foil similarities.
Collapse
|
40
|
Marconi D, Manenti R, Catricalà E, Della Rosa PA, Siri S, Cappa SF. The neural substrates of inferential and referential semantic processing. Cortex 2013; 49:2055-66. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2011] [Revised: 12/07/2011] [Accepted: 08/01/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
41
|
The bank of standardized stimuli (BOSS): comparison between French and English norms. Behav Res Methods 2013; 44:961-70. [PMID: 22351613 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-011-0184-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Throughout the last decades, numerous picture data sets have been developed, such as the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) set, and have been normalized for variables such as name and familiarity; however, due to cultural and linguistic differences, norms can vary from one country to another. The effect due specifically to culture has already been demonstrated by comparing samples from different countries where the same language is spoken. On the other hand, it is still not clear how differences between languages may affect norms. The present study explores this issue by collecting and comparing norms on names and many other features from French Canadian speakers and English Canadian speakers living in Montreal, who thus live in similar cultural environments. Norms were collected for the photos of objects from the Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS) by asking participants to name the objects, to categorize them, and to rate their familiarity, visual complexity, object agreement, viewpoint agreement, and manipulability. Names and ratings from the French speakers are available in Appendix A, available in the supplemental materials. The results show that most of the norms are comparable across linguistic groups and also that the ratings given are correlated across linguistic groups. The only significant group differences were found in viewpoint agreement and visual complexity. Overall, there was good concordance between the norms collected from French and English native speakers living in the same cultural setting.
Collapse
|
42
|
O’Sullivan M, Lepage M, Bouras M, Montreuil T, Brodeur MB. North-American norms for name disagreement: pictorial stimuli naming discrepancies. PLoS One 2012; 7:e47802. [PMID: 23133526 PMCID: PMC3485028 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047802] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2012] [Accepted: 09/20/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Pictorial stimuli are commonly used by scientists to explore central processes; including memory, attention, and language. Pictures that have been collected and put into sets for these purposes often contain visual ambiguities that lead to name disagreement amongst subjects. In the present work, we propose new norms which reflect these sources of name disagreement, and we apply this method to two sets of pictures: the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (S&V) set and the Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS). Naming responses of the presented pictures were classified within response categories based on whether they were correct, incorrect, or equivocal. To characterize the naming strategy where an alternative name was being used, responses were further divided into different sub-categories that reflected various sources of name disagreement. Naming strategies were also compared across the two sets of stimuli. Results showed that the pictures of the S&V set and the BOSS were more likely to elicit alternative specific and equivocal names, respectively. It was also found that the use of incorrect names was not significantly different across stimulus sets but that errors were more likely caused by visual ambiguity in the S&V set and by a misuse of names in the BOSS. Norms for name disagreement presented in this paper are useful for subsequent research for their categorization and elucidation of name disagreement that occurs when choosing visual stimuli from one or both stimulus sets. The sources of disagreement should be examined carefully as they help to provide an explanation of errors and inconsistencies of many concepts during picture naming tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mary O’Sullivan
- Department of Kinesiology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Martin Lepage
- Douglas Mental Health University Institute, & Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Maria Bouras
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Tina Montreuil
- Douglas Mental Health University Institute, & Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
- Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Mathieu B. Brodeur
- Douglas Mental Health University Institute, & Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Pisoni A, Papagno C, Cattaneo Z. Neural correlates of the semantic interference effect: New evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroscience 2012; 223:56-67. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2012] [Revised: 07/21/2012] [Accepted: 07/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
44
|
An Italian battery for the assessment of semantic memory disorders. Neurol Sci 2012; 34:985-93. [DOI: 10.1007/s10072-012-1181-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2012] [Accepted: 08/22/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
45
|
Moreno-Martínez FJ, Montoro PR. An ecological alternative to Snodgrass & Vanderwart: 360 high quality colour images with norms for seven psycholinguistic variables. PLoS One 2012; 7:e37527. [PMID: 22662166 PMCID: PMC3360784 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 117] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2012] [Accepted: 04/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
This work presents a new set of 360 high quality colour images belonging to 23 semantic subcategories. Two hundred and thirty-six Spanish speakers named the items and also provided data from seven relevant psycholinguistic variables: age of acquisition, familiarity, manipulability, name agreement, typicality and visual complexity. Furthermore, we also present lexical frequency data derived from Internet search hits. Apart from the high number of variables evaluated, knowing that it affects the processing of stimuli, this new set presents important advantages over other similar image corpi: (a) this corpus presents a broad number of subcategories and images; for example, this will permit researchers to select stimuli of appropriate difficulty as required, (e.g., to deal with problems derived from ceiling effects); (b) the fact of using coloured stimuli provides a more realistic, ecologically-valid, representation of real life objects. In sum, this set of stimuli provides a useful tool for research on visual object- and word-processing, both in neurological patients and in healthy controls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Pedro R. Montoro
- Departamento de Psicología Básica I, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Brancati C, Barba C, Metitieri T, Melani F, Pellacani S, Viggiano MP, Guerrini R. Impaired object identification in idiopathic childhood occipital epilepsy. Epilepsia 2012; 53:686-94. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03410.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
47
|
Pine KJ, Reeves L, Howlett N, Fletcher BC. Giving cognition a helping hand: The effect of congruent gestures on object name retrieval. Br J Psychol 2012; 104:57-68. [DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02098.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
48
|
The role of imagery-related properties in picture naming: A newly standardized set of 360 pictures for Japanese. Behav Res Methods 2012; 44:934-45. [DOI: 10.3758/s13428-011-0176-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
49
|
"It's a hair-dryer…No, it's a drill": misidentification-related false recognitions in younger and older adults. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2011; 54:310-6. [PMID: 21871676 DOI: 10.1016/j.archger.2011.06.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2011] [Revised: 06/21/2011] [Accepted: 06/22/2011] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Memory for visual objects, although typically highly accurate, can be distorted, especially in older adults. Here we asked whether also erroneous identifications of visual objects subsequently corrected and replaced by a correct identification might induce false recognitions, and whether this is more likely to occur in older people. For this aim a new paradigm was developed. In the first phase, participants performed a visual object identification task with degraded pictures of objects and produced correct and false but subsequently corrected identifications. In the second phase, participants performed a surprise recognition task in which also false identifications were presented. False identifications elicited false recognitions, with a stronger and more reliable effect in elderly participants, suggesting that correcting the initial visual error is not sufficient to correct the memory for the experience. Moreover, misidentification-related false recognitions coexisted in memory along with correct recognitions of correct identifications. These findings are discussed in relation with age-related deficits in memory updating and strategic retrieval.
Collapse
|
50
|
Viggiano MP, Marzi T, Forni M, Righi S, Franceschini R, Peru A. Semantic category effects modulate visual priming in neglect patients. Cortex 2011; 48:1128-37. [PMID: 21757194 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2010] [Revised: 01/31/2011] [Accepted: 05/24/2011] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies indicate that extinguished stimuli can still be unconsciously processed, leading to implicit priming effects. Here we investigated whether these implicit effects might be modulated by the semantic nature of the stimuli. Five neglect patients and ten controls performed an identification task of items belonging to living and non-living categories. In the study phase photographs of animals and artifacts were presented either to the left visual field (LVF) or to the right visual field (RVF). In the identification phase, each stimulus was displayed centrally and was revealed in a sequence of frames where the item was represented by an increasingly less and less filtered image up to a complete version. The results showed that lateralized stimuli differentially affected controls' and neglect patients' memory retrieval. In controls memory traces from the study phase served as efficient primes, thereby reducing the amount of information necessary for the identification of both stimulus categories. Moreover, hemispheric differences emerged with an advantage of the RVF/left hemisphere for artifact items, while no difference was found for living things. Neglect patients showed a priming effect for artifact items presented either to the RVF/left hemisphere or LVF/right hemisphere, as well as for living items presented to the RVF/left hemisphere, but not for living items presented to the LVF/right hemisphere. The priming effect observed for extinguished artifacts is consistent with the evidence of the existence of a specific mechanism destined to analyze, in an automatic and implicit fashion, motor-relevant information of manipulable objects and tools, which are important for identification process. Results are discussed in relation to current models of organization of conceptual knowledge within the framework of different processes performed by the two hemispheres.
Collapse
|