1
|
Samuelsson C, Marklund U, Lyxell B, Danielsson H. The use of gestures in book reading and play situations related to vocabulary in young children with cochlear implants. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2025:1-17. [PMID: 39791482 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2024.2449216] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2024] [Accepted: 12/18/2024] [Indexed: 01/12/2025]
Abstract
Gestures are essential in early language development. We investigate the use of gestures in children with cochlear implants (CIs), with a particular focus on deictic, iconic, and conventional gestures. The aim is to understand how the use of gestures in everyday interactions relates to age, vocabulary testing results, and language development reported by parents. Seven children with CIs, aged 27-39 months, participated in the study. The children's use of gestures was analysed in caregivers' video recordings of daily life interactions. The Picture Naming Game was used to test vocabulary. The Swedish version of CDI was used for caregiver reports of vocabulary and gesture use. All seven children used gestures, with deictic gestures being the most common type. Iconic gestures were more frequent during play than during book reading, and iconic gestures were more frequent when vocabulary was low. The results suggest that the use of gestures in combination with speech or vocalisations is related to vocabulary development. This connection is more pronounced during book reading, indicating the significance of this activity for language and gesture use in children with CIs. The study highlights the importance of gestures in communication and language development, especially in children with CIs. It emphasises that assessing gesture use should be a routine part of language assessments for children with challenges in their language development, such as hearing impairments. The study has some limitations, which are discussed in terms of small sample size and lack of detailed information on sign language use in the participating families.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christina Samuelsson
- Clinical Science Intervention and Technology, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | | | - Björn Lyxell
- BKV, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
- Specialpedagogik, Oslo University, Oslo, Norway
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Motamedi Y, Murgiano M, Grzyb B, Gu Y, Kewenig V, Brieke R, Donnellan E, Marshall C, Wonnacott E, Perniss P, Vigliocco G. Language development beyond the here-and-now: Iconicity and displacement in child-directed communication. Child Dev 2024; 95:1539-1557. [PMID: 38563146 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/04/2024]
Abstract
Most language use is displaced, referring to past, future, or hypothetical events, posing the challenge of how children learn what words refer to when the referent is not physically available. One possibility is that iconic cues that imagistically evoke properties of absent referents support learning when referents are displaced. In an audio-visual corpus of caregiver-child dyads, English-speaking caregivers interacted with their children (N = 71, 24-58 months) in contexts in which the objects talked about were either familiar or unfamiliar to the child, and either physically present or displaced. The analysis of the range of vocal, manual, and looking behaviors caregivers produced suggests that caregivers used iconic cues especially in displaced contexts and for unfamiliar objects, using other cues when objects were present.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yasamin Motamedi
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Margherita Murgiano
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Beata Grzyb
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Yan Gu
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, UK
| | - Viktor Kewenig
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Ricarda Brieke
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Ed Donnellan
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Chloe Marshall
- Institute of Education, University College London, London, UK
| | - Elizabeth Wonnacott
- Department of Language and Cognition, University College London, London, UK
- Department of Education, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | | | - Gabriella Vigliocco
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Kosie JE, Lew-Williams C. Infant-directed communication: Examining the many dimensions of everyday caregiver-infant interactions. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13515. [PMID: 38618899 PMCID: PMC11333185 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13515] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2023] [Revised: 03/22/2024] [Accepted: 03/30/2024] [Indexed: 04/16/2024]
Abstract
Everyday caregiver-infant interactions are dynamic and multidimensional. However, existing research underestimates the dimensionality of infants' experiences, often focusing on one or two communicative signals (e.g., speech alone, or speech and gesture together). Here, we introduce "infant-directed communication" (IDC): the suite of communicative signals from caregivers to infants including speech, action, gesture, emotion, and touch. We recorded 10 min of at-home play between 44 caregivers and their 18- to 24-month-old infants from predominantly white, middle-class, English-speaking families in the United States. Interactions were coded for five dimensions of IDC as well as infants' gestures and vocalizations. Most caregivers used all five dimensions of IDC throughout the interaction, and these dimensions frequently overlapped. For example, over 60% of the speech that infants heard was accompanied by one or more non-verbal communicative cues. However, we saw marked variation across caregivers in their use of IDC, likely reflecting tailored communication to the behaviors and abilities of their infant. Moreover, caregivers systematically increased the dimensionality of IDC, using more overlapping cues in response to infant gestures and vocalizations, and more IDC with infants who had smaller vocabularies. Understanding how and when caregivers use all five signals-together and separately-in interactions with infants has the potential to redefine how developmental scientists conceive of infants' communicative environments, and enhance our understanding of the relations between caregiver input and early learning. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Infants' everyday interactions with caregivers are dynamic and multimodal, but existing research has underestimated the multidimensionality (i.e., the diversity of simultaneously occurring communicative cues) inherent in infant-directed communication. Over 60% of the speech that infants encounter during at-home, free play interactions overlap with one or more of a variety of non-speech communicative cues. The multidimensionality of caregivers' communicative cues increases in response to infants' gestures and vocalizations, providing new information about how infants' own behaviors shape their input. These findings emphasize the importance of understanding how caregivers use a diverse set of communicative behaviors-both separately and together-during everyday interactions with infants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica E Kosie
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, USA
- School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona, USA
| | - Casey Lew-Williams
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Prahl A, McDaniel J. Strategies for Teaching Verbs to Children with and without Language Impairment. Semin Speech Lang 2023; 44:267-286. [PMID: 37758181 DOI: 10.1055/s-0043-1773785] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/03/2023]
Abstract
The aim of this feasibility study was to extend the current evidence base on intransitive verb learning by evaluating and comparing three strategies (syntactic cues, semantic cues, combined cues) for teaching novel verbs to expand the vocabularies of children with and without language impairment. Twenty-three children with typical development, seven children with developmental language disorder, and eight children with Down syndrome participated in Studies 1, 2, and 3, respectively. They were taught novel, intransitive verbs using syntactic, semantic, and combined cues and then asked to receptively identify and expressively label the novel verbs. Across all conditions, participants learned novel verbs receptively with large effect sizes and participants with typical development and Down syndrome also learned the verbs expressively with large effect sizes. There were no significant differences between conditions. This study extends word-learning research by evaluating not only receptive but also expressive intransitive verb learning to expand one's vocabulary. The results provide positive evidence for three effective strategies for teaching intransitive verbs to children with and without language impairment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alison Prahl
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Baylor University, Waco, Texas
| | - Jena McDaniel
- Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, Nashville, Tennessee
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Goppelt-Kunkel M, Stroh AL, Hänel-Faulhaber B. Sign learning of hearing children in inclusive day care centers-does iconicity matter? Front Psychol 2023; 14:1196114. [PMID: 37655202 PMCID: PMC10467423 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1196114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2023] [Accepted: 06/22/2023] [Indexed: 09/02/2023] Open
Abstract
An increasing number of experimental studies suggest that signs and gestures can scaffold vocabulary learning for children with and without special educational needs and/or disabilities (SEND). However, little research has been done on the extent to which iconicity plays a role in sign learning, particularly in inclusive day care centers. This current study investigated the role of iconicity in the sign learning of 145 hearing children (2;1 to 6;3 years) from inclusive day care centers with educators who started using sign-supported speech after a training module. Children's sign use was assessed via a questionnaire completed by their educators. We found that older children were more likely to learn signs with a higher degree of iconicity, whereas the learning of signs by younger children was less affected by iconicity. Children with SEND did not benefit more from iconicity than children without SEND. These results suggest that whether iconicity plays a role in sign learning depends on the age of the children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Madlen Goppelt-Kunkel
- Department of Special Education, Faculty of Education, Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Anna-Lena Stroh
- Department of Special Education, Faculty of Education, Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
- Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Barbara Hänel-Faulhaber
- Department of Special Education, Faculty of Education, Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Pronina M, Grofulovic J, Castillo E, Prieto P, Igualada A. Narrative Abilities at Age 3 Are Associated Positively With Gesture Accuracy but Negatively With Gesture Rate. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023; 66:951-965. [PMID: 36763840 DOI: 10.1044/2022_jslhr-21-00414] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/18/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Though the frequency of gesture use by infants has been related to the development of different language abilities in the initial stages of language acquisition, less is known about whether this frequency (or "gesture rate") continues to correlate with language measures in later stages of language acquisition, or whether the relation to language skills also depends on the accuracy with which such gestures are produced (or reproduced). This study sets out to explore whether preschoolers' narrative abilities are related to these two variables, namely, gesture rate and gesture accuracy. METHOD A total of 31 typically developing 3- to 4-year-old children participated in a multimodal imitation task, a context-based gesture elicitation task, and a narrative retelling task. RESULTS Results showed that there was a significant positive correlation between the children's narrative scores and their gesture accuracy scores, whereas higher rates of gesture use did not correlate with higher levels of narrative skill. Further multimodal regression analysis confirmed that gesture accuracy was a positive predictor of narrative performance, and moreover, showed that gesture rate was a negative predictor. CONCLUSIONS The fact that both gesture accuracy and gesture rate are strongly and differently linked to oral language abilities supports the claim that language and gesture are highly complex systems, and that complementary measures of gesture performance can help us assess with greater granularity the relationship between gesture and language development. These findings highlight the need to use gesture during clinical assessments as an informative indicator of language development and suggest that future research should further investigate the value of multimodal programs in the treatment of language and communication disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariia Pronina
- Department of Translation and Language Sciences, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | - Eva Castillo
- Department of Translation and Language Sciences, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Pilar Prieto
- Department of Translation and Language Sciences, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
- Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Alfonso Igualada
- Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain
- Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació, Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Rombouts E, Leenen L, Maes B, Zink I. Gesture-speech integration is related to vocabulary skills in children with developmental language disorder, Williams syndrome and typical development. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2023; 58:206-220. [PMID: 36036738 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12780] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2021] [Accepted: 07/27/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Individuals with developmental language disorder or Williams syndrome are reported to use more gestures than individuals with typical development. However, these two groups differ considerably in visuospatial and language skills, two skills that are hypothesized to shape gesture rate. AIMS We first examined whether children with both developmental language disorder and children Williams syndrome do indeed use more gestures. Our second aim was to disentangle the role of vocabulary and visuospatial skills in the use of supplementary gestures (i.e., containing unique information). To account for participant heterogeneity, analyses included both group comparisons and vocabulary and visuospatial skills at an individual level. As a third aim, the role of visuospatial skills was further examined in relation to gestures containing spatial content. METHODS & PROCEDURES In a cross-sectional group design, three participant groups watched and then retold a cartoon: children with typical development (n = 25), children with developmental language disorder (n = 25) and children/young people with Williams syndrome (n = 14). Their narrations were transcribed and hand gestures were coded based on gesture-speech integration (redundant, adds information to particular lexical items, gives information that is entirely absent from speech) and spatial content. Participants' expressive vocabulary and visuospatial skills were measured. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Between-group comparisons showed that individuals with developmental language disorder or Williams syndrome did indeed use more gestures. Poisson loglinear modelling demonstrated that a relative higher use of supplementary gestures was determined by lower expressive vocabulary skills. Neither the group distinction nor visuospatial skills shaped the supplementary gesture rate nor spatial gesture rate. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS Regardless of neurodevelopmental condition or typical development, a higher use of supplementary gestures was influenced by expressive vocabulary skills. Children with lower vocabulary skills spontaneously capitalized on the multimodality of communication to express constituents that were not present in their verbal speech. This finding is a promising starting point for future gesture intervention studies examining whether implicit modelling of gesture use can encourage gestures even more in these children and if this allows them to achieve higher linguistic complexity. On a methodological note, the observed intra-group skill variability demonstrates that group comparisons need to be complemented with correlational measures accounting for skills at an individual level. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on the subject Children with developmental language disorder and children with Williams syndrome are more inclined to use gestures than typically developing children. Research conducted in adults with typical development points towards the role of lexical and visuospatial skills in gesture use, but it is unclear how these skills shape gesture use in children with atypical development. What this paper adds to existing knowledge This study compares the rate of gestures that convey meaning that is not expressed in speech between the three aforementioned populations. Novel is the inclusion of the group distinction, individual lexical skills, and visuospatial skills in one encompassing statistical model. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? The inclination to use gestures that replace speech is related to lexical skills. Visuospatial skills do not seem to play a role and should not be considered as a factor when thinking about gesture intervention. Understanding how gestures relate to specific skills is a first step to understanding how gesture interventions can bolster language production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ellen Rombouts
- Department of Neurosciences, Experimental Otorhinolaryngology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Liesl Leenen
- Speech-Language Pathology & Audiology Sciences Master Program, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Bea Maes
- Parenting and Special Education Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Inge Zink
- Department of Neurosciences, Experimental Otorhinolaryngology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Lee C, Lew‐Williams C. The dynamic functions of social cues during children's word learning. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Crystal Lee
- Department of Psychology Princeton University Princeton New Jersey USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Goppelt-Kunkel M, Wienholz A, Hänel-Faulhaber B. Sign learning and its use in a co-enrollment kindergarten setting. Front Psychol 2022; 13:920497. [PMID: 36160579 PMCID: PMC9493243 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.920497] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2022] [Accepted: 07/25/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Experimental studies report positive effects of signing for language acquisition and communication in children with and without language development delays. However, little data are available on natural kindergarten settings. Therefore, our study used questionnaire data to investigate the sign learning in hearing children (aged 3;7–5;9 years) with and without language development delays in an inclusive kindergarten group with a co-enrolled deaf child (aged 3;8 years) and a deaf signing educator. We observed that the hearing children in this co-enrollment group learned more signs than the hearing children from groups with only hearing educators who learned signs in a training program. Hearing children’s sign learning showed a tendency toward correlating positively with their level of spoken language development. However, the individual background for children with language development delays impacted this relationship. Additionally, we examined the modality use of all children in interactions with hearing and deaf educators and peers using questionnaire and video data. Despite acquiring signs, hearing children predominantly used spoken language with hearing educators and predominantly nonverbal communication strategies with the deaf educator and the deaf child. Children with language development delays used code-blending with hearing educators in a few cases. The deaf child used mainly sign language for interactions with the deaf educator and mainly nonverbal communication with hearing educators and peers. Overall, our results suggest that the presence of a deaf educator increases sign learning in hearing children. However, in interactions during free play, they barely used signs making it particularly challenging for the deaf child to participate. This reveals that, in addition to a deaf role model, more sign language competent peers and targeted approaches increasing the use of the visual modality are required.
Collapse
|
10
|
Frizelle P, McKean C. Using Theory to Drive Intervention Efficacy: The Role of Dose Form in Interventions for Children with DLD. CHILDREN 2022; 9:children9060859. [PMID: 35740796 PMCID: PMC9221793 DOI: 10.3390/children9060859] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2022] [Revised: 05/09/2022] [Accepted: 06/05/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
‘Dose form’ is a construct that has evolved over the last number of years and is central to treating childhood language disorders. In this commentary, we present a framework of dose form that includes techniques, procedures, manner of instruction, and intervention context. We present key findings from a systematic review exploring the impact of intervention dose form on oral language outcomes (specifically morphosyntax and vocabulary learning) in children with DLD. We then discuss the hypothesized theoretical mechanisms of action underpinning these findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline Frizelle
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, University College Cork, T12 AK54 Cork, Ireland
- Correspondence:
| | - Cristina McKean
- Department of Speech and Language Sciences, School of Education, Communication & Language Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle NE1 7RU, UK;
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Foran LG, Beverly BL, Shelley-Tremblay J, Estis JM. Can gesture input support toddlers' fast mapping? JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2022; 50:1-23. [PMID: 35388778 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000922000149] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Forty-eight toddlers participated in a word-learning task to assess gesture input on mapping nonce words to unfamiliar objects. Receptive fast mapping and expressive naming for target object-word pairs were tested in three conditions - with a point, with a shape gesture, and in a no-gesture, word-only condition. No statistically significant effect of gesture for receptive fast-mapping was found but age was a factor. Two year olds outperformed one year olds for both measures. Only one girl in the one-year-old group correctly named any items. There was a significant interaction between gesture and gender for expressive naming. Two-year-old girls were six times more likely than two-year-old boys to correctly name items given point and shape gestures; whereas, boys named more items taught with the word only than with a point or shape gesture. The role of gesture input remains unclear, particularly for children under two years and for toddler boys.
Collapse
|
12
|
Factor L, Goffman L. Phonological characteristics of novel gesture production in children with developmental language disorder: Longitudinal findings. APPLIED PSYCHOLINGUISTICS 2022; 43:333-362. [PMID: 35342208 PMCID: PMC8955622 DOI: 10.1017/s0142716421000540] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Children with developmental language disorder (DLD; aka specific language impairment) are characterized based on deficits in language, especially morphosyntax, in the absence of other explanatory conditions. However, deficits in speech production, as well as fine and gross motor skill, have also been observed, implicating both the linguistic and motor systems. Situated at the intersection of these domains, and providing insight into both, is manual gesture. In the current work, we asked whether children with DLD showed phonological deficits in the production of novel gestures and whether gesture production at 4 years of age is related to language and motor outcomes two years later. Twenty-eight children (14 with DLD) participated in a two-year longitudinal novel gesture production study. At the first and final time points, language and fine motor skills were measured and gestures were analyzed for phonological feature accuracy, including handshape, path, and orientation. Results indicated that, while early deficits in phonological accuracy did not persist for children with DLD, all children struggled with orientation while handshape was the most accurate. Early handshape and orientation accuracy were also predictive of later language skill, but only for the children with DLD. Theoretical and clinical implications of these findings are discussed.
Collapse
|
13
|
Frizelle P, Tolonen AK, Tulip J, Murphy CA, Saldana D, McKean C. The Impact of Intervention Dose Form on Oral Language Outcomes for Children With Developmental Language Disorder. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2021; 64:3253-3288. [PMID: 34213951 DOI: 10.1044/2021_jslhr-20-00734] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The aim of this study was to extract key learning from intervention studies in which qualitative aspects of dosage, dose form, have been examined for children with developmental language disorder (DLD)-in vocabulary, morphosyntax, and phonology domains. This research paper emerged from a pair of systematic reviews, aiming to synthesize available evidence regarding qualitative and quantitative aspects of dosage. While quantitative aspects had been experimentally manipulated, the available evidence for dose form (tasks or activities within which teaching episodes are delivered) was less definitive. Despite this, the review uncovered insights of value to DLD research. Method A preregistered systematic review (PROSPERO ID: CRD42017076663) adhering to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines was completed. Included papers were quasi-experimental, randomized controlled trial, or cohort analytic studies, published in any language between January 2006 and May 2019; oral language interventions with vocabulary, morphosyntax, or phonology outcomes; and participants with DLD (M = 3-18 years). The intention was to include papers in which dose form was experimentally manipulated or statistically analyzed, while quantitative dosage aspects were controlled, such that definitive conclusions about optimal dose form could be drawn and gaps in the evidence identified. Results Two hundred and twenty-four papers met the above inclusion criteria; 27 focused on dose form. No study controlled for all quantitative aspects of dosage such that we could effectively address our original research questions. Despite this, key points of learning emerged with implications for future research. Conclusions There is tentative evidence of advantages for explicit over implicit instruction and of the benefits of variability in input, elicited production, and gestural and other visual supports. With careful design of dose form, there is potential to design more efficient interventions. Speech-language pathology research would benefit from an agreed taxonomy of dose form components and standardized reporting of intervention studies, to enable cross-study comparisons and a systematic accrual of knowledge to identify optimal dose form for clinical application.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline Frizelle
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, University College Cork, Ireland
| | - Anna-Kaisa Tolonen
- Research Unit of Logopedics, Faculty of Humanities, University of Oulu, Finland
| | - Josie Tulip
- Department of Speech and Language Sciences, School of Education, Communication & Language Sciences, Newcastle University, United Kingdom
| | - Carol-Anne Murphy
- Faculty of Education and Health Sciences, School of Allied Health, University of Limerick, Ireland
| | - David Saldana
- Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación, University of Seville, Spain
| | - Cristina McKean
- Department of Speech and Language Sciences, School of Education, Communication & Language Sciences, Newcastle University, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Kandana Arachchige KG, Simoes Loureiro I, Blekic W, Rossignol M, Lefebvre L. The Role of Iconic Gestures in Speech Comprehension: An Overview of Various Methodologies. Front Psychol 2021; 12:634074. [PMID: 33995189 PMCID: PMC8118122 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.634074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2020] [Accepted: 04/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Iconic gesture-speech integration is a relatively recent field of investigation with numerous researchers studying its various aspects. The results obtained are just as diverse. The definition of iconic gestures is often overlooked in the interpretations of results. Furthermore, while most behavioral studies have demonstrated an advantage of bimodal presentation, brain activity studies show a diversity of results regarding the brain regions involved in the processing of this integration. Clinical studies also yield mixed results, some suggesting parallel processing channels, others a unique and integrated channel. This review aims to draw attention to the methodological variations in research on iconic gesture-speech integration and how they impact conclusions regarding the underlying phenomena. It will also attempt to draw together the findings from other relevant research and suggest potential areas for further investigation in order to better understand processes at play during speech integration process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Wivine Blekic
- Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium
| | - Mandy Rossignol
- Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium
| | - Laurent Lefebvre
- Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Mons, Mons, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Mertens UJ, Rohlfing KJ. Progressive Reduction of Iconic Gestures Contributes to School-Aged Children's Increased Word Production. Front Psychol 2021; 12:651725. [PMID: 33981277 PMCID: PMC8107226 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2021] [Accepted: 03/30/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The economic principle of communication, according to which successful communication can be reached by least effort, has been studied for verbal communication. With respect to nonverbal behavior, it implies that forms of iconic gestures change over the course of communication and become reduced in the sense of less pronounced. These changes and their effects on learning are currently unexplored in relevant literature. Addressing this research gap, we conducted a word learning study to test the effects of changing gestures on children's slow mapping. We applied a within-subject design and tested 51 children, aged 6.7 years (SD = 0.4), who learned unknown words from a story. The storyteller acted on the basis of two conditions: In one condition, in which half of the target words were presented, the story presentation was enhanced with progressively reduced iconic gestures (PRG); in the other condition, half of the target words were accompanied by fully executed iconic gestures (FEG). To ensure a reliable gesture presentation, children were exposed to a recorded person telling a story in both conditions. We tested the slow mapping effects on children's productive and receptive word knowledge three minutes as well as two to three days after being presented the story. The results suggest that children's production of the target words, but not their understanding thereof, was enhanced by PRG.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ulrich J Mertens
- Psycholinguistics, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Katharina J Rohlfing
- Psycholinguistics, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Jackson E, Leitão S, Claessen M. Word learning and verbal working memory in children with developmental language disorder. AUTISM & DEVELOPMENTAL LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENTS 2021; 6:23969415211004109. [PMID: 36381524 PMCID: PMC9620694 DOI: 10.1177/23969415211004109] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/19/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND AIMS Previous research into word learning in children with developmental language disorder (DLD) indicates that the learning of word forms and meanings, rather than form-referent links, is problematic. This difficulty appears to arise with impaired encoding, while retention of word knowledge remains intact. Evidence also suggests that word learning skills may be related to verbal working memory. We aimed to substantiate these findings in the current study by exploring word learning over a series of days. METHODS Fifty children with DLD (mean age 6; 11, 72% male) and 54 age-matched typically developing (TD) children (mean age 6; 10, 56% male) were taught eight novel words across a four-day word learning protocol. Day 1 measured encoding, Days 2 and 3 measured re-encoding, and Day 4 assessed retention. At each day, word learning success was evaluated using Naming, Recognition, Description, and Identification tasks. RESULTS Children with DLD showed comparable performance to the TD group on the Identification task, indicating an intact ability to learn the form-referent links. In contrast, children with DLD performed significantly worse for Naming and Recognition (signifying an impaired ability to learn novel word forms), and for Description, indicating problems establishing new word meanings. These deficits for the DLD group were apparent at Days 1, 2, and 3 of testing, indicating impairments with initial encoding and re-encoding; however, the DLD and TD groups demonstrated a similar rate of learning. All children found the retention assessments at Day 4 difficult, and there were no significant group differences. Finally, verbal working memory emerged as a significant moderator of performance on the Naming and Recognition tasks, such that children with DLD and poor verbal working memory had the lowest levels of accuracy. CONCLUSIONS This study demonstrates that children with DLD struggle with learning novel word forms and meanings, but are unimpaired in their ability to establish new form-referent links. The findings suggest that the word learning deficit may be attributed to problems with encoding, rather than with retention, of new word knowledge; however, further exploration is required given the poor performance of both groups for retention testing. Furthermore, we found evidence that an impaired ability to learn word forms may only be apparent in children who have DLD and low levels of verbal working memory. IMPLICATIONS When working with children with DLD, speech-language pathologists should assess word learning using tasks that evaluate the ability to learn word forms, meanings, and form-referent links to develop a profile of individual word learning strengths and weaknesses. Clinicians should also assess verbal working memory to identify children at particular risk of word learning deficits. Future research should explore the notion of optimal intervention intensity for facilitating word learning in children with poor language and verbal working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Jackson
- Emily Jackson, School of Occupational
Therapy, Social Work, and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, GPO Box U1987,
Perth, Western Australia 6845, Australia.
| | | | - Mary Claessen
- School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and
Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Özer D, Göksun T. Gesture Use and Processing: A Review on Individual Differences in Cognitive Resources. Front Psychol 2020; 11:573555. [PMID: 33250817 PMCID: PMC7674851 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.573555] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2020] [Accepted: 09/29/2020] [Indexed: 01/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Speakers use spontaneous hand gestures as they speak and think. These gestures serve many functions for speakers who produce them as well as for listeners who observe them. To date, studies in the gesture literature mostly focused on group-comparisons or the external sources of variation to examine when people use, process, and benefit from using and observing gestures. However, there are also internal sources of variation in gesture use and processing. People differ in how frequently they use gestures, how salient their gestures are, for what purposes they produce gestures, and how much they benefit from using and seeing gestures during comprehension and learning depending on their cognitive dispositions. This review addresses how individual differences in different cognitive skills relate to how people employ gestures in production and comprehension across different ages (from infancy through adulthood to healthy aging) from a functionalist perspective. We conclude that speakers and listeners can use gestures as a compensation tool during communication and thinking that interacts with individuals' cognitive dispositions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Demet Özer
- Department of Psychology, Koç University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Lüke C, Ritterfeld U, Grimminger A, Rohlfing KJ, Liszkowski U. Integrated Communication System: Gesture and Language Acquisition in Typically Developing Children and Children With LD and DLD. Front Psychol 2020; 11:118. [PMID: 32116924 PMCID: PMC7010863 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2019] [Accepted: 01/15/2020] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Gesture and language development are strongly connected to each other. Two types of gestures in particular are analyzed regarding their role for language acquisition: pointing and iconic gestures. With the present longitudinal study, the predictive values of index-finger pointing at 12 months and the comprehension of iconic gestures at 3;0 years for later language skills in typically developing (TD) children and in children with a language delay (LD) or developmental language disorder (DLD) are examined. Forty-two monolingual German children and their primary caregivers participated in the study and were followed longitudinally from 1;0 to 6;0 years. Within a total of 14 observation sessions, the gestural and language abilities of the children were measured using standardized as well as ad hoc tests, parent questionnaires and semi-natural interactions between the child and their caregivers. At the age of 2;0 years, 10 of the 42 children were identified as having a LD. The ability to point with the extended index finger at 1;0 year is predictive for language skills at 5;0 and 6;0 years. This predictive effect is mediated by the language skills of the children at 3;0 years. The comprehension of iconic gestures at 3;0 years correlates with index-finger pointing at 1;0 year and also with earlier and later language skills. It mediates the predictive value of index-finger pointing at 1;0 year for grammar skills at 5;0 and 6;0 years. Children with LD develop the ability to understand the iconicity in gestures later than TD children and score lower in language tests until the age of 6;0 years. The language differences between these two groups of children persist partially until the age of 5;0 years even when the two children with manifested DLD within the group of children with LD are excluded from analyses. Beyond that age, no differences in the language skills between children with and without a history of LD are found when children with a manifest DLD are excluded. The findings support the assumption of an integrated speech–gesture communication system, which functions similarly in TD children and children with LD or DLD, but with a time delay.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carina Lüke
- Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Psycholinguistics, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany.,Department of Language and Communication, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, TU Dortmund University, Dortmund, Germany
| | - Ute Ritterfeld
- Department of Language and Communication, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, TU Dortmund University, Dortmund, Germany
| | - Angela Grimminger
- Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Psycholinguistics, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Katharina J Rohlfing
- Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Psycholinguistics, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Ulf Liszkowski
- Developmental Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Jackson E, Leitão S, Claessen M, Boyes M. The evaluation of word-learning abilities in people with developmental language disorder: a scoping review. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2019; 54:742-755. [PMID: 31276299 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12490] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2018] [Revised: 04/25/2019] [Accepted: 06/11/2019] [Indexed: 05/15/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The ability to learn new words is critical in the development of oral and written language, and significantly impacts engagement in social, academic and vocational situations. Many studies have evaluated the word-learning process in people with developmental language disorder (DLD). However, methodologies for assessment are heterogeneous, creating difficulties in synthesizing findings and identifying gaps in the knowledge base. AIMS To scope systematically the literature and identify key methodological parameters considered in evaluations of word learning in people with DLD; and to identify gaps in the literature to guide further research in this area. METHODS & PROCEDURES Twelve databases were searched and a total of 70 studies that met eligibility criteria were identified. The studies were evaluated according to key parameters that researchers varied in their word-learning methodologies. MAIN CONTRIBUTION Most research has focused on word learning in the oral modality, and specifically in children with DLD. Fewer studies have explored word learning in adults and adolescents with DLD, and in the written modality. Depending on the research question and theoretical perspective driving the investigation, methodologies for assessing word learning considered a range of parameters, including words being learned, learning context and cues to support learning in the tasks. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS This review aggregates a variety of methods used previously to assess word learning. Findings highlight the need for further research to explore areas such as: the learning of varied word types (e.g., adjectives and adverbs); learning in the written modality; and word learning (both oral and written) in adolescents and adults with DLD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Jackson
- School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Bentley, WA, Australia
| | - Suze Leitão
- School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Bentley, WA, Australia
| | - Mary Claessen
- School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Bentley, WA, Australia
| | - Mark Boyes
- School of Occupational Therapy, Social Work and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Bentley, WA, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
van Berkel-van Hoof L, Hermans D, Knoors H, Verhoeven L. Effects of Signs on Word Learning by Children With Developmental Language Disorder. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2019; 62:1798-1812. [PMID: 31158060 DOI: 10.1044/2019_jslhr-l-18-0275] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Purpose This study investigated the effects of signs on word learning by children with developmental language disorder (DLD), in comparison with typically developing (TD) children, and the relation between a possible sign effect and children's linguistic and cognitive abilities. Method Nine- to 11-year-old children with DLD ( n = 40) and TD children ( n = 26) participated in a word learning experiment. Half of the spoken pseudowords were taught with a pseudosign with learning outcomes being assessed in accuracy and speed. To investigate whether sign effects would hold for children with varying linguistic and cognitive abilities, we measured children's linguistic (vocabulary, syntax) and cognitive (divided attention, working memory [WM], lexical access) skills. Results The children with DLD showed a positive sign effect in both accuracy and speed. For the TD children, there was no effect of signs on word learning. Principal component analyses of the linguistic and cognitive measures evidenced a 4-component solution (language skills, visual WM, verbal WM, and executive attention). Repeated-measures analyses of covariance with the component scores as covariates yielded no significant interactions with the linguistic and cognitive components. Conclusions Our results suggest that children with DLD benefit from signs for word learning, regardless of their linguistic and cognitive abilities. This implies that using sign-supported speech as a means to improve the vocabulary skills of children with DLD is effective, even still at the age of 9-11 years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daan Hermans
- Royal Dutch Kentalis, Kentalis Academy, Sint-Michielsgestel, the Netherlands
| | - Harry Knoors
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
- Royal Dutch Kentalis, Kentalis Academy, Sint-Michielsgestel, the Netherlands
| | - Ludo Verhoeven
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Goodrich Smith W, Black AK, Hudson Kam CL. Learning speech-internal cues to pronoun interpretation from co-speech gesture: a training study. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2019; 46:433-458. [PMID: 30657105 PMCID: PMC6436995 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000918000557] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
This study explores whether children can learn a structural processing bias relevant to pronoun interpretation from brief training. Over three days, 42 five-year-olds were exposed to narratives exhibiting a first-mentioned tendency. Two characters were introduced, and the first-mentioned was later described engaging in a solo activity. In our primary condition of interest, the Gesture Training condition, the solo-activity sentence contained an ambiguous pronoun, but co-speech gesture clarified the referent. There were two comparison conditions. In the Gender Training condition the characters were different genders, thereby avoiding ambiguity. In the Name Training condition, the first-mentioned name was simply repeated. Ambiguous pronoun interpretation was tested pre- and post-training. Children in the Gesture condition were significantly more likely to interpret ambiguous pronouns as the first-mentioned character after training. Results from the comparison conditions were ambiguous: there was a small but non-significant effect of training, but also no significant differences between conditions.
Collapse
|
22
|
Vogt SS, Kauschke C. With Some Help From Others' Hands: Iconic Gesture Helps Semantic Learning in Children With Specific Language Impairment. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2017; 60:3213-3225. [PMID: 29098283 DOI: 10.1044/2017_jslhr-l-17-0004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2017] [Accepted: 06/15/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Semantic learning under 2 co-speech gesture conditions was investigated in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing (TD) children. Learning was analyzed between conditions. METHOD Twenty children with SLI (aged 4 years), 20 TD children matched for age, and 20 TD children matched for language scores were taught rare nouns and verbs. Children heard the target words while seeing either iconic gestures illustrating a property of the referent or a control gesture focusing children's attention on the word. Following training, children were asked to define the words' meaning. Responses were coded for semantic information provided on each word. RESULTS Performance of the SLI and age-matched groups proved superior to that of the language-matched group. Overall, children defined more words taught with iconic gestures than words taught with attention-getting gestures. However, only children with SLI, but not TD children, provided more semantic information on each word taught with iconic gestures. Performance did not differ in terms of word class. CONCLUSIONS Results suggest that iconic co-speech gestures help both children with and without SLI learn new words but, in particular, assist children with SLI understand and reflect the words' meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susanne S Vogt
- Department of Health and Social Affairs, University of Applied Sciences Fresenius, Idstein, Germany
| | - Christina Kauschke
- Department of Germanic Linguistics, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|