1
|
Edelman B, Del Vecchio T. When crying turns to hitting: Examining maternal responses to negative affect. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 74:101918. [PMID: 38199035 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2023] [Revised: 12/22/2023] [Accepted: 12/22/2023] [Indexed: 01/12/2024]
Abstract
Physical aggression in toddlerhood is empirically linked to anger and often conceptualized as a byproduct of frustration and related negative affect. Further, parenting is the major environmental construct implicated in the development of aggressive behaviors. Given parents' role as "external regulators," parents' responses to their toddlers' negative affect may serve to escalate or de-escalate their toddlers' affective experience, thereby impacting the likelihood of subsequent aggression. In the present study, we examined whether parents' negative affect, harsh, soothing, and distracting responses to their toddlers' negative affect mediated the relation between toddlers' negative affect and their aggressive behavior in brief conflict episodes. During a laboratory visit, a community sample of 69 mother-toddler dyads was observed in a structured interaction task. We found that child negative affect is associated with subsequent aggressive behavior by way of maternal harsh responses to negative affect. Negative emotional expression, soothing, and distraction neither facilitated or hindered children's escalation from negative affect to aggression. Our findings support a dyadic intervention in which patterns of coercive parent-child interactions are targets for prevention and intervention of toddler aggression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brooke Edelman
- Department of Psychology, St. John's University, New York, NY, United States.
| | - Tamara Del Vecchio
- Department of Psychology, St. John's University, New York, NY, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bonham MD, Hawkins E, Waters AM, Shanley DC. Can't Stop, Won't Stop? The Role of Inhibitory Control and Callous-Unemotional Traits in Childhood Conduct Problems and Aggression. Dev Neuropsychol 2022; 47:210-225. [PMID: 35470719 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2022.2069770] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2021] [Revised: 04/18/2022] [Accepted: 04/19/2022] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
Disruptive behaviors such as conduct problems and aggression are some of the most prevalent childhood psychological concerns. The etiology of disruptive behaviors is heterogenous and the relationships between the myriad risk factors that contribute to these problems are not yet fully understood. This study examined the relationship between inhibitory control and callous-unemotional traits (CU traits) with conduct problems and aggression in a community sample of children (aged 6 to 11 years). Caregivers (n= 148) completed a survey assessing a range of known risk factors (including hyperactivity and inattention). Children were found to display more conduct problems and aggression if they had greater difficulties with inhibitory control and a higher number of CU traits. Interestingly, when children had CU traits, inhibitory control difficulties exacerbated the severity of conduct problems (but not aggression). Differences in severity between conduct problems and aggression highlight the unique relationships between risk factors such as inhibitory control and CU traits, and lay the groundwork for future studies to explore the trajectories of this relationship.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mikaela D Bonham
- School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute of Queensland, Griffith University, Mount Gravatt and Southport, Queensland, Australia
| | - Erinn Hawkins
- School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute of Queensland, Griffith University, Mount Gravatt and Southport, Queensland, Australia
| | - Allison M Waters
- School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute of Queensland, Griffith University, Mount Gravatt and Southport, Queensland, Australia
| | - Dianne C Shanley
- School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute of Queensland, Griffith University, Mount Gravatt and Southport, Queensland, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Perlman SB, Lunkenheimer E, Panlilio C, Pérez-Edgar K. Parent-to-Child Anxiety Transmission Through Dyadic Social Dynamics: A Dynamic Developmental Model. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2022; 25:110-129. [PMID: 35195833 PMCID: PMC9990140 DOI: 10.1007/s10567-022-00391-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/14/2022] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
The intergenerational transmission of psychopathology is one of the strongest known risk factors for childhood disorder and may be a malleable target for prevention and intervention. Anxious parents have distinct parenting profiles that impact socioemotional development, and these parenting effects may result in broad alterations to the biological and cognitive functioning of their children. Better understanding the functional mechanisms by which parental risk is passed on to children can provide (1) novel markers of risk for socioemotional difficulties, (2) specific targets for intervention, and (3) behavioral and biological indices of treatment response. We propose a developmental model in which dyadic social dynamics serve as a key conduit in parent-to-child transmission of anxiety. Dyadic social dynamics capture the moment-to-moment interactions between parent and child that occur on a daily basis. In shaping the developmental trajectory from familial risk to actual symptoms, dyadic processes act on mechanisms of risk that are evident prior to, and in the absence of, any eventual disorder onset. First, we discuss dyadic synchrony or the moment-to-moment coordination between parent and child within different levels of analysis, including neural, autonomic, behavioral, and emotional processes. Second, we discuss how overt emotion modeling of distress is observed and internalized by children and later reflected in their own behavior. Thus, unlike synchrony, this is a more sequential process that cuts across levels of analysis. We also discuss maladaptive cognitive and affective processing that is often evident with increases in child anxiety symptoms. Finally, we discuss additional moderators (e.g., parent sex, child fearful temperament) that may impact dyadic processes. Our model is proposed as a conceptual framework for testing hypotheses regarding dynamic processes that may ultimately guide novel treatment approaches aimed at intervening on dyadically linked biobehavioral mechanisms before symptom onset.
Collapse
|
4
|
Simon P, Nader-Grosbois N. Preschoolers' Empathy Profiles and Their Social Adjustment. Front Psychol 2021; 12:782500. [PMID: 34956001 PMCID: PMC8704132 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.782500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/24/2021] [Accepted: 11/18/2021] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Preschoolers face new challenges in their social life: the development of social and emotional abilities in order to have positive relationships with peers and adults. Empathy, the ability to share and understand the emotions of others, contributes to this socio-emotional adjustment. This exploratory study examines mothers and fathers' perceptions of their child's empathy and individual factors, such as age, gender, and personality, which are related to cognitive and affective empathy in 63 typically developing preschoolers. Links between children's individual characteristics (empathy and personality) and their social adjustment on the one hand and risk of developing internalized vs. externalized behaviors on the other were also investigated. Parents completed four questionnaires about their child's empathy, personality, and social (mal)adjustment. The results showed that mothers and fathers perceived their children's cognitive and affective empathy, attention to others' feelings, and social actions (such as helping), in the same way, except for emotion contagion. Gender differences appeared specifically for some components of empathy: girls were said to pay more attention to others' emotions while boys had better cognitive empathy. Moreover, children's empathy as perceived by mothers or fathers was positively linked with their age, and with personality factors (extraversion, emotional stability, agreeableness, and openness to experience). Cognitive empathy and personality were found to be partly related to higher social skills and lower externalized and internalized behaviors. The results nuanced specific links between cognitive and affective empathy and social adjustment as well as behavior problems at preschool age. These results may have some implications for future research and prevention in childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Poline Simon
- Chair Baron Frère in Special Education, Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Nathalie Nader-Grosbois
- Chair Baron Frère in Special Education, Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Yoo HN, Smetana JG. Associations among child temperament, parenting, and young children's moral and conventional understanding: The moderating role of self‐regulation. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12571] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ha Na Yoo
- Department of Psychology University of Rochester Rochester New York USA
- Department of Psychology University of Utah Asia Campus Incheon South Korea
| | - Judith G. Smetana
- Department of Psychology University of Rochester Rochester New York USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Caporaso JS, Marcovitch S, Boseovski JJ. Executive function and the development of social information processing during the preschool years. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
7
|
Neppl TK, Jeon S, Diggs O, Donnellan MB. Positive parenting, effortful control, and developmental outcomes across early childhood. Dev Psychol 2020; 56:444-457. [PMID: 32077716 DOI: 10.1037/dev0000874] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The current study evaluated bidirectional associations between mother and father positive parenting and child effortful control. Data were drawn from 220 families when children were 3, 4, 5, and 6 years old. Parenting and effortful control were assessed when the child was 3, 4, and 5 years old. These variables were used to statistically predict child externalizing and school performance assessed when the child was 6 years old. The study used random intercept cross-lagged panel models to evaluate within-person and between-person associations between parenting and effortful control. Results suggest that prior positive parenting was associated with later effortful control, whereas effortful control was not associated with subsequent parenting from ages 3 to 5. Stable between-child differences in effortful control from ages 3 to 5 were associated with school performance at age 6. These stable between-child differences in effortful control were correlated with externalizing at age 3. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tricia K Neppl
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Iowa State University
| | - Shinyoung Jeon
- Early Childhood Education Institute, University of Oklahoma-Tulsa
| | - Olivia Diggs
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Iowa State University
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Zava F, Sette S, Baumgartner E, Coplan RJ. Shyness and empathy in early childhood: Examining links between feelings of empathy and empathetic behaviours. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020; 39:54-77. [PMID: 32918304 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12347] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2019] [Revised: 08/04/2020] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Although shy children have been described as less empathetic than their more sociable peers, this may be due to a performance rather than a competence deficit. The aim of this study was to explore the moderating role of shyness in the association between empathic feelings and empathic-related reactions. Participants were 212 preschoolers (Mage = 58.32 months, SD = 10.72). Children provided self-reports of empathetic feelings, parents rated child shyness and empathic behaviours (e.g., reparative behaviours), and teachers assessed indices of socio-emotional functioning (e.g., prosocial behaviours). Results revealed interaction effects between empathic feelings and shyness in the prediction of outcome variables. Among children with lower levels of shyness, empathy rated by children was positively related to empathetic and reparative behaviours (rated by parents) and prosocial behaviours (rated by teachers). At higher levels of shyness, these relations were attenuated. These results can be interpreted to suggest that although shy children may not differ from their more sociable counterparts in experiencing empathy, they seem to be less likely to act empathically.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Federica Zava
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
| | - Stefania Sette
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
| | - Emma Baumgartner
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
| | - Robert J Coplan
- Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Cristofani C, Sesso G, Cristofani P, Fantozzi P, Inguaggiato E, Muratori P, Narzisi A, Pfanner C, Pisano S, Polidori L, Ruglioni L, Valente E, Masi G, Milone A. The Role of Executive Functions in the Development of Empathy and Its Association with Externalizing Behaviors in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders and Other Psychiatric Comorbidities. Brain Sci 2020; 10:E489. [PMID: 32731515 PMCID: PMC7465618 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci10080489] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2020] [Revised: 07/21/2020] [Accepted: 07/21/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Executive functions have been previously shown to correlate with empathic attitudes and prosocial behaviors. People with higher levels of executive functions, as a whole, may better regulate their emotions and reduce perceived distress during the empathetic processes. Our goal was to explore the relationship between empathy and executive functioning in a sample of children and adolescents diagnosed with Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder alone or associated with comorbid Disruptive Behavior Disorders and/or Autism Spectrum Disorder. We also aimed to examine the role of empathic dimensions and executive skills in regulating externalizing behaviors. The 151 participants with ADHD were assigned to four groups according to their psychiatric comorbidity (either "pure" or with ASD and/or ODD/CD) and assessed by means of either parent- or self-reported questionnaires, namely the BRIEF-2, the BES, and the IRI. No questionnaire was found to discriminate between the four groups. Affective Empathy was found to positively correlate with Emotional and Behavioral Regulation competences. Furthermore, Aggressiveness and Oppositional Defiant Problems were positively associated with Executive Emotional and Behavioral Regulation competences. On the other hand, Rule-Breaking Behaviors and Conduct Problems were negatively associated with Affective Empathy and with Behavioral skills. Our study provides an additional contribution for a better understanding of the complex relationship between empathic competence and executive functions, showing that executive functioning and empathic attitudes interact with each other to regulate aggressive behaviors. This study further corroborates developmental models of empathy and their clinical implications, for which externalizing behaviors could be attenuated by enhancing executive functioning skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiara Cristofani
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Gianluca Sesso
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
| | - Paola Cristofani
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Pamela Fantozzi
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Emanuela Inguaggiato
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Pietro Muratori
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Antonio Narzisi
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Chiara Pfanner
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Simone Pisano
- Department of Neuroscience, AORN Santobono-Pausilipon, 80122 Naples, Italy;
- Department of Translational Medical Sciences, Federico II University, 80138 Naples, Italy
| | - Lisa Polidori
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Laura Ruglioni
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Elena Valente
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Gabriele Masi
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| | - Annarita Milone
- IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, 56128 Pisa (Calambrone), Italy; (C.C.); (G.S.); (P.C.); (P.F.); (E.I.); (P.M.); (A.N.); (C.P.); (L.P.); (L.R.); (E.V.); (G.M.)
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Dyadic synchrony and repair processes are related to preschool children's risk exposure and self-control. Dev Psychopathol 2020; 33:1072-1084. [PMID: 32524930 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579420000358] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
We examined associations between preschool children's cumulative risk exposure, dyadic interaction patterns, and self-control abilities in 238 mother-child dyads. Positive interactive synchrony, relationship ruptures, and latency to repair were micro-coded during a 3-5 minute joint challenge task. Children's self-control was assessed via two laboratory tasks and by parent report. Structural equation modeling and mediation analyses were utilized to examine the direct and indirect effects of cumulative risk on children's observed and parent-reported self-control abilities. Parent-child interactive processes of dyadic synchrony and latency to repair ruptures in synchrony were examined as mediators. Dyadic synchrony and latency to repair ruptures were found to mediate associations between cumulative risk exposure and children's behavioral and parent-reported self-control. Children exposed to more cumulative risk engaged in less dyadic synchrony and experienced longer latencies to repair ruptures with their caregiver, which in turn was associated with lower child self-control. Though cross-sectional, findings suggest dyadic synchrony and repair processes may represent viable mechanistic pathways linking cumulative risk exposure and deficits in child self-control. However, independent replications using longitudinal and experimental intervention designs are needed to determine causal pathways and inform new approaches for targeting the effects of early risk exposure through a focus on two-generational interventions.
Collapse
|
11
|
Plickert G, Pals H. Parental Anger and Trajectories of Emotional Well-Being from Adolescence to Young Adulthood. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE 2020; 30:440-457. [PMID: 31671254 DOI: 10.1111/jora.12536] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2018] [Revised: 09/10/2019] [Accepted: 09/21/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
This study assesses the relationship of parental anger on children's anger and self-derogation from adolescence to young adulthood. We examine a life-course perspective and incorporate theories of emotion regulation and self-referent behavior. Using structural equation models and hierarchical growth curve models with the Kaplan Longitudinal and Multigenerational data, the results indicate that parental anger leads to anger in adolescence, supporting theories of learning through modeling in adolescence. In young adulthood, as predicted by self-referent behavior theory, parental anger contributes to increases in self-derogation. Interestingly, maternal anger, but not paternal anger, is correlated with child's anger and self-derogation.
Collapse
|
12
|
Abramson L, Uzefovsky F, Toccaceli V, Knafo-Noam A. The genetic and environmental origins of emotional and cognitive empathy: Review and meta-analyses of twin studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 114:113-133. [PMID: 32353470 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2019] [Revised: 02/20/2020] [Accepted: 03/24/2020] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Empathy is considered a cornerstone of human social experience, and as such has been widely investigated from psychological and neuroscientific approaches. To better understand the factors influencing individual differences in empathy, we reviewed and meta-analyzed the behavioral genetic literature of emotional empathy- sharing others' emotions (k=13), and cognitive empathy- understanding others' emotions (k = 15), as manifested in twin studies. Results showed that emotional empathy is more heritable, 48.3 % [41.3 %-50.6 %], than cognitive empathy, 26.9 % [18.1 %-35.8 %]. Moreover, cognitive empathy as examined by performance tests was affected by the environment shared by family members, 11.9 % [2.6 %-21.0 %], suggesting that emotional understanding is influenced, to some degree, by environmental factors that have similar effects on family members beyond their genetic relatedness. The effects of participants' age and the method used to asses empathy on the etiology of empathy were also examined. These findings have implications for understanding how individual differences in empathy are formed. After discussing these implications, we suggest theoretical and methodological future research directions that could potentially elucidate the relations between genes, brain, and empathy.
Collapse
|
13
|
Caplan B, Morgan J, Noroña AN, Tung I, Lee SS, Baker BL. The nature and nurture of social development: The role of 5-HTTLPR and gene-parenting interactions. JOURNAL OF FAMILY PSYCHOLOGY : JFP : JOURNAL OF THE DIVISION OF FAMILY PSYCHOLOGY OF THE AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION (DIVISION 43) 2019; 33:927-937. [PMID: 31414862 PMCID: PMC6878128 DOI: 10.1037/fam0000572] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
Social skills are traditionally viewed as acquired through social environments including parenting. However, biopsychosocial models highlight the importance of genetic influences and gene-environment interactions (G×Es) in child development. Extant G×E investigations often fail to account for developmental changes in the phenotype or rigorously assess the social environment using observational measures. The present study prospectively assessed 110 children (44.5% female) and their parents to explore biologically plausible independent and interactive associations of the serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) and observed positive and negative parenting in prediction of (a) initial levels of social skills at school entry (age 6 years) and (b) developmental changes in social skills across the early school years (ages 6-9 years). Overall, the SS (vs. SL/LL) 5-HTTLPR genotype inversely predicted social skills across all domains, although parenting behavior moderated these associations wherein putative G×E effects differed by developmental timing and social skills domain. Positive parenting positively predicted concurrent (age 6) overall social skills for children with SL/LL genotypes, but not the SS genotype. However, for the SS group only, age 6 positive parenting positively predicted prospective growth in social responsibility, although negative parenting positively predicted growth in social cooperation. Findings suggest that 5-HTTLPR may signal differential sensitivities to parenting styles and patterns of social development, which may help to inform targeted intervention approaches to enhance person-environment fit. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Caplan
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles
| | - Julia Morgan
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles
| | | | - Irene Tung
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh
| | - Steve S. Lee
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles
| | - Bruce L. Baker
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Yan Z, Hong S, Liu F, Su Y. A meta‐analysis of the relationship between empathy and executive function. Psych J 2019; 9:34-43. [DOI: 10.1002/pchj.311] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2019] [Revised: 06/12/2019] [Accepted: 07/14/2019] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Zhiqiang Yan
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental HealthPeking University Beijing China
| | - Skylar Hong
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental HealthPeking University Beijing China
| | - Fuli Liu
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental HealthPeking University Beijing China
| | - Yanjie Su
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental HealthPeking University Beijing China
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Kraft SJ, Lowther E, Beilby J. The Role of Effortful Control in Stuttering Severity in Children: Replication Study. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2019; 28:14-28. [PMID: 30517950 PMCID: PMC6503866 DOI: 10.1044/2018_ajslp-17-0097] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2017] [Revised: 01/08/2018] [Accepted: 05/16/2018] [Indexed: 05/17/2023]
Abstract
Background In 2014, Kraft et al. assessed the temperament, home environment, and significant life events of 69 North American children who stutter to examine the combined and compounded effects of these individualized factors on mediating overt stuttering severity. The temperament domain of effortful control was singularly found to be significantly predictive of stuttering severity. Purpose Because of the clinical significance of the initial study's findings, a replication study with a different, larger cohort of children who stutter was warranted to validate the reported outcomes. Method The current study assesses 98 children who stutter, ages 2;4 to 12;6 (years; months, M = 6;7), recruited from Perth, Australia. Results The results support the previous findings of Kraft, Ambrose, and Chon (2014) , with effortful control remaining the sole significant contributor to variability in stuttering severity, as rated by both parents and clinicians. Conclusion These cumulative and consistent outcomes support the need to develop targeted intervention strategies that specifically strengthen aspects of effortful control as a means to support positive therapeutic change in children who stutter.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelly Jo Kraft
- Behavioral Speech & Genetics Lab, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI
| | - Emily Lowther
- Behavioral Speech & Genetics Lab, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI
| | - Janet Beilby
- Social Work and Speech Pathology, School of Occupational Therapy, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
The Dark Tetrad and callous reactions to mourner grief: Patterns of annoyance, boredom, entitlement, schadenfreude, and humor. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.08.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
17
|
Xiao SX, Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N. Longitudinal relations of preschoolers’ dispositional and situational anger to their prosocial behavior: The moderating role of shyness. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12346] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sonya Xinyue Xiao
- T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics Arizona State University Tempe Arizona
| | - Tracy L. Spinrad
- T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics Arizona State University Tempe Arizona
| | - Nancy Eisenberg
- Department of Psychology Arizona State University Tempe Arizona
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abramson L, Paz Y, Knafo-Noam A. From negative reactivity to empathic responding: Infants high in negative reactivity express more empathy later in development, with the help of regulation. Dev Sci 2018; 22:e12766. [PMID: 30339317 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2018] [Accepted: 10/10/2018] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Empathy has great effect on human well-being, promoting healthy relationships and social competence. Although it is increasingly acknowledged that infants show empathy toward others, individual differences in infants' empathy from the first year of life have rarely been investigated longitudinally. Here we examined how negative reactivity and regulation, two temperament traits that predict empathic responses in older children and adults, relate to infants' empathy. Infants were studied at the ages of nine (N = 275) and 18 (N = 301) months (194 infants were studied at both ages). Empathic responses were assessed by infants' observed reactions to an experimenter's simulated distress. Negative reactivity (fear, sadness, and distress to limitations) and regulation (soothability and effortful control) were assessed by parental reports. Negative reactivity was also examined by infants' observed reactions to an adult stranger (fear) and during interaction with their mothers (displays of sadness/distress). When examined cross-sectionally, infants' fear and distress to limitations associated with self-distress in response to others' distress. In contrast, when examined longitudinally, early sadness and distress to limitations, but not fear, associated with later empathic concern and inquisitiveness. Moreover, this longitudinal relation was moderated by infants' soothability and was evident only for children that had high soothability by the later time-point. Our findings suggest that infants who at an earlier age show negative reactivity, react later in development with more empathy if they achieve sufficient regulation abilities. By that, the findings stress the developmental nature of temperament-empathy relations during infancy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Yael Paz
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Children With Cochlear Implants and Their Parents: Relations Between Parenting Style and Children's Social-Emotional Functioning. Ear Hear 2018; 38:321-331. [PMID: 28002081 DOI: 10.1097/aud.0000000000000387] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Parenting a child who has a severe or profound hearing loss can be challenging and at times stressful, and might cause parents to use more adverse parenting styles compared with parents of hearing children. Parenting styles are known to impact children's social-emotional development. Children with a severe to profound hearing loss may be more reliant on their parents in terms of their social-emotional development when compared with their hearing peers who typically have greater opportunities to interact with and learn from others outside their family environment. Identifying the impact which parenting styles pertain on the social-emotional development of children who have cochlear implants (CIs) could help advance these children's well-being. Therefore, the authors compared parenting styles of parents with hearing children and of parents with children who have a CI, and examined the relations between parenting styles and two key aspects of children's social-emotional functioning: emotion regulation and empathy. DESIGN Ninety-two hearing parents and their children (aged 1 to 5 years old), who were either hearing (n = 46) or had a CI (n = 46), participated in this cross-sectional study. Parents completed questionnaires concerning their parenting styles (i.e., positive, negative and uninvolved), and regarding the extent to which their children expressed negative emotions (i.e., anger and sadness) and empathy. Furthermore, an emotion-regulation task measuring negative emotionality was administered to the children. RESULTS No differences in reported parenting styles were observed between parents of hearing children and parents of children with a CI. In addition, negative and uninvolved parenting styles were related to higher levels of negative emotionality in both groups of children. No relation was found between positive parenting and children's social-emotional functioning. Hearing status did not moderate these relationships. Language mediated the relationship between parenting styles and children's social-emotional functioning. CONCLUSIONS Children's hearing status did not impact parenting styles. This may be a result of the support that parents of children with a CI receive during their enrollment in the rehabilitation program preceding and after implantation. Rehabilitation programs should dedicate more attention to informing parents about the impact of parenting behaviors on children's social-emotional functioning. Offering parenting courses as part of the program could promote children's well-being. Future longitudinal research should address the directionality of the relations between parenting styles and children's social-emotional functioning.
Collapse
|
20
|
Parenting Profiles and Children's Social Skills: The Mediating Role of Effortful Control. ADONGHAKOEJI 2018. [DOI: 10.5723/kjcs.2018.39.1.89] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
21
|
Preschool children and chimpanzees incur costs to watch punishment of antisocial others. Nat Hum Behav 2017; 2:45-51. [DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0264-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2017] [Accepted: 11/09/2017] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
22
|
Coetzee SK, Laschinger HK. Toward a comprehensive, theoretical model of compassion fatigue: An integrative literature review. Nurs Health Sci 2017; 20:4-15. [DOI: 10.1111/nhs.12387] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2017] [Revised: 08/13/2017] [Accepted: 08/16/2017] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Siedine K. Coetzee
- School of Nursing Science; North-West University; Potchefstroom South Africa
| | - Heather K.S. Laschinger
- Arthur Labatt Family School of Nursing; University of Western Ontario; London Ontario Canada
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Emotion socialization and child conduct problems: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2017; 54:65-80. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2016] [Revised: 04/05/2017] [Accepted: 04/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
24
|
You and me: Investigating the role of self-evaluative emotion in preschool prosociality. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 155:67-83. [PMID: 27918978 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2015] [Revised: 10/06/2016] [Accepted: 11/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Self-evaluative emotions depend on internalized social standards and motivate social action. However, there is a lack of empirical research documenting the impact of self-evaluative emotion on 3- and 4-year-olds' prosociality. Extant research relates children's experiences of guilt to empathetic concern and making amends. However, the relationship between guilt and both concern and making amends is potentially reductive. Empathetic concern involves similar bodily expressions to guilt, and amend making is used to distinguish guilt from shame in children. This is the first study to relate the development of both positive and negative self-evaluative emotions to empathetic concern and prosocial choice (making amends and spontaneous help). Results confirm that the broad capacity for self-evaluative emotion is established during the preschool years and relates to empathetic concern. Moreover, these social emotions can be used to predict prosocial choice. Making amends was best predicted by empathetic concern and by children's responses to achievement (pride following success and lack of shame following failure). Alongside moral pride, pride in response to achievement and resilience to shame was also the best predictor of spontaneous help. The data support the idea that young children's prosocial choices may be partially driven by the affective drive to maintain an "ideal" self. Psychologists have emphasized that in order to be adaptive, self-evaluative emotion should be guilt oriented rather than shame oriented. However, the adaptive role of pride has been neglected. We call on future research to redress the focus on negative self-evaluation in moral development and further explore the prosocial potential of pride.
Collapse
|
25
|
Wang FL, Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL. Role of temperament in early adolescent pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems using a bifactor model: Moderation by parenting and gender. Dev Psychopathol 2016; 28:1487-1504. [PMID: 26646352 PMCID: PMC4900935 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579415001224] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
We contribute to the literature on the relations of temperament to externalizing and internalizing problems by considering parental emotional expressivity and child gender as moderators of such relations and examining prediction of pure and co-occurring problem behaviors during early to middle adolescence using bifactor models (which provide unique and continuous factors for pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems). Parents and teachers reported on children's (4.5- to 8-year-olds; N = 214) and early adolescents' (6 years later; N = 168) effortful control, impulsivity, anger, sadness, and problem behaviors. Parental emotional expressivity was measured observationally and with parents' self-reports. Early-adolescents' pure externalizing and co-occurring problems shared childhood and/or early-adolescent risk factors of low effortful control, high impulsivity, and high anger. Lower childhood and early-adolescent impulsivity and higher early-adolescent sadness predicted early-adolescents' pure internalizing. Childhood positive parental emotional expressivity more consistently related to early-adolescents' lower pure externalizing compared to co-occurring problems and pure internalizing. Lower effortful control predicted changes in externalizing (pure and co-occurring) over 6 years, but only when parental positive expressivity was low. Higher impulsivity predicted co-occurring problems only for boys. Findings highlight the probable complex developmental pathways to adolescent pure and co-occurring externalizing and internalizing problems.
Collapse
|
26
|
Huang H, Su Y, Jin J. Empathy-Related Responding in Chinese Toddlers: Factorial Structure and Cognitive Contributors. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2016; 26:e1983. [PMID: 28701902 PMCID: PMC5484378 DOI: 10.1002/icd.1983] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/10/2014] [Revised: 12/26/2015] [Accepted: 03/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
The critical role of the second year of life in the development of empathy is well accepted by psychologists. However, the developmental trends of the different components of empathy and the potential factors underlying these components during this critical period remain unclear. Eighty‐four Chinese toddlers in the second year of life participated in the present study. Empathy‐related responses were observed during three simulated procedures performed by each child's primary caregiver, the experimenter and a baby doll. An exploratory factor analysis was used to identify the different components of empathy. The shared representation of the self and others was measured using the Tasks for the Observation of Self‐Concept, self/other awareness was measured with a series of mirror tests and inhibitory control was measured using the Cylinder Inhibitory Control Task. The results showed that the empathy of Chinese toddlers contains three factors: sympathy, personal distress and orientation. Potential cognitive factors contribute to the different components of empathy through both independent and joint effects. © 2016 The Authors Infant and Child Development Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heqing Huang
- Department of Psychology and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health Peking University Beijing China.,College of Early Childhood Education Capital Normal University Beijing China
| | - Yanjie Su
- Department of Psychology and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health Peking University Beijing China
| | - Jian Jin
- Department of Psychology and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health Peking University Beijing China
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Vaish A. Flexible Concern: The Development of Multidetermined and Context-Dependent Empathic Responding. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2016. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
28
|
Main A, Zhou Q, Liew J, Lee C. Prosocial Tendencies among Chinese American Children in Immigrant Families: Links to Cultural and Socio-demographic Factors and Psychological Adjustment. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2016. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12182] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
29
|
The influence of effortful control and empathy on perception of school climate. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY OF EDUCATION 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s10212-015-0261-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
30
|
Taylor ZE, Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, effortful control, and parenting as predictors of children's sympathy across early childhood. Dev Psychol 2015; 51:17-25. [PMID: 25329555 PMCID: PMC4314214 DOI: 10.1037/a0038189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The goal of this study was to examine physiological and environmental predictors of children's sympathy (an emotional response consisting of feelings of concern or sorrow for others who are distressed or in need) and whether temperamental effortful control mediated these relations. Specifically, in a study of 192 children (23% Hispanic; 54% male), respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), a measure thought to reflect physiological regulation, and observed authoritative parenting (both at 42 months) were examined as predictors of children's effortful control (at 54 months) and, in turn, children's sympathy (at 72 and 84 months). Measures of both baseline RSA and RSA suppression were examined. In a structural equation model, observed parenting was positively related to children's subsequent sympathy through its positive relation to effortful control. Furthermore, the indirect path from baseline RSA to higher sympathy through effortful control was marginally significant. Authoritative parenting and baseline RSA uniquely predicted individual differences in children's effortful control. Findings highlight the potential role of both authoritative parenting and physiological regulation in the development of children's sympathy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zoe E Taylor
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies
| | | | - Tracy L Spinrad
- T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics, Arizona State University
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Colasante T, Zuffianò A, Bae NY, Malti T. Inhibitory Control and Moral Emotions: Relations to Reparation in Early and Middle Childhood. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 2014; 175:511-27. [DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2014.976535] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
32
|
López-Pérez B, Ambrona T. The role of cognitive emotion regulation on the vicarious emotional response. MOTIVATION AND EMOTION 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s11031-014-9452-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
|
33
|
Academic performance and social competence of adolescents: predictions based on effortful control and empathy. SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2014; 16:E87. [PMID: 24230950 DOI: 10.1017/sjp.2013.87] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
This study explored the predictive power of effortful control (EC) on empathy, academic performance, and social competence in adolescents. We obtained self-report measures of EC and dispositional empathy in 359 students (197 girls and 162 boys) aged between 12 and 14 years. Each student provided information about the prosocial behavior of the rest of his/her classmates and completed a sociogram. At the end of the school year, we calculated the mean grade of each student and the teacher responsible for each class completed a questionnaire on the academic skills of his/her students. The study confirmed the existence of a structural equation model (SEM) in which EC directly predicted academic performance and social competence. Additionally, empathic concern partially mediated the effect of EC on social competence. Finally, social competence significantly predicted academic performance. The article discusses the practical applications of the model proposed.
Collapse
|
34
|
Carnes-Holt K, Bratton SC. The Efficacy of Child Parent Relationship Therapy for Adopted Children With Attachment Disruptions. JOURNAL OF COUNSELING AND DEVELOPMENT 2014. [DOI: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2014.00160.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sue C. Bratton
- Department of Counseling and Higher Education, University of North Texas
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
This mixed-method study assessed the nature of language brokering and the relationship between language brokering and prosocial capacities in a sample of 139 college students from ethnically diverse immigrant families. The prosocial capacities of interest were empathic concern and two forms of perspective-taking: general perspective-taking (understanding the perspectives of others) and transcultural perspective-taking (understanding of divergent cultural values). As predicted, structural equation modeling identified a significant pathway from language brokering for parents to skill in transcultural perspective-taking. We illustrated this pathway with a qualitative case study. We also identified a significant bidirectional relationship between language brokering for others (e.g., other relatives, friends) and empathic concern. The experience of language brokering for others develops empathic concern; at the same time, those with higher levels of empathic concern broker more for people outside their immediate families.
Collapse
|
36
|
Interpretive understanding, sympathy, and moral emotion attribution in oppositional defiant disorder symptomatology. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2013; 44:633-45. [PMID: 23322355 DOI: 10.1007/s10578-013-0357-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the relations between interpretive understanding, sympathy, and moral emotion attribution (MEA) in the prediction of oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) symptomatology in an ethnically diverse sample of 128 4- and 8-year-old children (49 % girls). Caregivers rated the children's ODD symptoms. Interpretive understanding was assessed using an advanced theory-of-mind task. Sympathy was measured via caregiver- and child-report. Strength of MEA was assessed utilizing the children's responses to six hypothetical moral transgressions. Results revealed that interpretive understanding, sympathy, and strength of MEA in the exclusion domain predicted ODD symptoms negatively. Caregiver-reported sympathy partially mediated and moderated the relation between interpretive understanding and ODD symptoms. Strength of MEA in the rule violation domain moderated the relation between interpretive understanding and ODD symptoms. The findings shed light on the importance of social-cognitive and affective-moral antecedents of ODD symptoms.
Collapse
|
37
|
|
38
|
Stevenson M, Crnic K. Intrusive fathering, children's self-regulation and social skills: a mediation analysis. JOURNAL OF INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY RESEARCH : JIDR 2013; 57:500-512. [PMID: 22458354 DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01549.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Fathers have unique influences on children's development, and particularly in the development of social skills. Although father-child relationship influences on children's social competence have received increased attention in general, research on fathering in families of children with developmental delays (DD) is scant. This study examined the pathway of influence among paternal intrusive behaviour, child social skills and child self-regulatory ability, testing a model whereby child regulatory behaviour mediates relations between fathering and child social skills. METHODS Participants were 97 families of children with early identified DD enrolled in an extensive longitudinal study. Father and mother child-directed intrusiveness was coded live in naturalistic home observations at child age 4.5, child behaviour dysregulation was coded from a video-taped laboratory problem-solving task at child age 5, and child social skills were measured using independent teacher reports at child age 6. Analyses tested for mediation of the relationship between fathers' intrusiveness and child social skills by child behaviour dysregulation. RESULTS Fathers' intrusiveness, controlling for mothers' intrusiveness and child behaviour problems, was related to later child decreased social skills and this relationship was mediated by child behaviour dysregulation. CONCLUSIONS Intrusive fathering appears to carry unique risk for the development of social skills in children with DD. Findings are discussed as they related to theories of fatherhood and parenting in children with DD, as well as implications for intervention and future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Stevenson
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona 85287, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Heikamp T, Trommsdorff G, Druey MD, Hübner R, von Suchodoletz A. Kindergarten children's attachment security, inhibitory control, and the internalization of rules of conduct. Front Psychol 2013; 4:133. [PMID: 23543810 PMCID: PMC3608905 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00133] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2012] [Accepted: 03/04/2013] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Starting from research on relations between attachment and the development of self-regulation, the present study aimed to investigate research questions on relations among inhibitory control, internalization of rules of conduct (i.e., behavior regulation, concern occasioned by others transgressions, confession, reparation after wrongdoing), and attachment security. Attachment security and internalization of rules of conduct of German kindergarten children (N = 82) were assessed by maternal reports. Children's inhibitory control was measured with the Stop-task. Regression analyses revealed that inhibitory control was positively related to attachment security and to internalization of rules of conduct. Mediational analysis using a bootstrapping approach indicated an indirect effect of attachment security on internalization processes via inhibitory control. Implications for further research on the development of inhibitory control and internalization of rules of conduct are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tobias Heikamp
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Boucher M, Lecours S, Philippe F, Arseneault S. Parental socialization of emotion and depression in adulthood: The role of attitudes toward sadness. EUROPEAN REVIEW OF APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY-REVUE EUROPEENNE DE PSYCHOLOGIE APPLIQUEE 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.erap.2012.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
41
|
Razza RA, Raymond K. Associations among Maternal Behavior, Delay of Gratification, and School Readiness across the Early Childhood Years. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2012. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00665.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
42
|
Abstract
AbstractWe propose that neuroscientific understanding of antisocial behavior can be advanced by focusing programmatic efforts on neurobehavioral trait constructs, that is, individual difference constructs with direct referents in neurobiology as well as behavior. As specific examples, we highlight inhibitory control and defensive reactivity as two such constructs with clear relevance for understanding antisocial behavior in the context of development. Variations in inhibitory control are theorized to reflect individual differences in the functioning of brain systems that operate to guide and inhibit behavior and regulate emotional response in the service of nonimmediate goals. Variations in defensive reactivity are posited to reflect individual differences in the sensitivity of the brain's aversive motivational (fear) system. We describe how these constructs have been conceptualized in the adult and child literatures and review work pertaining to traditional psychometric (rating and behaviorally based) assessment of these constructs and their known physiological correlates at differing ages as well as evidence linking these constructs to antisocial behavior problems in children and adults. We outline a psychoneurometric approach, which entails systematic development of neurobiological measures of target trait constructs through reference to psychological phenotypes, as a paradigm for linking clinical disorders to neurobiological systems. We provide a concrete illustration of this approach in the domain of externalizing proneness and discuss its broader implications for research on conduct disorder, antisocial personality, and psychopathy.
Collapse
|
43
|
Factors Affecting Preschooler's Prosocial Behavior and Cognitive Problem-solving Ability : Focusing on the Parents' and Preschooler's Effortful Control, and Affectionate Parenting Behavior. ADONGHAKOEJI 2012. [DOI: 10.5723/kjcs.2012.33.3.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
44
|
Chen SH, Kennedy M, Zhou Q. Parents’ Expression and Discussion of Emotion in the Multilingual Family. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2012; 7:365-83. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691612447307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Parents regularly use words to express and discuss emotion with their children, but does it matter which language they use to do so? In this article, we examine this question in the multilingual family context by integrating findings from both psychological and linguistic research. We propose that parents’ use of different languages for emotional expression or discussion holds significant implications for children’s emotional experience, understanding, and regulation. Finally, we suggest that an understanding of the implications of emotion-related language shifts is critical, particularly in adapting interventions within a rapidly diversifying society.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen H. Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley
| | | | - Qing Zhou
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Batanova MD, Loukas A. What are the unique and interacting contributions of school and family factors to early adolescents' empathic concern and perspective taking? J Youth Adolesc 2012; 41:1382-91. [PMID: 22639382 DOI: 10.1007/s10964-012-9768-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2012] [Accepted: 04/18/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Empathy in children has received considerable attention in the literature, but limited research has investigated the contributions of various socializing factors on both affective (e.g., empathic concern) and cognitive (e.g., perspective taking) components of empathy in early adolescents. Guided by socialization theories, this study examined the unique and interacting contributions of school connectedness and parent-child conflict to subsequent levels of both components of empathy across a 1-year period of time. Participants were 487 10- to 14- year old middle school students (54 % female; 76 % European-American) involved in two waves of a study with 1 year between each wave. Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that, among girls, reports of parent-child conflict contributed to a decrease in empathic concern one year later, whereas school connectedness was a protective factor that offset the negative impact of parent-child conflict on girls' subsequent perspective taking. Alternatively, only boys' reports of school connectedness contributed to subsequent increases in both empathic concern and perspective taking 1 year later. Findings indicate that school connectedness and conflict with parents play different socializing roles for girls' and boys' empathic concern and perspective taking. The current study calls for further research and youth programs to consider the important contributions that socializing agents can make on both components of empathy for early adolescent girls and boys.
Collapse
|
46
|
Lau PSY, Wu FKY. Emotional competence as a positive youth development construct: a conceptual review. ScientificWorldJournal 2012; 2012:975189. [PMID: 22666176 PMCID: PMC3361327 DOI: 10.1100/2012/975189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2011] [Accepted: 08/31/2011] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The concept of emotional competence as a positive youth development construct is reviewed in this paper. Differences between emotional intelligence and emotional competence are discussed and an operational definition is adopted. Assessment methods of emotional competence with an emphasis on its quantitative nature are introduced. In the discussion of theories of emotional competence, the functionalist and developmental perspectives and the relationships with positive youth development are highlighted. Possible antecedents, especially the influence of early child-caregiver, and expected outcomes of emotional competence are examined. Practical ways to promote emotional competence among adolescents, particularly the role of parents and teachers, and the future direction of research are also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick S Y Lau
- Department of Educational Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
| | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Smetana JG, Rote WM, Jambon M, Tasopoulos-Chan M, Villalobos M, Comer J. Developmental Changes and Individual Differences in Young Children’s Moral Judgments. Child Dev 2012; 83:683-96. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01714.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
48
|
Kiff CJ, Lengua LJ, Zalewski M. Nature and nurturing: parenting in the context of child temperament. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2011; 14:251-301. [PMID: 21461681 PMCID: PMC3163750 DOI: 10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 287] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Accounting for both bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and child temperament can fine-tune theoretical models of the role of parenting and temperament in children's development of adjustment problems. Evidence for bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and children's characteristics of frustration, fear, self-regulation, and impulsivity was reviewed, and an overall model of children's individual differences in response to parenting is proposed. In general, children high in frustration, impulsivity and low in effortful control are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of negative parenting, while in turn, many negative parenting behaviors predict increases in these characteristics. Frustration, fearfulness, and effortful control also appear to elicit parenting behaviors that can predict increases in these characteristics. Irritability renders children more susceptible to negative parenting behaviors. Fearfulness operates in a very complex manner, sometimes increasing children's responses to parenting behaviors and sometimes mitigating them and apparently operating differently across gender. Important directions for future research include the use of study designs and analytic approaches that account for the direction of effects and for developmental changes in parenting and temperament over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cara J Kiff
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Box 351525, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
49
|
Kiff CJ, Lengua LJ, Zalewski M. Nature and nurturing: parenting in the context of child temperament. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2011. [PMID: 21461681 DOI: 10.1007/s10567-011-009304] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/22/2023]
Abstract
Accounting for both bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and child temperament can fine-tune theoretical models of the role of parenting and temperament in children's development of adjustment problems. Evidence for bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and children's characteristics of frustration, fear, self-regulation, and impulsivity was reviewed, and an overall model of children's individual differences in response to parenting is proposed. In general, children high in frustration, impulsivity and low in effortful control are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of negative parenting, while in turn, many negative parenting behaviors predict increases in these characteristics. Frustration, fearfulness, and effortful control also appear to elicit parenting behaviors that can predict increases in these characteristics. Irritability renders children more susceptible to negative parenting behaviors. Fearfulness operates in a very complex manner, sometimes increasing children's responses to parenting behaviors and sometimes mitigating them and apparently operating differently across gender. Important directions for future research include the use of study designs and analytic approaches that account for the direction of effects and for developmental changes in parenting and temperament over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cara J Kiff
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Box 351525, Seattle, WA 98195, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Zhou Q, Chen SH, Main A. Commonalities and Differences in the Research on Children’s Effortful Control and Executive Function: A Call for an Integrated Model of Self-Regulation. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2011. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00176.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 242] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|