1
|
Grove CR, Klatt BN, Wagner AR, Anson ER. Vestibular perceptual testing from lab to clinic: a review. Front Neurol 2023; 14:1265889. [PMID: 37859653 PMCID: PMC10583719 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1265889] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2023] [Accepted: 09/18/2023] [Indexed: 10/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Not all dizziness presents as vertigo, suggesting other perceptual symptoms for individuals with vestibular disease. These non-specific perceptual complaints of dizziness have led to a recent resurgence in literature examining vestibular perceptual testing with the aim to enhance clinical diagnostics and therapeutics. Recent evidence supports incorporating rehabilitation methods to retrain vestibular perception. This review describes the current field of vestibular perceptual testing from scientific laboratory techniques that may not be clinic friendly to some low-tech options that may be more clinic friendly. Limitations are highlighted suggesting directions for additional research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin R. Grove
- Department of Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- Division of Physical Therapy, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation School of Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States
| | - Brooke N. Klatt
- Physical Therapy Department, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Andrew R. Wagner
- Department of Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery, Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, OH, United States
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
| | - Eric R. Anson
- Department of Otolaryngology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, United States
- Physical Therapy Department, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, United States
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Does path integration contribute to human navigation in large-scale space? Psychon Bull Rev 2022:10.3758/s13423-022-02216-8. [DOI: 10.3758/s13423-022-02216-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/03/2022] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
3
|
Qi Y, Mou W, Lei X. Cue combination in goal-oriented navigation. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2021; 74:1981-2001. [PMID: 33885351 PMCID: PMC8902265 DOI: 10.1177/17470218211015796] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
This study examined cue combination of self-motion and landmark cues in
goal-localisation. In an immersive virtual environment, before walking a two-leg
path, participants learned the locations of three goal objects (one at the path
origin, that is, home) and landmarks. After walking the path without seeing
landmarks or goals, participants indicated the locations of the home and
non-home goals in four conditions: (1) path integration only, (2) landmarks
only, (3) both path integration and the landmarks, and (4) path integration and
rotated landmarks. The ratio of the length between the testing position (P) and
the turning point (T) over the length between the T and the three goals (G)
(i.e., PT/TG) was manipulated. The results showed the cue combination
consistently for participants’ heading estimates but not for goal-localisation.
In Experiments 1 and 2 (using distal landmarks), the cue combination for goal
estimates appeared in a small length ratio (PT/TG = 0.5) but disappeared in a
large length ratio (PT/TG = 2). In Experiments 3 and 4 (using proximal
landmarks), while the cue combination disappeared for the home with a medium
length ratio (PT/TG = 1), it appeared for the non-home goal with a large length
ratio (PT/TG = 2) and only disappeared with a very large length ratio
(PT/TG = 3). These findings are explained by a model stipulating that cue
combination occurs in self-localisation (e.g., heading estimates), which leads
to one estimate of the goal location; proximal landmarks produce another goal
location estimate; these two goal estimates are then combined, which may only
occur for non-home goals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yafei Qi
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
| | - Weimin Mou
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
| | - Xuehui Lei
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
The influence of yaw rotation on spatial navigation during development. Neuropsychologia 2021; 154:107774. [PMID: 33600832 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107774] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Revised: 11/19/2020] [Accepted: 02/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Sensory cues enable navigation through space, as they inform us about movement properties, such as the amount of travelled distance and the heading direction. In this study, we focused on the ability to spatially update one's position when only proprioceptive and vestibular information is available. We aimed to investigate the effect of yaw rotation on path integration across development in the absence of visual feedback. To this end, we utilized the triangle completion task: participants were guided through two legs of a triangle and asked to close the shape by walking along its third imagined leg. To test the influence of yaw rotation across development, we tested children between 6 and 11 years old (y.o.) and adults on their perceptions of angles of different degrees. Our results demonstrated that the amount of turn while executing the angle influences performance at all ages, and in some aspects, also interacted with age. Indeed, whilst adults seemed to adjust their heading towards the end of their walked path, younger children took less advantage of this strategy. The amount of disorientation the path induced also affected participants' full maturational ability to spatially navigate with no visual feedback. Increasing induced disorientation required children to be older to reach adult-level performance. Overall, these results provide novel insights on the maturation of spatial navigation-related processes.
Collapse
|
5
|
Grove CR, Heiderscheit BC, Pyle GM, Loyd BJ, Whitney SL. The Gait Disorientation Test: A New Method for Screening Adults With Dizziness and Imbalance. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2020; 102:582-590. [PMID: 33338462 DOI: 10.1016/j.apmr.2020.11.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2020] [Revised: 11/18/2020] [Accepted: 11/20/2020] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To develop and evaluate a new method for identifying gait disorientation due to vestibular dysfunction. DESIGN The gait disorientation test (GDT) involves a timed comparison of the ability to walk 6.096 m with eyes open versus eyes closed. In this prospective study, participants were grouped based on vestibular function. All participants completed a clinical examination, self-report- and performance-based measures relevant to vestibular rehabilitation, and the tasks for the GDT. Vestibular-impaired participants underwent the criterion standard, videonystagmography and/or rotational chair testing. SETTING Ambulatory clinic, tertiary referral center. PARTICIPANTS Participants (N=40) (20 vestibular-impaired, 30 women, 49.9±16.1years old) were enrolled from a convenience/referral sample of 52 adults. MAIN OUTCOME AND MEASURE(S) We determined test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient model 3,1; calculated the minimal detectable change (MDC); examined concurrent validity through Spearman correlation coefficients; assessed criterion validity with the area under the curve (AUC) from receiver operator characteristic analysis; and computed the sensitivity, specificity, diagnostic odds ratio (DOR), likelihood ratios for positive (LR+) and negative (LR-) tests, and posttest probabilities of a diagnosis of vestibulopathy. The 95% confidence interval demonstrates measurement uncertainty. RESULTS Test-retest reliability was 0.887 (0.815, 0.932). The MDC was 3.7 seconds. Correlations with other measures ranged from 0.59 (0.34, 0.76) to -0.85 (-0.92, -0.74). The AUC was 0.910 (0.822, 0.998), using a threshold of 4.5 seconds. The sensitivity and specificity were 0.75 (0.51, 0.91) and 0.95 (0.75, 1), respectively. The DOR=57 (6, 541.47), LR+ =15 (2.18, 103.0), and LR- =0.26 (0.12, 0.9). Positive posttest probabilities were 89%-94%. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE The GDT has good reliability, excellent discriminative ability, strong convergent validity, and promising clinical utility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin R Grove
- School of Medicine and Public Health, Department of Surgery, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI; Institute for Clinical and Translational Research, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI.
| | - Bryan C Heiderscheit
- Institute for Clinical and Translational Research, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI; Department of Orthopedics and Rehabilitation, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI
| | - G Mark Pyle
- School of Medicine and Public Health, Department of Surgery, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI
| | - Brian J Loyd
- Department of Physical Therapy and Rehabilitation Science, University of Montana, Missoula, MT; Department of Physical Therapy and Athletic Training, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT
| | - Susan L Whitney
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Harootonian SK, Wilson RC, Hejtmánek L, Ziskin EM, Ekstrom AD. Path integration in large-scale space and with novel geometries: Comparing vector addition and encoding-error models. PLoS Comput Biol 2020; 16:e1007489. [PMID: 32379824 PMCID: PMC7244182 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007489] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2019] [Revised: 05/22/2020] [Accepted: 03/24/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Path integration is thought to rely on vestibular and proprioceptive cues yet most studies in humans involve primarily visual input, providing limited insight into their respective contributions. We developed a paradigm involving walking in an omnidirectional treadmill in which participants were guided on two sides of a triangle and then found their back way to origin. In Experiment 1, we tested a range of different triangle types while keeping the distance of the unguided side constant to determine the influence of spatial geometry. Participants overshot the angle they needed to turn and undershot the distance they needed to walk, with no consistent effect of triangle type. In Experiment 2, we manipulated distance while keeping angle constant to determine how path integration operated over both shorter and longer distances. Participants underestimated the distance they needed to walk to the origin, with error increasing as a function of the walked distance. To attempt to account for our findings, we developed configural-based computational models involving vector addition, the second of which included terms for the influence of past trials on the current one. We compared against a previously developed configural model of human path integration, the Encoding-Error model. We found that the vector addition models captured the tendency of participants to under-encode guided sides of the triangles and an influence of past trials on current trials. Together, our findings expand our understanding of body-based contributions to human path integration, further suggesting the value of vector addition models in understanding these important components of human navigation. How do we remember where we have been? One important mechanism for doing so is called path integration, which refers to the computation of one’s position in space with only self-motion cues. By tracking the direction and distance we have walked, we can create a mental arrow from the current location to the origin, termed the homing vector. Previous studies have shown that the homing vector is subject to systematic distortions depending on previously experienced paths, yet what influences these patterns of errors, particularly in humans, remains uncertain. In this study, we compare two models of path integration based on participants walking two sides of a triangle without vision and then completing the third side based on their estimate of the homing vector. We found no effect of triangle shape on systematic errors, while the systematic errors scaled with path length logarithmically, similar to Weber-Fechner law. While we show that both models captured participants’ behavior, a model based on vector addition best captured the patterns of error in the homing vector. Our study therefore has important implications for how humans track their location, suggesting that vector-based models provide a reasonable and simple explanation for how we do so.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sevan K. Harootonian
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
- Psychology Department, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
| | - Robert C. Wilson
- Psychology Department, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
- Cognitive Science Program, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
- Evelyn McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
| | - Lukáš Hejtmánek
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
- Third Faculty of Medicine, Charles University, Ruská, Prague, Czech Republic
| | - Eli M. Ziskin
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
- Psychology Department, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
| | - Arne D. Ekstrom
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
- Psychology Department, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
- Evelyn McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Piccardi L, Palmiero M, Bocchi A, Boccia M, Guariglia C. How does environmental knowledge allow us to come back home? Exp Brain Res 2019; 237:1811-1820. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-019-05552-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2018] [Accepted: 04/29/2019] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
|
8
|
Zajac L, Burte H, Taylor HA, Killiany R. Self-reported navigation ability is associated with optic flow-sensitive regions' functional connectivity patterns during visual path integration. Brain Behav 2019; 9:e01236. [PMID: 30884216 PMCID: PMC6456774 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.1236] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2018] [Accepted: 01/16/2019] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Spatial navigation is a complex cognitive skill that varies between individuals, and the mechanisms underlying this variability are not clear. Studying simpler components of spatial navigation may help illuminate factors that contribute to variation in this complex skill; path integration is one such component. Optic flow provides self-motion information while moving through an environment and is sufficient for path integration. This study aims to investigate whether self-reported navigation ability is related to information transfer between optic flow-sensitive (OF-sensitive) cortical regions and regions important to navigation during environmental spatial tasks. METHODS Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to define OF-sensitive regions and map their functional connectivity (FC) with the retrosplenial cortex and hippocampus during visual path integration (VPI) and turn counting (TC) tasks. Both tasks presented visual self-motion through a real-world environment. Correlations predicting a positive association between self-reported navigation ability (measured with the Santa Barbara Sense of Direction scale) and FC strength between OF-sensitive regions and retrosplenial cortex and OF-sensitive regions and the hippocampus were performed. RESULTS During VPI, FC strength between left cingulate sulcus visual area (L CSv) and right retrosplenial cortex and L CSv and right hippocampus was positively associated with self-reported navigation ability. FC strength between right cingulate sulcus visual area (R CSv) and right retrosplenial cortex during VPI was also positively associated with self-reported navigation ability. These relationships were specific to VPI, and whole-brain exploratory analyses corroborated these results. CONCLUSIONS These findings support the hypothesis that perceived spatial navigation ability is associated with communication strength between OF-sensitive and navigationally relevant regions during visual path integration, which may represent the transformation accuracy of visual motion information into internal spatial representations. More broadly, these results illuminate underlying mechanisms that may explain some variability in spatial navigation ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren Zajac
- Department of Anatomy & Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts.,Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Heather Burte
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts
| | - Holly A Taylor
- Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts
| | - Ronald Killiany
- Department of Anatomy & Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts.,Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Descloux V, Maurer R. Perspective taking to assess topographical disorientation: Group study and preliminary normative data. APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY-ADULT 2019; 27:199-218. [DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2018.1528262] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Virginie Descloux
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Genève, Switzerland
| | - Roland Maurer
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Genève, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Chrastil ER, Sherrill KR, Hasselmo ME, Stern CE. Which way and how far? Tracking of translation and rotation information for human path integration. Hum Brain Mapp 2016; 37:3636-55. [PMID: 27238897 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23265] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2016] [Revised: 05/03/2016] [Accepted: 05/13/2016] [Indexed: 12/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Path integration, the constant updating of the navigator's knowledge of position and orientation during movement, requires both visuospatial knowledge and memory. This study aimed to develop a systems-level understanding of human path integration by examining the basic building blocks of path integration in humans. To achieve this goal, we used functional imaging to examine the neural mechanisms that support the tracking and memory of translational and rotational components of human path integration. Critically, and in contrast to previous studies, we examined movement in translation and rotation tasks with no defined end-point or goal. Navigators accumulated translational and rotational information during virtual self-motion. Activity in hippocampus, retrosplenial cortex (RSC), and parahippocampal cortex (PHC) increased during both translation and rotation encoding, suggesting that these regions track self-motion information during path integration. These results address current questions regarding distance coding in the human brain. By implementing a modified delayed match to sample paradigm, we also examined the encoding and maintenance of path integration signals in working memory. Hippocampus, PHC, and RSC were recruited during successful encoding and maintenance of path integration information, with RSC selective for tasks that required processing heading rotation changes. These data indicate distinct working memory mechanisms for translation and rotation, which are essential for updating neural representations of current location. The results provide evidence that hippocampus, PHC, and RSC flexibly track task-relevant translation and rotation signals for path integration and could form the hub of a more distributed network supporting spatial navigation. Hum Brain Mapp 37:3636-3655, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth R Chrastil
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts.,Massachusetts General Hospital, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
| | - Katherine R Sherrill
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts.,Massachusetts General Hospital, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
| | - Michael E Hasselmo
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Chantal E Stern
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts.,Massachusetts General Hospital, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Legge GE, Gage R, Baek Y, Bochsler TM. Indoor Spatial Updating with Reduced Visual Information. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0150708. [PMID: 26943674 PMCID: PMC4778963 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150708] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2015] [Accepted: 02/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose Spatial updating refers to the ability to keep track of position and orientation while moving through an environment. People with impaired vision may be less accurate in spatial updating with adverse consequences for indoor navigation. In this study, we asked how artificial restrictions on visual acuity and field size affect spatial updating, and also judgments of the size of rooms. Methods Normally sighted young adults were tested with artificial restriction of acuity in Mild Blur (Snellen 20/135) and Severe Blur (Snellen 20/900) conditions, and a Narrow Field (8°) condition. The subjects estimated the dimensions of seven rectangular rooms with and without these visual restrictions. They were also guided along three-segment paths in the rooms. At the end of each path, they were asked to estimate the distance and direction to the starting location. In Experiment 1, the subjects walked along the path. In Experiment 2, they were pushed in a wheelchair to determine if reduced proprioceptive input would result in poorer spatial updating. Results With unrestricted vision, mean Weber fractions for room-size estimates were near 20%. Severe Blur but not Mild Blur yielded larger errors in room-size judgments. The Narrow Field was associated with increased error, but less than with Severe Blur. There was no effect of visual restriction on estimates of distance back to the starting location, and only Severe Blur yielded larger errors in the direction estimates. Contrary to expectation, the wheelchair subjects did not exhibit poorer updating performance than the walking subjects, nor did they show greater dependence on visual condition. Discussion If our results generalize to people with low vision, severe deficits in acuity or field will adversely affect the ability to judge the size of indoor spaces, but updating of position and orientation may be less affected by visual impairment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gordon E. Legge
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, Minnesota, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Rachel Gage
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, Minnesota, United States of America
| | - Yihwa Baek
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, Minnesota, United States of America
| | - Tiana M. Bochsler
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, Minnesota, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Bias in Human Path Integration Is Predicted by Properties of Grid Cells. Curr Biol 2015; 25:1771-6. [PMID: 26073138 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2014] [Revised: 04/01/2015] [Accepted: 05/14/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Accurate wayfinding is essential to the survival of many animal species and requires the ability to maintain spatial orientation during locomotion. One of the ways that humans and other animals stay spatially oriented is through path integration, which operates by integrating self-motion cues over time, providing information about total displacement from a starting point. The neural substrate of path integration in mammals may exist in grid cells, which are found in dorsomedial entorhinal cortex and presubiculum and parasubiculum in rats. Grid cells have also been found in mice, bats, and monkeys, and signatures of grid cell activity have been observed in humans. We demonstrate that distance estimation by humans during path integration is sensitive to geometric deformations of a familiar environment and show that patterns of path integration error are predicted qualitatively by a model in which locations in the environment are represented in the brain as phases of arrays of grid cells with unique periods and decoded by the inverse mapping from phases to locations. The periods of these grid networks are assumed to expand and contract in response to expansions and contractions of a familiar environment. Biases in distance estimation occur when the periods of the encoding and decoding grids differ. Our findings explicate the way in which grid cells could function in human path integration.
Collapse
|
13
|
Yamamoto N, Meléndez JA, Menzies DT. Homing by path integration when a locomotion trajectory crosses itself. Perception 2015; 43:1049-60. [PMID: 25509682 DOI: 10.1068/p7624] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Path integration is a process with which navigators derive their current position and orientation by integrating self-motion signals along a locomotion trajectory. It has been suggested that path integration becomes disproportionately erroneous when the trajectory crosses itself. However, there is a possibility that this previous finding was confounded by effects of the length of a traveled path and the amount of turns experienced along the path, two factors that are known to affect path integration performance. The present study was designed to investigate whether the crossover of a locomotion trajectory truly increases errors of path integration. In an experiment, blindfolded human navigators were guided along four paths that varied in their lengths and turns, and attempted to walk directly back to the beginning of the paths. Only one of the four paths contained a crossover. Results showed that errors yielded from the path containing the crossover were not always larger than those observed in other paths, and the errors were attributed solely to the effects of longer path lengths or greater degrees of turns. These results demonstrated that path crossover does not always cause significant disruption in path integration processes. Implications of the present findings for models of path integration are discussed.
Collapse
|
14
|
Ekstrom AD, Arnold AEGF, Iaria G. A critical review of the allocentric spatial representation and its neural underpinnings: toward a network-based perspective. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8:803. [PMID: 25346679 PMCID: PMC4193251 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00803] [Citation(s) in RCA: 146] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2014] [Accepted: 09/21/2014] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
While the widely studied allocentric spatial representation holds a special status in neuroscience research, its exact nature and neural underpinnings continue to be the topic of debate, particularly in humans. Here, based on a review of human behavioral research, we argue that allocentric representations do not provide the kind of map-like, metric representation one might expect based on past theoretical work. Instead, we suggest that almost all tasks used in past studies involve a combination of egocentric and allocentric representation, complicating both the investigation of the cognitive basis of an allocentric representation and the task of identifying a brain region specifically dedicated to it. Indeed, as we discuss in detail, past studies suggest numerous brain regions important to allocentric spatial memory in addition to the hippocampus, including parahippocampal, retrosplenial, and prefrontal cortices. We thus argue that although allocentric computations will often require the hippocampus, particularly those involving extracting details across temporally specific routes, the hippocampus is not necessary for all allocentric computations. We instead suggest that a non-aggregate network process involving multiple interacting brain areas, including hippocampus and extra-hippocampal areas such as parahippocampal, retrosplenial, prefrontal, and parietal cortices, better characterizes the neural basis of spatial representation during navigation. According to this model, an allocentric representation does not emerge from the computations of a single brain region (i.e., hippocampus) nor is it readily decomposable into additive computations performed by separate brain regions. Instead, an allocentric representation emerges from computations partially shared across numerous interacting brain regions. We discuss our non-aggregate network model in light of existing data and provide several key predictions for future experiments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arne D Ekstrom
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis Davis, CA, USA ; Department of Psychology, University of California at Davis Davis, CA, USA ; Neuroscience Graduate Group, University of California at Davis Davis, CA, USA
| | - Aiden E G F Arnold
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis Davis, CA, USA ; Department of Psychology, Hotchkiss Brain Institute and Alberta Children's Hospital Research Institute, University of Calgary Calgary, AB, Canada
| | - Giuseppe Iaria
- Department of Psychology, Hotchkiss Brain Institute and Alberta Children's Hospital Research Institute, University of Calgary Calgary, AB, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
Research investigating how people remember the distance of paths they walk has shown two apparently conflicting effects of experience during encoding on subsequent distance judgments. By the feature accumulation effect, discrete path features such as turns, houses, or other landmarks cause an increase in remembered distance. By the distractor effect, performance of a concurrent task during path encoding causes a decrease in remembered distance. In this study, we ask the following: What are the conditions that determine whether the feature accumulation or the distractor effect dominates distortions of space? In two experiments, blindfolded participants were guided along two legs of a right triangle while reciting nonsense syllables. On some trials, one of the two legs contained features: horizontally mounted car antennas (gates) that bent out of the way as participants walked past. At the end of the second leg, participants either indicated the remembered path leg lengths using their hands in a ratio estimation task or attempted to walk, unguided, straight back to the beginning. In addition to response mode, visual access to the paths and time between encoding and response were manipulated to determine whether these factors would affect feature accumulation or distractor effects. Path legs with added features were remembered as shorter than those without, but this result was significant only in the haptic response mode data. This finding suggests that when people form spatial memory representations with the intention of navigating in room-scale spaces, interfering with information accumulation substantially distorts spatial memory.
Collapse
|
16
|
Yamamoto N, Philbeck JW, Woods AJ, Gajewski DA, Arthur JC, Potolicchio SJ, Levy L, Caputy AJ. Medial temporal lobe roles in human path integration. PLoS One 2014; 9:e96583. [PMID: 24802000 PMCID: PMC4011851 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096583] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2014] [Accepted: 04/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Path integration is a process in which observers derive their location by integrating self-motion signals along their locomotion trajectory. Although the medial temporal lobe (MTL) is thought to take part in path integration, the scope of its role for path integration remains unclear. To address this issue, we administered a variety of tasks involving path integration and other related processes to a group of neurosurgical patients whose MTL was unilaterally resected as therapy for epilepsy. These patients were unimpaired relative to neurologically intact controls in many tasks that required integration of various kinds of sensory self-motion information. However, the same patients (especially those who had lesions in the right hemisphere) walked farther than the controls when attempting to walk without vision to a previewed target. Importantly, this task was unique in our test battery in that it allowed participants to form a mental representation of the target location and anticipate their upcoming walking trajectory before they began moving. Thus, these results put forth a new idea that the role of MTL structures for human path integration may stem from their participation in predicting the consequences of one's locomotor actions. The strengths of this new theoretical viewpoint are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Naohide Yamamoto
- Department of Psychology, Cleveland State University, Cleveland, Ohio, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
| | - John W. Philbeck
- Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
- School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Adam J. Woods
- Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
- Department of Aging and Geriatric Research, Cognitive Aging and Memory Clinical Translational Research Program, Institute on Aging, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Daniel A. Gajewski
- Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
| | - Joeanna C. Arthur
- Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
- Office of Basic & Applied Research, National Geospatial-Intelligence Agency, Springfield, Virginia, United States of America
| | - Samuel J. Potolicchio
- Department of Neurology, George Washington University Medical Center, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
| | - Lucien Levy
- Department of Radiology, George Washington University Medical Center, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
| | - Anthony J. Caputy
- Department of Neurological Surgery, George Washington University Medical Center, Washington, District of Columbia, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
|
18
|
|
19
|
Lafon M, Vidal M, Berthoz A. Selective influence of prior allocentric knowledge on the kinesthetic learning of a path. Exp Brain Res 2009; 194:541-52. [PMID: 19229527 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-1728-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2007] [Accepted: 01/26/2009] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Matthieu Lafon
- Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de l'Action (LPPA), CNRS Collège de France, 11 Place Marcelin Berthelot, 75005 Paris, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Action and the representation of distance in cognitive maps acquired through imagined traversal: The development of a new methodology. Mem Cognit 2008; 36:518-33. [DOI: 10.3758/mc.36.3.518] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
21
|
Ellard CG, Wagar LS. Plasticity of the Association between Visual Space and Action Space in a Blind-Walking Task. Perception 2008; 37:1044-53. [DOI: 10.1068/p5798] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Many experiments have shown that a brief visual preview provides sufficient information to complete certain kinds of movements (reaching, grasping, and walking) with high precision. This suggests that participants must possess a calibration between visual target location and the kinaesthetic, proprioceptive, and/or vestibular stimulation generated during movement towards the target. We investigated the properties of this calibration using a cue-conflict paradigm in which participants were trained with mismatched locomotor and visual input. After training, participants were presented with visual targets and were asked to either walk to them or locate them in a spatial updating task. Our results showed that the training was sufficient to produce significant, systematic miscalibrations of the association between visual space and action space. These findings suggest that the association between action space and visual space is modifiable by experience. This plasticity could be either due to modification of a simple, task-specific sensory motor association or it could reflect a change in the gain of a path integration signal or a reorganisation of the relationship between perceived space and action space. We suggest further experiments that might help to distinguish between these possibilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin G Ellard
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON N2L 3G1, Canada
| | - Lori S Wagar
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON N2L 3G1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Arthur JC, Philbeck JW, Chichka D. Spatial memory enhances the precision of angular self-motion updating. Exp Brain Res 2007; 183:557-68. [PMID: 17684736 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-1075-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2006] [Accepted: 07/19/2007] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Humans are typically able to keep track of brief changes in their head and body orientation, even when visual and auditory cues are temporarily unavailable. Determining the magnitude of one's displacement from a known location is one form of self-motion updating. Most research on self-motion updating during body rotations has focused on the role of a restricted set of sensory signals (primarily vestibular) available during self-motion. However, humans can and do internally represent spatial aspects of the environment, and little is known about how remembered spatial frameworks may impact angular self-motion updating. Here, we describe an experiment addressing this issue. Participants estimated the magnitude of passive, non-visual body rotations (40 degrees -130 degrees ), using non-visual manual pointing. Prior to each rotation, participants were either allowed full vision of the testing environment, or remained blindfolded. Within-subject response precision was dramatically enhanced when the body rotations were preceded by a visual preview of the surrounding environment; constant (signed) and absolute (unsigned) error were much less affected. These results are informative for future perceptual, cognitive, and neuropsychological studies, and demonstrate the powerful role of stored spatial representations for improving the precision of angular self-motion updating.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joeanna C Arthur
- Department of Psychology, The George Washington University, 2125 G. Street, NW, Washington, DC 20052, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Allen GL, Kirasic KC, Rashotte MA, Haun DBM. Aging and path integration skill: kinesthetic and vestibular contributions to wayfinding. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2004; 66:170-9. [PMID: 15095949 DOI: 10.3758/bf03194870] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In a triangle completion task designed to assess path integration skill, younger and older adults performed similarly after being led, while blindfolded, along the route segments on foot, which provided both kinesthetic and vestibular information about the outbound path. In contrast, older adults' performance was impaired, relative to that of younger adults, after they were conveyed, while blindfolded, along the route segments in a wheelchair, which limited them principally to vestibular information. Correlational evidence suggested that cognitive resources were significant factors in accounting for age-related decline in path integration performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gary L Allen
- Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina 29208, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Sun HJ, Campos JL, Young M, Chan GSW, Ellard CG. The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and optic flow in distance estimation. Perception 2004; 33:49-65. [PMID: 15035328 DOI: 10.1068/p5145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
By systematically varying cue availability in the stimulus and response phases of a series of same-modality and cross-modality distance matching tasks, we examined the contributions of static visual information, idiothetic information, and optic flow information. The experiment was conducted in a large-scale, open, outdoor environment. Subjects were presented with information about a distance and were then required to turn 180 before producing a distance estimate. Distance encoding and responding occurred via: (i) visually perceived target distance, or (ii) traversed distance through either blindfolded locomotion or during sighted locomotion. The results demonstrated that subjects performed with similar accuracy across all conditions. In conditions in which the stimulus and the response were delivered in the same mode, when visual information was absent, constant error was minimal; whereas, when visual information was present, overestimation was observed. In conditions in which the stimulus and response modes differed, a consistent error pattern was observed. By systematically comparing complementary conditions, we found that the availability of visual information during locomotion (particularly optic flow) led to an 'under-perception' of movement relative to conditions in which visual information was absent during locomotion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hong-Jin Sun
- Department of Psychology, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Philbeck JW, Behrmann M, Levy L, Potolicchio SJ, Caputy AJ. Path Integration Deficits during Linear Locomotion after Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16:510-20. [PMID: 15165344 DOI: 10.1162/089892904323057254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Animal navigation studies have implicated structures in and around the hippocampal formation as crucial in performing path integration (a method of determining one's position by monitoring internally generated self-motion signals). Less is known about the role of these structures for human path integration. We tested path integration in patients who had undergone left or right medial temporal lobectomy as therapy for epilepsy. This procedure removed approximately 50% of the anterior portion of the hippocampus, as well as the amygdala and lateral temporal lobe. Participants attempted to walk without vision to a previously viewed target 2–6 m distant. Patients with right, but not left, hemisphere lesions exhibited both a decrease in the consistency of path integration and a systematic underregistration of linear displacement (and/or velocity) during walking. Moreover, the deficits were observable even when there were virtually no angular acceleration vestibular signals. The results suggest that structures in the medial temporal lobe participate in human path integration when individuals walk along linear paths and that this is so to a greater extent in right hemisphere structures than left. This information is relevant for future research investigating the neural substrates of navigation, not only in humans (e.g., functional neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies), but also in rodents and other animals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John W Philbeck
- Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington DC 20052, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Féry YA, Magnac R, Israël I. Commanding the direction of passive whole-body rotations facilitates egocentric spatial updating. Cognition 2004; 91:B1-10. [PMID: 14738773 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
In conditions of slow passive transport without vision, even tenuous inertial signals from semi-circular canals and the haptic-kinaesthetic system should provide information about changes relative to the environment provided that it is possible to command the direction of the body's movements voluntarily. Without such control, spatial updating should be impaired because incoming signals cannot be compared to the expected sensory consequences provided by voluntary command. Participants were seated in a rotative robot (Robuter) and learnt the positions of five objects in their surroundings. They were then blindfolded and assigned either to the active group (n=7) or to the passive group (n=7). Members of the active group used a joystick to control the direction of rotation of the robot. The acceleration (25 degrees /s2) and plateau velocity (9 degrees /s) were kept constant. The participants of the passive group experienced the same stimuli passively. After the rotations, the participants had to point to the objects whilst blindfolded. Participants in the active group significantly outperformed the participants in the passive group. Thus, even tenuous inertial cues are useful for spatial updating in the absence of vision, provided that such signals are integrated as feedback associated with intended motor command.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yves-André Féry
- Laboratoire Actes, UPRES EA n. 3596, UFR STAPS, Université des Antilles et de la Guyane, Pointe à Pitre, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Mou W, McNamara TP, Valiquette CM, Rump B. Allocentric and Egocentric Updating of Spatial Memories. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2004; 30:142-57. [PMID: 14736303 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.142] [Citation(s) in RCA: 197] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In 4 experiments, the authors investigated spatial updating in a familiar environment. Participants learned locations of objects in a room, walked to the center, and turned to appropriate facing directions before making judgments of relative direction (e.g., "Imagine you are standing at X and facing Y. Point to Z.") or egocentric pointing judgments (e.g., "You are facing Y. Point to Z."). Experiments manipulated the angular difference between the learning heading and the imagined heading and the angular difference between the actual heading and the imagined heading. Pointing performance was best when the imagined heading was parallel to the learning view, even when participants were facing in other directions, and when actual and imagined headings were the same. Room geometry did not affect these results. These findings indicated that spatial reference directions in memory were not updated during locomotion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weimin Mou
- Chinese Academy of Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Beijing, China
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
28
|
Ellard CG, Shaughnessy SC. A comparison of visual and nonvisual sensory inputs to walked distance in a blind-walking task. Perception 2003; 32:567-78. [PMID: 12854643 DOI: 10.1068/p5041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments were conducted in order to assess the contribution of locomotor information to estimates of egocentric distance in a walking task. In the first experiment, participants were either shown, or led blind to, a target located at a distance ranging from 4 to 10 m and were then asked to indicate the distance to the target by walking to the location previously occupied by the target. Participants in both the visual and locomotor conditions were very accurate in this task and there was no significant difference between conditions. In the second experiment, a cue-conflict paradigm was used in which, without the knowledge of the participants, the visual and locomotor targets (the targets they were asked to walk to) were at two different distances. Most participants did not notice the conflict, but despite this their responses showed evidence that they had averaged the visual and locomotor inputs to arrive at a walked estimate of distance. Together, these experiments demonstrate that, although they showed poor awareness of their position in space without vision, in some conditions participants were able to use such nonvisual information to arrive at distance estimates as accurate as those given by vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin G Ellard
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Klatzky RL, Lippa Y, Loomis JM, Golledge RG. Encoding, learning, and spatial updating of multiple object locations specified by 3-D sound, spatial language, and vision. Exp Brain Res 2003; 149:48-61. [PMID: 12592503 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1334-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2002] [Accepted: 10/30/2002] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Participants standing at an origin learned the distance and azimuth of target objects that were specified by 3-D sound, spatial language, or vision. We tested whether the ensuing target representations functioned equivalently across modalities for purposes of spatial updating. In experiment 1, participants localized targets by pointing to each and verbalizing its distance, both directly from the origin and at an indirect waypoint. In experiment 2, participants localized targets by walking to each directly from the origin and via an indirect waypoint. Spatial updating bias was estimated by the spatial-coordinate difference between indirect and direct localization; noise from updating was estimated by the difference in variability of localization. Learning rate and noise favored vision over the two auditory modalities. For all modalities, bias during updating tended to move targets forward, comparably so for three and five targets and for forward and rightward indirect-walking directions. Spatial language produced additional updating bias and noise from updating. Although spatial representations formed from language afford updating, they do not function entirely equivalently to those from intrinsically spatial modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roberta L Klatzky
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213,USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Loomis JM, Lippa Y, Golledge RG, Klatzky RL. Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-d sound and spatial language. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2002; 28:335-45. [PMID: 11911388 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.2.335] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Blind and blindfolded sighted observers were presented with auditory stimuli specifying target locations. The stimulus was either sound from a loudspeaker or spatial language (e.g., "2 o'clock, 16 ft"). On each trial, an observer attempted to walk to the target location along a direct or indirect path. The ability to mentally keep track of the target location without concurrent perceptual information about it (spatial updating) was assessed in terms of the separation between the stopping points for the 2 paths. Updating performance was very nearly the same for the 2 modalities, indicating that once an internal representation of a location has been determined, subsequent updating performance is nearly independent of the modality used to specify the representation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jack M Loomis
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara 93106-9660, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Awards for Distinguished Scientific Early Career Contributions to Psychology. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2001. [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.11.897] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|