1
|
Duncan DH, van Moorselaar D, Theeuwes J. Visual statistical learning requires attention. Psychon Bull Rev 2024:10.3758/s13423-024-02605-1. [PMID: 39497006 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02605-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/19/2024] [Indexed: 11/06/2024]
Abstract
Statistical learning is a person's ability to automatically learn environmental regularities through passive exposure. Since the earliest studies of statistical learning in infants, it has been debated exactly how "passive" this learning can be (i.e., whether attention is needed for learning to occur). In Experiment 1 of the current study, participants performed a serial feature search task where they searched for a target shape among heterogenous nontarget shapes. Unbeknownst to the participants, one of these nontarget shapes was presented much more often in location. Even though the regularity concerned a nonsalient, nontarget item that did not receive any attentional priority during search, participants still learned its regularity (responding faster when it was presented at this high-probability location). While this may suggest that not much, if any, attention is needed for learning to occur, follow-up experiments showed that if an attentional strategy (i.e., color subset search or exogenous cueing) effectively prevents attention from being directed to this critical regularity, incidental learning is no longer observed. We conclude that some degree of attention to a regularity is needed for visual statistical learning to occur.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dock H Duncan
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
- Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
| | - Dirk van Moorselaar
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Jan Theeuwes
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), Amsterdam, the Netherlands
- William James Center for Research, ISPA-Instituto Universitario, Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Takacs A, Toth‐Faber E, Schubert L, Tarnok Z, Ghorbani F, Trelenberg M, Nemeth D, Münchau A, Beste C. Neural representations of statistical and rule-based predictions in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Hum Brain Mapp 2024; 45:e26719. [PMID: 38826009 PMCID: PMC11144952 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26719] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2023] [Revised: 04/11/2024] [Accepted: 05/06/2024] [Indexed: 06/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Gilles de la Tourette syndrome (GTS) is a disorder characterised by motor and vocal tics, which may represent habitual actions as a result of enhanced learning of associations between stimuli and responses (S-R). In this study, we investigated how adults with GTS and healthy controls (HC) learn two types of regularities in a sequence: statistics (non-adjacent probabilities) and rules (predefined order). Participants completed a visuomotor sequence learning task while EEG was recorded. To understand the neurophysiological underpinnings of these regularities in GTS, multivariate pattern analyses on the temporally decomposed EEG signal as well as sLORETA source localisation method were conducted. We found that people with GTS showed superior statistical learning but comparable rule-based learning compared to HC participants. Adults with GTS had different neural representations for both statistics and rules than HC adults; specifically, adults with GTS maintained the regularity representations longer and had more overlap between them than HCs. Moreover, over different time scales, distinct fronto-parietal structures contribute to statistical learning in the GTS and HC groups. We propose that hyper-learning in GTS is a consequence of the altered sensitivity to encode complex statistics, which might lead to habitual actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam Takacs
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of MedicineTechnische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
- University Neuropsychology Center, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
| | - Eszter Toth‐Faber
- Institute of PsychologyELTE Eötvös Loránd UniversityBudapestHungary
- Brain, Memory and Language Research Group, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, HUN‐REN Research Centre for Natural SciencesBudapestHungary
| | - Lina Schubert
- Institute of Systems Motor ScienceUniversity of LübeckLübeckGermany
| | - Zsanett Tarnok
- Vadaskert Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Hospital and Outpatient ClinicBudapestHungary
| | - Foroogh Ghorbani
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of MedicineTechnische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
- University Neuropsychology Center, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
| | - Madita Trelenberg
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of MedicineTechnische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
| | - Dezso Nemeth
- INSERMUniversité Claude Bernard Lyon 1, CNRS, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon CRNL U1028 UMR5292BronFrance
- NAP Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University and Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, HUN‐REN Research Centre for Natural SciencesBudapestHungary
- Department of Education and Psychology, Faculty of Social SciencesUniversity of Atlántico MedioLas Palmas de Gran CanariaSpain
| | | | - Christian Beste
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of MedicineTechnische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
- University Neuropsychology Center, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität DresdenDresdenGermany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lin W, Qian J. Priming effect of individual similarity and ensemble perception in visual search and working memory. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2024; 88:719-734. [PMID: 38127115 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-023-01902-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2023] [Accepted: 11/22/2023] [Indexed: 12/23/2023]
Abstract
Perceptual priming is a well-known phenomenon showing that the repetition of an object's feature can facilitate subsequent detection of that item. Although the priming effect has been rigorously studied in visual search, less is known about its effect on working memory and it is unclear whether the repetition of similar features, and furthermore, ensemble perception created by a large set of similar features, can induce priming. In this study, we investigated the priming effects of individual similarity and ensemble perception in visual search and visual working memory (VWM). We replicated the classic perceptual priming effect (Experiment 1a) and found that visual search was enhanced when the current target had a similar color to the previous target (Experiment 1b), but not when the similar color had been shown as a distractor before (Experiment 1c). However, if the target and distractors of similar colors formed ensemble perception, the search efficiency was again promoted even when the current target shared the same color with the previous distractor (Experiment 1d). For VWM, repeating the ensembles of the target- and nontarget-color subsets did not significantly affect the memory capacity, while switching the two harmed the memory fidelity but not capacity (Experiment 2). We suggest different underlying mechanisms for priming in visual search and VWM: in the former, the perception history of individual similarity and stimuli ensemble exert their effects on through the priority map, by forming a gradient distribution of attentional weights that peak at the previous target feature and diminish as stimulus diverges from the previously selected one; while in the latter, perception history of memory ensemble may influence the deployment of existing memory resources across trials, thereby affecting the memory fidelity but not its capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wenting Lin
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou, 510006, China
| | - Jiehui Qian
- Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou, 510006, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Chang TY, Cha O, McGugin R, Tomarken A, Gauthier I. How general is ensemble perception? PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2024; 88:695-708. [PMID: 37861726 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-023-01883-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2023] [Accepted: 09/28/2023] [Indexed: 10/21/2023]
Abstract
People can summarize features of groups of objects (e.g., the mean size of apples). Claims of dissociations or common mechanisms supporting such ensemble perception (EP) judgments have generally been made on the basis of correlations between pairs of tasks. These correlations can be inflated because they use the same stimuli, summary statistics and/or task format. Performance on EP tasks also correlates with that on object recognition (OR) tasks. Here, we seek evidence for a general EP ability that is also distinct from OR ability. Two-hundred participants completed three tasks that did not overlap in stimuli, summary statistic or task format. Participants performed a diversity comparison for arrays of nonsense blobs, a mean identity judgment with ensembles of Transformer toys, and the novel object memory task with novel objects (NOMT-Greeble). We hypothesized that EP contributes to the first two of these tasks, while OR contributes only to the last two. Performance on the two tasks suggested to tap an EP ability were correlated after controlling for the third task. Confirmatory factor analysis was used to test our predictions without the confound of measurement error. Correlations between factors assumed to share influence from EP or from OR were higher than that between the factors that we expect did not share these influences. The results provide the first clear evidence for a domain-general EP ability distinct from OR. We argue that understanding such a general ability will require a change in designs and analytical approaches in the study if individual differences in EP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ting-Yun Chang
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 111 21St Avenue South, Nashville, TN, 37240, USA.
| | - Oakyoon Cha
- Department of Psychology, Sungshin Women's University, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Rankin McGugin
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 111 21St Avenue South, Nashville, TN, 37240, USA
| | - Andrew Tomarken
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 111 21St Avenue South, Nashville, TN, 37240, USA
| | - Isabel Gauthier
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 111 21St Avenue South, Nashville, TN, 37240, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Su Y, Wachtler T, Shi Z. Reference induces biases in late visual processing. Sci Rep 2023; 13:18624. [PMID: 37903860 PMCID: PMC10616182 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-44827-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2023] [Accepted: 10/12/2023] [Indexed: 11/01/2023] Open
Abstract
How we perceive a visual stimulus can be influenced by its surrounding context. For example, the presence of a reference skews the perception of a similar feature in a stimulus, a phenomenon called reference repulsion. Ongoing research so far remains inconclusive regarding the stage of visual information processing where such repulsion occurs. We examined the influence of a reference on late visual processing. We measured the repulsion effect caused by an orientation reference presented after an orientation ensemble stimulus. The participants' reported orientations were significantly biased away from the post-stimulus reference, displaying typical characteristics of reference repulsion. Moreover, explicit discrimination choices between the reference and the stimulus influenced the magnitudes of repulsion effects, which can be explained by an encoding-decoding model that differentiates the re-weighting of sensory representations in implicit and explicit processes. These results support the notion that reference repulsion may arise at a late decision-related stage of visual processing, where different sensory decoding strategies are employed depending on the specific task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yannan Su
- Faculty of Biology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany.
- Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany.
| | - Thomas Wachtler
- Faculty of Biology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Munich, Germany
| | - Zhuanghua Shi
- General and Experimental Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Foster DH, Nascimento SM. Little information loss with red-green color deficient vision in natural environments. iScience 2023; 26:107421. [PMID: 37593460 PMCID: PMC10428128 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2023.107421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2022] [Revised: 05/10/2023] [Accepted: 07/13/2023] [Indexed: 08/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Inherited color vision deficiency affects red-green discrimination in about one in twelve men from European populations. Its effects have been studied mainly in primitive foraging but also in detecting blushing and breaking camouflage. Yet there is no obvious relationship between these specific tasks and vision in the real world. The aim here was to quantify the impact of color vision deficiency by estimating computationally the information available to observers about colored surfaces in natural scenes. With representative independent sets of 50 and 100 hyperspectral images, estimated information was found to be only a little less in red-green color vision deficiency than in normal trichromacy. Colorimetric analyses revealed the importance of large lightness variations within scenes, small redness-greenness variations, and uneven frequencies of different colored surfaces. While red-green color vision deficiency poses challenges in some tasks, it has much less effect on gaining information from natural environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David H. Foster
- Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK
| | - Sérgio M.C. Nascimento
- Physics Center of Minho and Porto Universities (CF-UM-UP), University of Minho, 4710-057 Braga, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Rafiei M, Chetverikov A, Hansmann-Roth S, Kristjansson Á. The influence of the tested item on serial dependence in perceptual decisions. Perception 2023; 52:255-265. [PMID: 36919274 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231157582] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/16/2023]
Abstract
Serial dependence in vision reflects how perceptual decisions can be biased by what we have recently perceived. Serial dependence studies test single items' effects on perceptual decisions. However, our visual world contains multiple objects at any given moment, so it's important to understand how past experiences affect not only a single object but also perception in a more general sense. Here we asked the question: What effect does a single item have when there is more than one subsequently presented test item? We displayed a single line (inducer) at the screen center, then either a single test-line or two simultaneous test-lines, varying in orientation space to the inducer. Next, participants reported test-line orientation using a left or right located response circle (to indicate which test-line should be reported). The results demonstrated that the inducer influenced subsequent perceptual judgments of a test-line even when two test-lines were presented. Distant items caused repulsive serial dependence, while close items caused attractive serial dependence. This shows how a single inducer can influence test-line judgments, even when two test-lines are presented, and can produce attractive and repulsive serial dependence biases when the item to report is revealed after it has disappeared.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohsen Rafiei
- 63541Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| | - Andrey Chetverikov
- 6029Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,63541Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| | - Sabrina Hansmann-Roth
- 63541Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| | - Árni Kristjansson
- 63541Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Hansmann-Roth S, Chetverikov A, Kristjánsson Á. Extracting statistical information about shapes in the visual environment. Vision Res 2023; 206:108190. [PMID: 36780808 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2023.108190] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2022] [Revised: 02/01/2023] [Accepted: 02/01/2023] [Indexed: 02/13/2023]
Abstract
It is well known that observers can use so-called summary statistics of visual ensembles to simplify perceptual processing. The assumption has been that instead of representing feature distributions in detail the visual system extracts the mean and variance of visual ensembles. But recent evidence from implicit testing using a method called feature distribution learning showed that far more detail of the distributions is retained than the summary statistic literature indicates. Observers also encode higher-order statistics such as the kurtosis of feature distributions of orientation and color. But this sort of learning has not been shown for more intricate aspects of visual information. Here we tested the learning of distractor ensembles for shape, using the feature distribution learning method. Using a linearized circular shape space, we found that learning of detailed distributions of shape does not occur for this shape space while observers were able to learn the mean and range of the distributions. Previous demonstrations of feature distribution learning involved simpler feature dimensions than the more complex shape space tested here, and our findings may therefore reveal important boundary conditions of feature distribution learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabrina Hansmann-Roth
- Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland.
| | - Andrey Chetverikov
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Iakovlev AU, Utochkin IS. Ensemble averaging: What can we learn from skewed feature distributions? J Vis 2023; 23:5. [PMID: 36602815 PMCID: PMC9832727 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.1.5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2021] [Accepted: 11/23/2022] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Many studies have shown that observers can accurately estimate the average feature of a group of objects. However, the way the visual system relies on the information from each individual item is still under debate. Some models suggest some or all items sampled and averaged arithmetically. Another strategy implies "robust averaging," when middle elements gain greater weight than outliers. One version of a robust averaging model was recently suggested by Teng et al. (2021), who studied motion direction averaging in skewed feature distributions and found systematic biases toward their modes. They interpreted these biases as evidence for robust averaging and suggested a probabilistic weighting model based on minimization of the virtual loss function. In four experiments, we replicated systematic skew-related biases in another feature domain, namely, orientation averaging. Importantly, we show that the magnitude of the bias is not determined by the locations of the mean or mode alone, but is substantially defined by the shape of the whole feature distribution. We test a model that accounts for such distribution-dependent biases and robust averaging in a biologically plausible way. The model is based on well-established mechanisms of spatial pooling and population encoding of local features by neurons with large receptive fields. Both the loss functions model and the population coding model with a winner-take-all decoding rule accurately predicted the observed patterns, suggesting that the pooled population response model can be considered a neural implementation of the computational algorithms of information sampling and robust averaging in ensemble perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Igor S Utochkin
- Institute for Mind and Biology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Unlocking crowding by ensemble statistics. Curr Biol 2022; 32:4975-4981.e3. [PMID: 36309011 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2022] [Revised: 08/16/2022] [Accepted: 10/03/2022] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
In crowding,1,2,3,4,5,6,7 objects that can be easily recognized in isolation appear jumbled when surrounded by other elements.8 Traditionally, crowding is explained by local pooling mechanisms,3,6,9,10,11,12,13,14,15 but many findings have shown that the global configuration of the entire stimulus display, rather than local aspects, determines crowding.8,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28 However, understanding global configurations is challenging because even slight changes can lead from crowding to uncrowding and vice versa.23,25,28,29 Unfortunately, the number of configurations to explore is virtually infinite. Here, we show that one does not need to know the specific configuration of flankers to determine crowding strength but only their ensemble statistics, which allow for the rapid computation of groups within the stimulus display.30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37 To investigate the role of ensemble statistics in (un)crowding, we used a classic vernier offset discrimination task in which the vernier was flanked by multiple squares. We manipulated the orientation statistics of the squares based on the following rationale: a central square with an orientation different from the mean orientation of the other squares stands out from the rest and groups with the vernier, causing strong crowding. If, on the other hand, all squares group together, the vernier is the only element that stands out, and crowding is weak. These effects should depend exclusively on the perceived ensemble statistics, i.e., on the mean orientation of the squares and not on their individual orientations. In two experiments, we confirmed these predictions.
Collapse
|
11
|
Perception of the non-dominant hand as larger after non-judgmental focus on its details. Sci Rep 2022; 12:15670. [PMID: 36123432 PMCID: PMC9485221 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-19919-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2022] [Accepted: 09/06/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
We investigated whether brief non-judgmental focus on the details of one's non-dominant hand might lead to changes in perception of its size, and if such a change would be related to central coherence, body dissatisfaction, or how much participants liked their hand. After two pilot experiments (N = 28 and N = 30 respectively: Appendix 1), a within-subject experiment (N = 82) was conducted. Subjects were mainly university students. They were asked to rate the size of their non-dominant hand and how much they liked it, and the size of an external object (a X-box controller) on a visual-analog scale before and after focusing on their details for 5 min, as well as the size of another object (a calculator) before and after a 5 min long distraction task. After completing the tasks, they were asked to respond to a brief questionnaire on body dissatisfaction. A s significant interaction between time and factors (non-dominant hand, X-box controller and calculator) emerged (F(2, 78) = 6.41, p = .003). Participants rated their hand as larger after focusing on its details compared to baseline, and this change was significantly larger than those reported for the X-box controller. No significant change in how they liked their hand was observed, and contrary to the pilot experiments, the perceived change in size of the hand was not related to body dissatisfaction. The significant change in reporting of the size of the hand after focusing on its details seems to be an interesting finding, worth further replications.
Collapse
|
12
|
Hansmann-Roth S, Þorsteinsdóttir S, Geng JJ, Kristjánsson Á. Temporal integration of feature probability distributions. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2022; 86:2030-2044. [PMID: 34997327 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-021-01621-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2021] [Accepted: 11/13/2021] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Humans are surprisingly good at learning the statistical characteristics of their visual environment. Recent studies have revealed that not only can the visual system learn repeated features of visual search distractors, but also their actual probability distributions. Search times were determined by the frequency of distractor features over consecutive search trials. The search displays applied in these studies involved many exemplars of distractors on each trial and while there is clear evidence that feature distributions can be learned from large distractor sets, it is less clear if distributions are well learned for single targets presented on each trial. Here, we investigated potential learning of probability distributions of single targets during visual search. Over blocks of trials, observers searched for an oddly colored target that was drawn from either a Gaussian or a uniform distribution. Search times for the different target colors were clearly influenced by the probability of that feature within trial blocks. The same search targets, coming from the extremes of the two distributions were found significantly slower during the blocks where the targets were drawn from a Gaussian distribution than from a uniform distribution indicating that observers were sensitive to the target probability determined by the distribution shape. In Experiment 2, we replicated the effect using binned distributions and revealed the limitations of encoding complex target distributions. Our results demonstrate detailed internal representations of target feature distributions and that the visual system integrates probability distributions of target colors over surprisingly long trial sequences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabrina Hansmann-Roth
- Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland.
- Université de Lille, CNRS, UMR 9193-SCALab-Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, 59000, Lille, France.
| | - Sóley Þorsteinsdóttir
- Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| | - Joy J Geng
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Icelandic Vision Lab, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
- School of Psychology, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Collins T. Serial dependence tracks objects and scenes in parallel and independently. J Vis 2022; 22:4. [PMID: 35687353 PMCID: PMC9202337 DOI: 10.1167/jov.22.7.4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The visual world is made up of objects and scenes. Object perception requires both discriminating an individual object from others and binding together different perceptual samples of that object across time. Such binding manifests by serial dependence, the attraction of the current perception of a visual attribute toward values of that attribute seen in the recent past. Scene perception is subserved by global mechanisms such as ensemble perception, the rapid extraction of the average feature value of a group of objects. The current study examined to what extent the perception of single objects in multi-object scenes depended on previous feature values of that object or on the average previous attribute of all objects in the ensemble. Results show that serial dependence occurs independently on two simultaneously present objects, that ensemble perception depends only on previous ensembles, and that serial dependence of an individual object occurs only for the features of that particular object. These results suggest that the temporal integration of successive perceptual samples operates simultaneously at independent levels of visual processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thérèse Collins
- Integrative Neuroscience and Cognition Center, University of Paris and CNRS, Paris, France.,
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Li Q, Chen W. 集群表征在不同刺激及属性间的差异. CHINESE SCIENCE BULLETIN-CHINESE 2022. [DOI: 10.1360/tb-2021-1068] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
15
|
Alt NP, Phillips LT. Person Perception, Meet People Perception: Exploring the Social Vision of Groups. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2021; 17:768-787. [PMID: 34797731 DOI: 10.1177/17456916211017858] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Groups, teams, and collectives-people-are incredibly important to human behavior. People live in families, work in teams, and celebrate and mourn together in groups. Despite the huge variety of human group activity and its fundamental importance to human life, social-psychological research on person perception has overwhelmingly focused on its namesake, the person, rather than expanding to consider people perception. By looking to two unexpected partners, the vision sciences and organization behavior, we find emerging work that presents a path forward, building a foundation for understanding how people perceive other people. And yet this nascent field is missing critical insights that scholars of social vision might offer: specifically, for example, the chance to connect perception to behavior through the mediators of cognition and motivational processes. Here, we review emerging work across the vision and social sciences to extract core principles of people perception: efficiency, capacity, and complexity. We then consider complexity in more detail, focusing on how people perception modifies person-perception processes and enables the perception of group emergent properties as well as group dynamics. Finally, we use these principles to discuss findings and outline areas fruitful for future work. We hope that fellow scholars take up this people-perception call.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas P Alt
- Department of Psychology, California State University, Long Beach
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Rafiei M, Chetverikov A, Hansmann-Roth S, Kristjánsson Á. You see what you look for: Targets and distractors in visual search can cause opposing serial dependencies. J Vis 2021; 21:3. [PMID: 34468704 PMCID: PMC8419872 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.10.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2021] [Accepted: 08/06/2021] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Visual perception is, at any given moment, strongly influenced by its temporal context-what stimuli have recently been perceived and in what surroundings. We have previously shown that to-be-ignored items produce a bias upon subsequent perceptual decisions that acts in parallel with other biases induced by attended items. However, our previous investigations were confined to biases upon the perceived orientation of a visual search target, and it is unclear whether these biases influence perceptual decisions in a more general sense. Here, we test whether the biases from visual search targets and distractors affect the perceived orientation of a neutral test line, one that is neither a target nor a distractor. To do so, we asked participants to search for an oddly oriented line among distractors and report its location for a few trials and next presented a test line irrelevant to the search task. Participants were asked to report the orientation of the test line. Our results indicate that in tasks involving visual search, targets induce a positive bias upon a neutral test line if their orientations are similar, whereas distractors produce an attractive bias for similar test lines and a repulsive bias if the orientations of the test line and the average orientation of the distractors are far apart in feature space. In sum, our results show that both attentional role and proximity in feature space between previous and current stimuli determine the direction of biases in perceptual decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohsen Rafiei
- Icelandic Vision Lab, Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
| | - Andrey Chetverikov
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
| | - Sabrina Hansmann-Roth
- Icelandic Vision Lab, Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
- Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives (SCALab), Université de Lille, Lille, France
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Icelandic Vision Lab, Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland
- School of Psychology, National Research University, Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russian Federation
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Tanrıkulu ÖD, Chetverikov A, Kristjánsson Á. Testing temporal integration of feature probability distributions using role-reversal effects in visual search. Vision Res 2021; 188:211-226. [PMID: 34371249 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2021.07.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2020] [Revised: 04/16/2021] [Accepted: 07/23/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The visual system is sensitive to statistical properties of complex scenes and can encode feature probability distributions in detail. But does the brain use these statistics to build probabilistic models of the ever-changing visual input? To investigate this, we examined how observers temporally integrate two different orientation distributions from sequentially presented visual search trials. If the encoded probabilistic information is used in a Bayesian optimal way, observers should weigh more reliable information more strongly, such as feature distributions with low variance. We therefore manipulated the variance of the two feature distributions. Participants performed sequential odd-one-out visual search for an oddly oriented line among distractors. During successive learning trials, the distractor orientations were sampled from two different Gaussian distributions on alternating trials. Then, observers performed a 'test trial' where the orientations of the target and distractors were switched, allowing us to assess observer's internal representation of distractor distributions based on changes in response times. In three experiments we observed that observer's search times on test trials depended mainly on the very last learning trial, indicating a strong recency effect. Since temporal integration has been previously observed with this method, we conclude that when the input is unreliable, the visual system relies more on the most recent stimulus. This indicates that the visual system prefers to utilize sensory history when the statistical properties of the environment are relatively stable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ömer Dağlar Tanrıkulu
- Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland.
| | - Andrey Chetverikov
- Visual Computation Lab, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Árni Kristjánsson
- Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik, Iceland; National Research University, Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russian Federation
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Geyer T, Seitz W, Zinchenko A, Müller HJ, Conci M. Why Are Acquired Search-Guiding Context Memories Resistant to Updating? Front Psychol 2021; 12:650245. [PMID: 33732200 PMCID: PMC7956950 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.650245] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2021] [Accepted: 02/09/2021] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Looking for goal-relevant objects in our various environments is one of the most ubiquitous tasks the human visual system has to accomplish (Wolfe, 1998). Visual search is guided by a number of separable selective-attention mechanisms that can be categorized as bottom-up driven - guidance by salient physical properties of the current stimuli - or top-down controlled - guidance by observers' "online" knowledge of search-critical object properties (e.g., Liesefeld and Müller, 2019). In addition, observers' expectations based on past experience also play also a significant role in goal-directed visual selection. Because sensory environments are typically stable, it is beneficial for the visual system to extract and learn the environmental regularities that are predictive of (the location of) the target stimulus. This perspective article is concerned with one of these predictive mechanisms: statistical context learning of consistent spatial patterns of target and distractor items in visual search. We review recent studies on context learning and its adaptability to incorporate consistent changes, with the aim to provide new directions to the study of processes involved in the acquisition of search-guiding context memories and their adaptation to consistent contextual changes - from a three-pronged, psychological, computational, and neurobiological perspective.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Geyer
- Department Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
- Munich Center for Neurosciences – Brain & Mind, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
| | - Werner Seitz
- Department Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
- Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
| | - Artyom Zinchenko
- Department Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
| | - Hermann J. Müller
- Department Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
- Munich Center for Neurosciences – Brain & Mind, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
| | - Markus Conci
- Department Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|