1
|
Taore A, Tiang M, Dakin SC. (The limits of) eye-tracking with iPads. J Vis 2024; 24:1. [PMID: 38953861 PMCID: PMC11223623 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2023] [Accepted: 04/22/2024] [Indexed: 07/04/2024] Open
Abstract
Applications for eye-tracking-particularly in the clinic-are limited by a reliance on dedicated hardware. Here we compare eye-tracking implemented on an Apple iPad Pro 11" (third generation)-using the device's infrared head-tracking and front-facing camera-with a Tobii 4c infrared eye-tracker. We estimated gaze location using both systems while 28 observers performed a variety of tasks. For estimating fixation, gaze position estimates from the iPad were less accurate and precise than the Tobii (mean absolute error of 3.2° ± 2.0° compared with 0.75° ± 0.43°), but fixation stability estimates were correlated across devices (r = 0.44, p < 0.05). For tasks eliciting saccades >1.5°, estimated saccade counts (r = 0.4-0.73, all p < 0.05) were moderately correlated across devices. For tasks eliciting saccades >8° we observed moderate correlations in estimated saccade speed and amplitude (r = 0.4-0.53, all p < 0.05). We did, however, note considerable variation in the vertical component of estimated smooth pursuit speed from the iPad and a catastrophic failure of tracking on the iPad in 5% to 20% of observers (depending on the test). Our findings sound a note of caution to researchers seeking to use iPads for eye-tracking and emphasize the need to properly examine their eye-tracking data to remove artifacts and outliers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aryaman Taore
- School of Optometry & Vision Science, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Michelle Tiang
- School of Optometry & Vision Science, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Steven C Dakin
- School of Optometry & Vision Science, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
- UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ríos HA, Lövestam-Adrian M, Plainis S, Tsilimbaris M, Joussen AM, Keegan D, Charles M, Cunha-Vaz J, Midena E. Additional measures of macular function beyond visual acuity. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2024; 262:1723-1736. [PMID: 37938378 PMCID: PMC11106142 DOI: 10.1007/s00417-023-06272-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2022] [Revised: 09/29/2023] [Accepted: 10/09/2023] [Indexed: 11/09/2023] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE Visual function is a complex process in which external visual stimuli are interpreted. Patients with retinal diseases and prolonged follow-up times may experience changes in their visual function that are not detected by the standard visual acuity measure, as they are a result of other alterations in visual function. With the advancement of different methods to evaluate visual function, additional measurements have become available, and further standardization suggests that some methods may be promising for use in clinical trials or routine clinical practice. The objectives of this article are to review these additional measurements and to provide guidance on their application. METHODS The Vision Academy's membership of international retinal disease experts reviewed the literature and developed consensus recommendations for the application of additional measures of visual function in routine clinical practice or clinical trials. RESULTS Measures such as low-luminance visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, retinal fixation and microperimetry, and reading performance are measures which can complement visual acuity measurements to provide an assessment of overall visual function, including impact on patients' quality of life. Measures such as dark adaptation, color vision testing, binocular vision testing, visual recognition testing, and shape discrimination require further optimization and validation before they can be implemented in everyday clinical practice. CONCLUSION Additional measurements of visual function may help identify patients who could benefit from earlier diagnosis, detection of disease progression, and therapeutic intervention. New and additional functional clinical trial endpoints are required to fully understand the early stages of macular disease, its progression, and the response to treatment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hernán Andrés Ríos
- Retina y Vítreo, Fundación Oftalmológica Nacional, Universidad del Rosario, Bogotá, Colombia
| | | | - Sotiris Plainis
- Laboratory of Optics and Vision, University of Crete Medical School, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
| | - Miltiadis Tsilimbaris
- Laboratory of Optics and Vision, University of Crete Medical School, Heraklion, Crete, Greece
| | | | - David Keegan
- Department of Ophthalmology, Mater Misericordiae University Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
| | | | - José Cunha-Vaz
- AIBILI - Association for Innovation and Biomedical Research on Light and Image, Coimbra, Portugal
| | - Edoardo Midena
- Department of Ophthalmology, University of Padova, Padua, Italy.
- IRCCS Fondazione Bietti, Rome, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Flowers CS, Legge GE, Engel SA. Customizing spatial remapping of letters to aid reading in the presence of a simulated central field loss. J Vis 2024; 24:17. [PMID: 38635281 PMCID: PMC11033602 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.4.17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2023] [Accepted: 03/22/2024] [Indexed: 04/19/2024] Open
Abstract
Reading is a primary concern of patients with central field loss (CFL) because it is typically performed with foveal vision. Spatial remapping offers one potential avenue to aid in reading; it entails shifting occluded letters to retinal areas where vision is functional. Here, we introduce a method of creating and testing different remapping strategies-ways to remap text-customized for CFL of different shapes. By simulating CFL in typically-sighted individuals, we tested the customization hypothesis-that the benefits of different remapping strategies will depend on the properties of the CFL. That is, remapping strategies will aid reading differentially in the presence of differently shaped CFL. In Experiment 1, letter recognition in the presence of differently shaped CFL was assessed in and around central vision. Using these letter recognition "maps" different spatial remappings were created and tested in Experiment 2 using a word recognition task. Results showed that the horizontal gap remapping, which did not remap any letters vertically, resulted in the best word recognition. Results were also consistent with the customization hypothesis; the benefits of different remappings on word recognition depended on the different CFL shapes. Although the horizontal gap remapping resulted in very good word recognition, tailoring remapping strategies to the shape of patients' CFL may aid reading with the wide range of sizes and shapes encountered by patients with CFL.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin S Flowers
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Gordon E Legge
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
- Center for Applied and Translational Sensory Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Stephen A Engel
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
- Center for Applied and Translational Sensory Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Yu H, Kwon M. Altered Eye Movements During Reading With Simulated Central and Peripheral Visual Field Defects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2023; 64:21. [PMID: 37843494 PMCID: PMC10584020 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.64.13.21] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2023] [Accepted: 09/22/2023] [Indexed: 10/17/2023] Open
Abstract
Purpose Although foveal vision provides fine spatial information, parafoveal and peripheral vision are also known to be important for efficient reading behaviors. Here we systematically investigate how different types and sizes of visual field defects affect the way visual information is acquired via eye movements during reading. Methods Using gaze-contingent displays, simulated scotomas were induced in 24 adults with normal or corrected-to-normal vision during a reading task. The study design included peripheral and central scotomas of varying sizes (aperture or scotoma size of 2°, 4°, 6°, 8°, and 10°) and no-scotoma conditions. Eye movements (e.g., forward/backward saccades, fixations, microsaccades) were plotted as a function of either the aperture or scotoma size, and their relationships were characterized by the best fitting model. Results When the aperture size of the peripheral scotoma decreased below 6° (11 visible letters), there were significant decreases in saccade amplitude and velocity, as well as substantial increases in fixation duration and the number of fixations. Its dependency on the aperture size is best characterized by an exponential decay or growth function in log-linear coordinates. However, saccade amplitude and velocity, fixation duration, and forward/regressive saccades increased more or less linearly with increasing central scotoma size in log-linear coordinates. Conclusions Our results showed differential impacts of central and peripheral vision loss on reading behaviors while lending further support for the importance of foveal and parafoveal vision in reading. These apparently deviated oculomotor behaviors may in part reflect optimal reading strategies to compensate for the loss of visual information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haojue Yu
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States
| | - MiYoung Kwon
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Ktistakis E, Simos P, Tsilimbaris MK, Plainis S. Efficacy οf Wet Age-related Macular Degeneration Treatment οn Reading: A Pilot Study Using Eye-movement Analysis. Optom Vis Sci 2023; 100:670-678. [PMID: 37966366 DOI: 10.1097/opx.0000000000002064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2023] Open
Abstract
SIGNIFICANCE Functional vision, as evaluated with silent passage reading speed, improves after anti-vascular endothelial growth factor (anti-VEGF) treatment in patients with wet age-related macular antidegeneration (wAMD), reflecting primarily a concomitant reduction in the number of fixations. Implementing eye movement analysis when reading may better characterize the effectiveness of therapeutic approaches in wAMD. PURPOSE This study aimed to evaluate silent reading performance by means of eye fixation analysis before and after anti-VEGF treatment in wAMD patients. METHODS Sixteen wAMD patients who underwent anti-VEGF treatment in one eye and visual acuity (VA) better than 0.5 logMAR served as the AMD group. Twenty adults without ocular pathology served as the control group. Central retinal thickness and near VA were assessed at baseline and 3 to 4 months after their first visit. Reading performance was evaluated using short passages of 0.4-logMAR print size. Eye movements were recorded using EyeLink II video eye tracker. Data analysis included computation of reading speed, fixation duration, number of fixations, and percentage of regressions. Frequency distributions of fixation durations were analyzed with ex-Gaussian fittings. RESULTS In the AMD group, silent reading speed in the treated eye correlated well with central retinal thickness reduction and improved significantly by an average of 15.9 ± 28.5 words per minute (P = .04). This improvement was accompanied by an average reduction of 0.24 ± 0.38 in fixations per word (P = .03). The corresponding improvement in monocular VA was not statistically significant. Other eye fixation parameters did not change significantly after treatment. No statistically significant differences were found in the control group. CONCLUSIONS Visual acuity tests may underestimate the potential therapeutic effects after anti-VEGF treatment in patients with relatively good acuity who are being treated for wAMD. Evaluating silent reading performance and eye fixation parameters may better characterize the effectiveness of therapeutic approaches in wAMD patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emmanouil Ktistakis
- Laboratory of Optics and Vision (LOV), School of Medicine, University of Crete, Heraklion, Greece
| | | | - Miltiadis K Tsilimbaris
- Laboratory of Optics and Vision (LOV), School of Medicine, University of Crete, Heraklion, Greece
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Plainis S, Ktistakis E, Tsilimbaris MK. Presbyopia correction with multifocal contact lenses: Evaluation of silent reading performance using eye movements analysis. Cont Lens Anterior Eye 2023; 46:101853. [PMID: 37164776 DOI: 10.1016/j.clae.2023.101853] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2023] [Revised: 04/27/2023] [Accepted: 05/01/2023] [Indexed: 05/12/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Many activities of daily living rely on reading, thus is not surprising that complaints from presbyopes originate in reading difficulties rather in visual acuity. Here, the effectiveness of presbyopia correction with multifocal contact lenses (CLs) is evaluated using an eye-fixation based method of silent reading performance. ΜETHODS: Visual performance of thirty presbyopic volunteers (age: 50 ± 5 yrs) was assessed monocularly and binocularly following 15 days of wear of monthly disposable CLs (AIR OPTIX™ plus HydraGlyde™, Alcon Laboratories) with: (a) single vision (SV) lenses - uncorrected for near (b) aspheric multifocal (MF) CLs. LogMAR acuity was measured with ETDRS charts. Reading performance was evaluated using standard IReST paragraphs displayed on a screen (0.4 logMAR print size at 40 cm distance). Eye movements were monitored with an infrared eyetracker (Eye-Link II, SR Research Ltd). Data analysis included computation of reading speed, fixation duration, fixations per word and percentage of regressions. RESULTS Average reading speed was 250 ± 68 and 235 ± 70 wpm, binocularly and monocularly, with SV CLs, improving statistically significantly to 280 ± 67 (p = 0.002) and 260 ± 59 wpm (p = 0.01), respectively, with MF CLs. Moreover, fixation duration, fixations per word and ex-Gaussian parameter of fixation duration, μ, showed a statistically significant improvement when reading with MF CLs, with fixation duration exhibiting the stronger correlation (r = 0.79, p < 0.001) with improvement in reading speed. The correlation between improvement in VA and reading speed was moderate (r = 0.46, p = 0.016), as was the correlation between VA and any eye fixation parameter. CONCLUSION Average silent reading speed in a presbyopic population was found improved with MF compared to SV CL correction and was faster with binocular compared to monocular viewing: this was mainly due to the faster average fixation duration and the lower number of fixations. Evaluating reading performance using eye fixation analysis could offer a reliable outcome of functional vision in presbyopia correction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sotiris Plainis
- Laboratory of Optics and Vision (LOV), School of Medicine, University of Crete, Greece; Optometry & Vision Science Research Group, Aston University School of Life and Health Sciences, Birmingham, UK.
| | - Emmanouil Ktistakis
- Laboratory of Optics and Vision (LOV), School of Medicine, University of Crete, Greece
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Nuthmann A, Thibaut M, Tran THC, Boucart M. Impact of neovascular age-related macular degeneration on eye-movement control during scene viewing: Viewing biases and guidance by visual salience. Vision Res 2022; 201:108105. [PMID: 36081228 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2022] [Revised: 06/06/2022] [Accepted: 07/19/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Human vision requires us to analyze the visual periphery to decide where to fixate next. In the present study, we investigated this process in people with age-related macular degeneration (AMD). In particular, we examined viewing biases and the extent to which visual salience guides fixation selection during free-viewing of naturalistic scenes. We used an approach combining generalized linear mixed modeling (GLMM) with a-priori scene parcellation. This method allows one to investigate group differences in terms of scene coverage and observers' well-known tendency to look at the center of scene images. Moreover, it allows for testing whether image salience influences fixation probability above and beyond what can be accounted for by the central bias. Compared with age-matched normally sighted control subjects (and young subjects), AMD patients' viewing behavior was less exploratory, with a stronger central fixation bias. All three subject groups showed a salience effect on fixation selection-higher-salience scene patches were more likely to be fixated. Importantly, the salience effect for the AMD group was of similar size as the salience effect for the control group, suggesting that guidance by visual salience was still intact. The variances for by-subject random effects in the GLMM indicated substantial individual differences. A separate model exclusively considered the AMD data and included fixation stability as a covariate, with the results suggesting that reduced fixation stability was associated with a reduced impact of visual salience on fixation selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antje Nuthmann
- Institute of Psychology, University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany.
| | - Miguel Thibaut
- University of Lille, Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, INSERM, Lille, France
| | - Thi Ha Chau Tran
- University of Lille, Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, INSERM, Lille, France; Ophthalmology Department, Lille Catholic Hospital, Catholic University of Lille, Lille, France
| | - Muriel Boucart
- University of Lille, Lille Neuroscience & Cognition, INSERM, Lille, France.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Vice JE, Biles MK, Maniglia M, Visscher KM. Oculomotor changes following learned use of an eccentric retinal locus. Vision Res 2022; 201:108126. [PMID: 36162313 PMCID: PMC9840844 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2022.108126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2022] [Revised: 07/27/2022] [Accepted: 09/01/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
People with bilateral central vision loss sometimes develop a new point of oculomotor reference called a preferred retinal locus (PRL) that is used for fixating and planning saccadic eye movements. How individuals develop and learn to effectively use a PRL is still debated; in particular, the time course of learning to plan saccades using a PRL and learning to stabilize peripheral fixation at the desired location. Here we address knowledge limitations through research describing how eye movements change as a person learns to adopt an eccentric retinal locus. Using a gaze-contingent, eye tracking-guided paradigm to simulate central vision loss, 40 participants developed a PRL by engaging in an oculomotor and visual recognition task. After 12 training sessions, significant improvements were observed in six eye movement metrics addressing different aspects involved in learning to use a PRL: first saccade landing dispersion, saccadic re-referencing, saccadic precision, saccadic latency, percentage of useful trials, and fixation stability. Importantly, our analyses allowed separate examination of the stability of target fixation separately from the dispersion and precision of the landing location of saccades. These measures explained 50% of the across-subject variance in accuracy. Fixation stability and saccadic precision showed a strong, positive correlation. Although there was no statistically significant difference in rate of learning, individuals did tend to learn saccadic precision faster than fixation stability. Saccadic precision was also more associated with accuracy than fixation stability for the behavioral task. This suggests effective intervention strategies in low vision should address both fixation stability and saccadic precision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason E Vice
- Vision Science Graduate Program, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States
| | - Mandy K Biles
- Department of Psychology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States
| | - Marcello Maniglia
- Department of Psychology, University of California at Riverside, United States
| | - Kristina M Visscher
- Department of Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Yu H, Shamsi F, Kwon M. Altered eye movements during reading under degraded viewing conditions: Background luminance, text blur, and text contrast. J Vis 2022; 22:4. [PMID: 36069942 PMCID: PMC9465940 DOI: 10.1167/jov.22.10.4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2022] [Accepted: 08/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Degraded viewing conditions caused by either natural environments or visual disorders lead to slow reading. Here, we systematically investigated how eye movement patterns during reading are affected by degraded viewing conditions in terms of spatial resolution, contrast, and background luminance. Using a high-speed eye tracker, binocular eye movements were obtained from 14 young normally sighted adults. Images of text passages were manipulated with varying degrees of background luminance (1.3-265 cd/m2), text blur (severe blur to no blur), or text contrast (2.6%-100%). We analyzed changes in key eye movement features, such as saccades, microsaccades, regressive saccades, fixations, and return-sweeps across different viewing conditions. No significant changes were observed for the range of tested background luminance values. However, with increasing text blur and decreasing text contrast, we observed a significant decrease in saccade amplitude and velocity, as well as a significant increase in fixation duration, number of fixations, proportion of regressive saccades, microsaccade rate, and duration of return-sweeps. Among all, saccade amplitude, fixation duration, and proportion of regressive saccades turned out to be the most significant contributors to reading speed, together accounting for 90% of variance in reading speed. Our results together showed that, when presented with degraded viewing conditions, the patterns of eye movements during reading were altered accordingly. These findings may suggest that the seemingly deviated eye movements observed in individuals with visual impairments may be in part resulting from active and optimal information acquisition strategies operated when visual sensory input becomes substantially deprived.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haojue Yu
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Foroogh Shamsi
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - MiYoung Kwon
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Morrice E, Murphy C, Soldano V, Addona C, Wittich W, Johnson AP. Assessing optimal colour and illumination to facilitate reading: an analysis of print size. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2021; 41:1209-1221. [PMID: 34549808 DOI: 10.1111/opo.12885] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2021] [Revised: 08/02/2021] [Accepted: 08/03/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study examined how optimal colour/illumination conditions and the efficacy of the iPad, LuxIQ and Smart Bulb varied as a function of print size in younger, older and visually impaired adults. METHODS Participants with visual impairments and simulated low vision (SLV) read the MNRead using the iPad, LuxIQ and Smart Bulb. RESULTS In the impairment condition at 1.20 logMAR, the iPad (M = 9.49, 95% CI [3.18, 19.42]) and LuxIQ (M = 15.95, 95% CI [9.54, 24.86]) improved the reading speeds. At 0.80 logMAR (SLV), all devices improved reading speeds of older adults (iPad (M = 28.70, 95% CI [14.65, 42.51]); LuxIQ (M = 49.63, 95% CI [30.04, 69.68]); Smart Bulb (M = 23.11, 95% CI [3.33, 42.11])), but in younger adults only the LuxIQ (M = 13.04, 95% CI [3.21, 21.27]) did so. In the impairment condition, the iPad (M = 5.54, 95% CI [0.31, 12.13]) and LuxIQ (M = 13.90, 95% CI [7.88, 23.49]) improved reading speeds. In the SLV condition, age was a significant predictor of reading speed at 1.20 logMAR (F3,164 = 10.74, p < 0.001, Adj. R2 = 0.16). At 0.80 logMAR, age and luminance, but not colour, were significant predictors (F3,164 = 52.52, p < 0.001, Adj. R2 = 0.49). In the impairment condition, both age and lux were significant predictors of reading speed at 1.20 (F3,85 = 7.14, p < 0.001, Adj. R2 = 0.20) and 0.80 logMAR (F3,85 = 7.97, p < 0.001, Adj. R2 = 0.22), but colour was not. CONCLUSIONS Light source effectiveness and optimal colour/illumination vary as a function of print size. It appears that print size is the most important factor for improving reading speed. As print size decreases, luminance becomes crucial, and only at the smallest print sizes does the effect of colour become useful.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elliott Morrice
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,Lethbridge-Layton-Mackay Rehabilitation Centre, Integrated Health and Social Services University Network for West-Central Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Caitlin Murphy
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,Lethbridge-Layton-Mackay Rehabilitation Centre, Integrated Health and Social Services University Network for West-Central Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Vanessa Soldano
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Cynthia Addona
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Walter Wittich
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,Lethbridge-Layton-Mackay Rehabilitation Centre, Integrated Health and Social Services University Network for West-Central Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation of Greater Montreal/The Nazareth and Louis Braille Institute, Integrated Health and Social Services Centre Montérégie-Centre, Longueuil, Quebec, Canada
| | - Aaron P Johnson
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.,Lethbridge-Layton-Mackay Rehabilitation Centre, Integrated Health and Social Services University Network for West-Central Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Abstract
In healthy vision, the fovea provides high acuity and serves as the locus for fixation achieved through saccadic eye movements. Bilateral loss of the foveal regions in both eyes causes individuals to adopt an eccentric locus for fixation. This review deals with the eye movement consequences of the loss of the foveal oculomotor reference and the ability of individuals to use an eccentric fixation locus as the new oculomotor reference. Eye movements are an integral part of everyday activities, such as reading, searching for an item of interest, eye-hand coordination, navigation, or tracking an approaching car. We consider how these tasks are impacted by the need to use an eccentric locus for fixation and as a reference for eye movements, specifically saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Vision Science, Volume 7 is September 2021. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Preeti Verghese
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California 94115, USA;
| | - Cécile Vullings
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California 94115, USA;
| | - Natela Shanidze
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California 94115, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Randomized Controlled Trial of a Spectacle Lens for Macular Degeneration. Optom Vis Sci 2020; 97:889-897. [PMID: 33055515 DOI: 10.1097/opx.0000000000001588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022] Open
Abstract
SIGNIFICANCE E-Scoop, a spectacle lens, provides no clinically relevant improvements on quality of life, visual acuity, and contrast sensitivity for patients with AMD. Because patients' burden is high and therapeutic options are scarce, the incentive to develop effective vision rehabilitation interventions remains. PURPOSE Patients with AMD experience low quality of life due to vision loss, despite angiogenesis inhibitor interventions that slow down progression for some patients. E-Scoop, which includes low-power prisms, 6% magnification, yellow tint, and antireflection coating, might aid in daily activities by improving distance viewing. Separately, these features have little proven effectiveness. E-Scoop has not been formally tested. This study aimed to determine the impact of E-Scoop on quality of life and the effect on visual acuity and contrast sensitivity. METHODS In this randomized controlled, open-label trial, 190 of 226 eligible patients were included. The primary outcome was quality of life measured with the 25-item National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire. Secondary outcomes were visual acuity and contrast sensitivity. The follow-up for quality of life was after 6 weeks for controls and after 3 weeks of use for E-Scoop wearers. The visual measures were repeated after 6 weeks, with optimal refractive correction, with and without E-Scoop. RESULTS Randomization resulted in 99 E-Scoop and 86 control group patients for intention-to-treat analysis. No differential change was found between the E-Scoop and control groups on the 25-item National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire using Rasch analysis (Cohen d = -0.07, P = .53). Statistically significant but small effects were found in favor of E-Scoop on binocular visual acuity (mean difference, 0.05 logMAR [2.5 letters, P < .001]) and contrast sensitivity (mean difference, 0.10 logCS [2 letters, P < .001]). CONCLUSIONS No effect of E-Scoop on quality of life was found. E-Scoop showed effects that were statistically significant, although not clinically meaningful and within typical variability, on visual measures.
Collapse
|
13
|
Arango T, Yu D, Lu ZL, Bex PJ. Effects of Task on Reading Performance Estimates. Front Psychol 2020; 11:2005. [PMID: 32903762 PMCID: PMC7438847 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2020] [Accepted: 07/20/2020] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Reading is a primary problem for low vision patients and a common functional endpoint for eye disease. However, there is limited agreement on reading assessment methods for clinical outcomes. Many clinical reading tests lack standardized materials for repeated testing and cannot be self-administered, which limit their use for vision rehabilitation monitoring and remote assessment. We compared three different reading assessment methods to address these limitations. Normally sighted participants (N = 12) completed MNREAD, and two forced-choice reading tests at multiple font sizes in counterbalanced order. In a word identification task, participants indicated whether 5-letter pentagrams, syntactically matched to English, were words or non-words. In a true/false reading task, participants indicated whether four-word sentences presented in RSVP were logically true or false. The reading speed vs. print size data from each experiment were fit by an exponential function with parameters for reading acuity, critical print size and maximum reading speed. In all cases, reading speed increased quickly as an exponential function of text size. Reading speed and critical print size significantly differed across tasks, but not reading acuity. Reading speeds were faster for word/non-word and true/false reading tasks, consistent with the elimination of eye movement load in RSVP but required larger text sizes to achieve those faster reading speeds. These different reading tasks quantify distinct aspects of reading behavior and the preferred assessment method may depend on the goal of intervention. Reading performance is an important clinical endpoint and a key quality of life indicator, however, differences across methods complicate direct comparisons across studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tiffany Arango
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Deyue Yu
- College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
| | - Zhong-Lin Lu
- Division of Arts and Sciences, NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China.,Center for Neural Science, Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, United States.,NYU-ECNU Institute of Brain and Cognitive Science at NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China
| | - Peter J Bex
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Agaoglu MN, Chung STL. Exploration of the functional consequences of fixational eye movements in the absence of a fovea. J Vis 2020; 20:12. [PMID: 32106298 PMCID: PMC7343529 DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.2.12] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
A recent theory posits that ocular drifts of fixational eye movements serve to reformat the visual input of natural images, so that the power of the input image is equalized across a range of spatial frequencies. This “spectral whitening” effect is postulated to improve the processing of high-spatial-frequency information and requires normal fixational eye movements. Given that people with macular disease exhibit abnormal fixational eye movements, do they also exhibit spectral whitening? To answer this question, we computed the power spectral density of movies of natural images translated in space and time according to the fixational eye movements (thus simulating the retinal input) of a group of observers with long-standing bilateral macular disease. Just as for people with normal vision, the power of the retinal input at low spatial frequencies was lower than that based on the 1/f2 relationship, demonstrating spectral whitening. However, the amount of whitening was much less for observers with macular disease when compared with age-matched controls with normal vision. A mediation analysis showed that the eccentricity of the preferred retinal locus adopted by these observers and the characteristics of ocular drifts are important factors limiting the amount of whitening. Finally, we did not find a normal aging effect on spectral whitening. Although these findings alone cannot form a causal link between macular disease and spectral properties of eye movements, they suggest novel potential means of modifying the characteristics of fixational eye movements, which may in turn improve functional vision for people with macular disease.
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
SIGNIFICANCE The International Reading Speed Texts (IReST) is a valid measure of reading speed in a Canadian sample. However, if clinicians desire to assess reading comprehension using the IReST, this will significantly reduce reading speeds of individuals with normal vision or reduced visual acuity and therefore should use the values presented here. PURPOSE The purposes of this study are (1) to validate the IReST in an English-speaking Canadian sample and (2) to examine how reading comprehension questions and reduced visual acuity affect reading speed on the IReST. METHODS For study 1, Canadian English speakers (n = 25) read all 10 IReST following the procedures used in the original IReST validation. For study 2, Canadian English speakers (n = 50) read all 10 IReST, half with normal/corrected-to-normal vision and half with reduced visual acuity, and were asked reading comprehension questions. RESULTS No significant differences were found between Canadian sample and the published IReST values (in all cases, P > .05; mean difference [Mdiff] = -5.30 to +11.43; Cohen d = -0.15 to +0.27; Bayes factors = 0.41, 0.09). Assessing reading comprehension with multiple-choice questions on the IReST significantly reduced reading speeds in the normal vision condition (Mdiff = 25.3; 95% confidence interval, -16.7 to -34.1) and in the simulated impairment condition (Mdiff = 59.3; 95% confidence interval, -47.7 to -71). CONCLUSIONS The IReST is a valid measure that can be used to assess reading speed in a Canadian English-speaking sample. If researchers/clinicians wish to assess both reading speed and comprehension, using multiple-choice reading comprehension questions, then the values provided by the IReST will likely underestimate an individual's true reading speed in individuals with normal/corrected-to-normal vision or reduced visual acuity.
Collapse
|
16
|
Costela FM, Reeves SM, Woods RL. Orientation of the preferred retinal locus (PRL) is maintained following changes in simulated scotoma size. J Vis 2020; 20:25. [PMID: 33555170 PMCID: PMC7424101 DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.7.25] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Although macular lesions often enlarge, we know little about what happens when the preferred retinal locus (PRL) is enveloped by the lesion. We present a prospective study of subjects with normal vision who were trained to develop a PRL using simulated scotomas with a gaze-contingent visual display. We hypothesized that, when subjects had developed a robust PRL and the scotoma size was increased, the PRL would move to remain outside the scotoma and in a direction that maintained the orientation (theta) of the PRL relative to the fovea. Nine subjects with normal vision were trained to develop a PRL and were then exposed to scotoma sizes that ranged from 4° to 24° in diameter. Subjects tracked a stimulus using saccades or smooth pursuits. Fixation stability was measured by calculating the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA). To measure the reassignment of the oculomotor reference (OMR) to the PRL, we analyzed the spread (BCEA) of saccade first landing points. All subjects developed a robust PRL that did not vary more than 0.8° on average between blocks of trials of a scotoma size, and they maintained the orientation of the PRL as the simulated scotoma size varied (±9° median standard deviation in theta, defined as orientation angle). Fixation stability and OMR to the PRL worsened (larger BCEA) with increasing scotoma size. This, and related studies, could guide development of a PRL training method to help people with central vision loss.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francisco M Costela
- Schepens Eye Research Institute, Massachusetts Eye and Ear , Boston, MA , USA.,Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School , Boston, MA , USA.,
| | - Stephanie M Reeves
- Schepens Eye Research Institute, Massachusetts Eye and Ear , Boston, MA , USA.,
| | - Russell L Woods
- Schepens Eye Research Institute, Massachusetts Eye and Ear , Boston, MA , USA.,Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School , Boston, MA , USA.,
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
|
18
|
Abstract
How people look at visual information reveals fundamental information about them; their interests and their states of mind. Previous studies showed that scanpath, i.e., the sequence of eye movements made by an observer exploring a visual stimulus, can be used to infer observer-related (e.g., task at hand) and stimuli-related (e.g., image semantic category) information. However, eye movements are complex signals and many of these studies rely on limited gaze descriptors and bespoke datasets. Here, we provide a turnkey method for scanpath modeling and classification. This method relies on variational hidden Markov models (HMMs) and discriminant analysis (DA). HMMs encapsulate the dynamic and individualistic dimensions of gaze behavior, allowing DA to capture systematic patterns diagnostic of a given class of observers and/or stimuli. We test our approach on two very different datasets. Firstly, we use fixations recorded while viewing 800 static natural scene images, and infer an observer-related characteristic: the task at hand. We achieve an average of 55.9% correct classification rate (chance = 33%). We show that correct classification rates positively correlate with the number of salient regions present in the stimuli. Secondly, we use eye positions recorded while viewing 15 conversational videos, and infer a stimulus-related characteristic: the presence or absence of original soundtrack. We achieve an average 81.2% correct classification rate (chance = 50%). HMMs allow to integrate bottom-up, top-down, and oculomotor influences into a single model of gaze behavior. This synergistic approach between behavior and machine learning will open new avenues for simple quantification of gazing behavior. We release SMAC with HMM, a Matlab toolbox freely available to the community under an open-source license agreement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Janet H Hsiao
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pok Fu Lam, Hong Kong
| | - Antoni B Chan
- Department of Computer Science, City University of Hong Kong, Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Reading Speed and Reading Comprehension in Age-related Macular Degeneration. Am J Ophthalmol 2018; 186:138-143. [PMID: 29246579 DOI: 10.1016/j.ajo.2017.11.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2017] [Revised: 11/28/2017] [Accepted: 11/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE To evaluate the impact of age-related macular degeneration (AMD) on short out-loud and sustained silent reading speeds, and reading comprehension. DESIGN Prospective, cross-sectional. METHODS Setting: Wilmer Eye Institute. POPULATION Literate, native-English speakers with and without AMD. AMD participants had better-eye visual acuity (VA) <20/32 and >20/100, while controls had binocular VA >20/32. PROCEDURES MNRead was used to assess short-duration out-loud reading speed. Sustained silent reading test was used to evaluate sustained silent reading speeds, while reading comprehension was assessed based on silent reading test text. OUTCOME MEASURES MNRead maximum reading speed, sustained-silent reading speed, and comprehension score. RESULTS Analyses included 24 AMD patients and 22 controls. In age-adjusted regressions, AMD participants, compared to controls, read 46 words per minute (wpm) slower on MNRead (95% confidence interval [CI]: -66, -26, P < .001), but there was no difference in sustained reading speeds between groups (β = 0.99, 95% CI: -41.8, 43.8, P = .96). In other models, there was a decrement of 12.6 wpm on MNRead per 0.1 worsening logMAR (95% CI: -18.7, -6.6, P < .001), but VA was not associated with a decrement in sustained reading speed (β = -10.1, 95% CI: -22.4, 2.1, P = .10). However, AMD participants had substantially lower comprehension scores than controls (53% vs 85% correct, P < .001), and each 1-line VA decrement was associated with 5.9% lower comprehension score (95% CI: -9.1, -2.7, P = .001). CONCLUSIONS AMD patients read slower than controls when forced to read out loud. When asked to read silently over a longer duration, both groups read at similar speeds, though AMD patients demonstrated substantially lower comprehension scores, suggesting that they chose to sacrifice comprehension for speed.
Collapse
|
20
|
Wallis S, Yang Y, Anderson SJ. Word Mode: a crowding-free reading protocol for individuals with macular disease. Sci Rep 2018; 8:1241. [PMID: 29352163 PMCID: PMC5775436 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-19859-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2017] [Accepted: 01/08/2018] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Central retinal loss through macular disease markedly reduces the ability to read largely because identification of a word using peripheral vision is negatively influenced by nearby text, a phenomenon termed visual crowding. Here, we present a novel peripheral reading protocol, termed Word Mode, that eliminates crowding by presenting each word in isolation but in a position that mimics its natural position in the line of text being read, with each new word elicited using a self-paced button press. We used a gaze-contingent paradigm to simulate a central scotoma in four normally-sighted observers, and measured oral reading speed for text positioned 7.5° in the inferior field. Compared with reading whole sentences, our crowding-free protocol increased peripheral reading speeds by up to a factor of seven, resulted in significantly fewer reading errors and fixations per sentence, and reduced both the critical print size and the text size required for spot reading by 0.2-0.3 logMAR. We conclude that the level of reading efficiency afforded by the crowding-free reading protocol Word Mode may return reading as a viable activity to many individuals with macular disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stuart Wallis
- School of Life & Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, B4 7ET, UK.
| | - Yit Yang
- Royal Wolverhampton NHS Trust, New Cross Hospital, Wolverhampton, WV10 0QP, UK
| | - Stephen J Anderson
- School of Life & Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, B4 7ET, UK
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
Low vision is any type of visual impairment that affects activities of daily living. In the context of low vision, we define plasticity as changes in brain or perceptual behavior that follow the onset of visual impairment and that are not directly due to the underlying pathology. An important goal of low-vision research is to determine how plasticity affects visual performance of everyday activities. In this review, we consider the levels of the visual system at which plasticity occurs, the impact of age and visual experience on plasticity, and whether plastic changes are spontaneous or require explicit training. We also discuss how plasticity may affect low-vision rehabilitation. Developments in retinal imaging, noninvasive brain imaging, and eye tracking have supplemented traditional clinical and psychophysical methods for assessing how the visual system adapts to visual impairment. Findings from contemporary research are providing tools to guide people with low vision in adopting appropriate rehabilitation strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gordon E Legge
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455;
| | - Susana T L Chung
- School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720;
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
PURPOSE Crowding, the increased difficulty in recognizing a target due to the proximity of adjacent objects, is identified as the main sensory constraint for the size of the visual span (the number of letters recognized without moving the eyes) and reading speed in peripheral vision. The goal of the present study is to assess the impact of temporal modulation on crowding, visual span, and reading in the periphery. METHODS Six normally sighted young adults participated in the study. Four temporal modulation patterns were examined: (1) moving scotoma (sequentially masking the component letters in a letter string or word), (2) moving window (sequentially presenting the component letters), (3) flashing (repeatedly masking and presenting all letters simultaneously), and (4) static (the control condition; no temporal changes during the presentation). For each condition, we obtained the spatial extent of crowding, the size of the visual span, and reading speeds measured by the rapid serial visual presentation method. RESULTS Compared with the static condition, the spatial extent of crowding was reduced in the moving window condition. Both the moving window and moving scotoma conditions led to a faster reading speed for print sizes smaller than critical print size (the smallest print size that allows maximum reading speed). However, none of the temporal modulations increased the size of the visual span and reading speed for print sizes larger than critical print size. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest that the temporal modulation patterns are of limited benefit for peripheral reading despite the substantial improvement for slow reading when print size is close to acuity threshold.
Collapse
|
23
|
Visual exploration of objects and scenes in patients with age-related macular degeneration. J Fr Ophtalmol 2016; 39:82-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jfo.2015.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2015] [Revised: 08/22/2015] [Accepted: 08/24/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
24
|
Bernard JB, Calabrèse A, Castet E. Role of syllable segmentation processes in peripheral word recognition. Vision Res 2014; 105:226-32. [PMID: 25449165 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.10.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2014] [Revised: 09/30/2014] [Accepted: 10/03/2014] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies of foveal visual word recognition provide evidence for a low-level syllable decomposition mechanism occurring during the recognition of a word. We investigated if such a decomposition mechanism also exists in peripheral word recognition. Single words were visually presented to subjects in the peripheral field using a 6° square gaze-contingent simulated central scotoma. In the first experiment, words were either unicolor or had their adjacent syllables segmented with two different colors (color/syllable congruent condition). Reaction times for correct word identification were measured for the two different conditions and for two different print sizes. Results show a significant decrease in reaction time for the color/syllable congruent condition compared with the unicolor condition. A second experiment suggests that this effect is specific to syllable decomposition and results from strategic, presumably involving attentional factors, rather than stimulus-driven control.
Collapse
|
25
|
Plank T, Rosengarth K, Schmalhofer C, Goldhacker M, Brandl-Rühle S, Greenlee MW. Perceptual learning in patients with macular degeneration. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1189. [PMID: 25368597 PMCID: PMC4201094 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2014] [Accepted: 10/01/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Patients with age-related macular degeneration (AMD) or hereditary macular dystrophies (JMD) rely on an efficient use of their peripheral visual field. We trained eight AMD and five JMD patients to perform a texture-discrimination task (TDT) at their preferred retinal locus (PRL) used for fixation. Six training sessions of approximately one hour duration were conducted over a period of approximately 3 weeks. Before, during and after training twelve patients and twelve age-matched controls (the data from two controls had to be discarded later) took part in three functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) sessions to assess training-related changes in the BOLD response in early visual cortex. Patients benefited from the training measurements as indexed by significant decrease (p = 0.001) in the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the presentation of the texture target on background and the visual mask, and in a significant location specific effect of the PRL with respect to hit rate (p = 0.014). The following trends were observed: (i) improvement in Vernier acuity for an eccentric line-bisection task; (ii) positive correlation between the development of BOLD signals in early visual cortex and initial fixation stability (r = 0.531); (iii) positive correlation between the increase in task performance and initial fixation stability (r = 0.730). The first two trends were non-significant, whereas the third trend was significant at p = 0.014, Bonferroni corrected. Consequently, our exploratory study suggests that training on the TDT can enhance eccentric vision in patients with central vision loss. This enhancement is accompanied by a modest alteration in the BOLD response in early visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tina Plank
- Institute for Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| | - Katharina Rosengarth
- Institute for Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| | - Carolin Schmalhofer
- Institute for Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| | - Markus Goldhacker
- Institute for Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| | - Sabine Brandl-Rühle
- Department of Ophthalmology, University Medical Center Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| | - Mark W Greenlee
- Institute for Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg Regensburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Using eye tracking to assess reading performance in patients with glaucoma: a within-person study. J Ophthalmol 2014; 2014:120528. [PMID: 24883203 PMCID: PMC4026991 DOI: 10.1155/2014/120528] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2013] [Accepted: 03/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Reading is often cited as a demanding task for patients with glaucomatous visual field (VF) loss, yet reading speed varies widely between patients and does not appear to be predicted by standard visual function measures. This within-person study aimed to investigate reading duration and eye movements when reading short passages of text in a patient's worse eye (most VF damage) when compared to their better eye (least VF damage). Reading duration and saccade rate were significantly different on average in the worse eye when compared to the better eye (P < 0.001) in 14 patients with glaucoma that had median (interquartile range) between-eye difference in mean deviation (MD; a standard clinical measure for VF loss) of 9.8 (8.3 to 14.8) dB; differences were not related to the size of the difference in MD between eyes. Patients with a more pronounced effect of longer reading duration on their worse eye made a larger proportion of "regressions" (backward saccades) and "unknown" EMs (not adhering to expected reading patterns) when reading with the worse eye when compared to the better eye. A between-eye study in patients with asymmetric disease, coupled with eye tracking, provides a useful experimental design for exploring reading performance in glaucoma.
Collapse
|
27
|
Rubin GS. Measuring reading performance. Vision Res 2013; 90:43-51. [PMID: 23506967 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.02.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2012] [Revised: 02/18/2013] [Accepted: 02/21/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Despite significant changes in the treatment of common eye conditions like cataract and age-related macular degeneration, reading difficulty remains the most common complaint of patients referred for low vision services. Clinical reading tests have been widely used since Jaeger introduced his test types in 1854. A brief review of the major developments in clinical reading tests is provided, followed by a discussion of some of the main controversies in clinical reading assessment. Data for the Salisbury Eye Evaluation (SEE) study demonstrate that standardised clinical reading tests are highly predictive of reading performance under natural, real world conditions, and that discrepancies between self-reported reading ability and measured reading performance may be indicative of people who are at a pre-clinical stage of disability, but are at risk for progression to clinical disability. If measured reading performance is to continue to increase in importance as a clinical outcome measure, there must be agreement on what should be measured (e.g. speed or comprehension) and how it should be measured (e.g. reading silently or aloud). Perhaps most important, the methods for assessing reading performance and the algorithms for scoring reading tests need to be optimised so that the reliability and responsiveness of reading tests can be improved.
Collapse
|
28
|
Amore FM, Fasciani R, Silvestri V, Crossland MD, de Waure C, Cruciani F, Reibaldi A. Relationship between fixation stability measured with MP-1 and reading performance. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2013; 33:611-7. [DOI: 10.1111/opo.12048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2012] [Accepted: 12/27/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Filippo M Amore
- National Centre of Services and Research for the Prevention of Blindness and Rehabilitation of Low Vision Patients; Rome; Italy
| | - Romina Fasciani
- Ophthalmology Department; Catholic University of Rome; Rome; Italy
| | - Valeria Silvestri
- National Centre of Services and Research for the Prevention of Blindness and Rehabilitation of Low Vision Patients; Rome; Italy
| | - Michael D Crossland
- UCL Institute of Ophthalmology and Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust; London; UK
| | - Chiara de Waure
- Institute of Hygiene; Catholic University of Rome; Rome; Italy
| | - Filippo Cruciani
- National Centre of Services and Research for the Prevention of Blindness and Rehabilitation of Low Vision Patients; Rome; Italy
| | - Alfredo Reibaldi
- National Centre of Services and Research for the Prevention of Blindness and Rehabilitation of Low Vision Patients; Rome; Italy
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Lesmes LA, Jackson ML, Bex P. Visual Function Endpoints to Enable Dry AMD Clinical Trials. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013; 10:e43-e50. [PMID: 32863843 DOI: 10.1016/j.ddstr.2012.11.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The slow progression of non-exudative age-related macular degeneration (dry AMD) presents challenges for drug discovery. The standard endpoint used for ophthalmic clinical trials, best-corrected visual acuity, is insensitive to the early stages and slow progression of dry AMD. Effective drug discovery for dry AMD treatments will therefore require novel applications of more effective visual function endpoints. This review will present candidates for visual function endpoints for dry AMD clinical trials. The promising visual assessments include contrast sensitivity, reading speed, microperimetry, and dark adaptation. Their adoption as exploratory endpoints in future trials will be critical for determining their accuracy, precision, and applicability, and ultimately determine their value for drug discovery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luis Andres Lesmes
- Schepens Eye Research Institute, 20 Staniford St, Boston, MA, United States
- Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, United States
| | - Mary Lou Jackson
- Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, 243 Charles St, Boston, MA, United States
- Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, United States
| | - Peter Bex
- Schepens Eye Research Institute, 20 Staniford St, Boston, MA, United States
- Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, 243 Charles St, Boston, MA, United States
- Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, United States
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Smith ND, Glen FC, Crabb DP. Eye movements during visual search in patients with glaucoma. BMC Ophthalmol 2012; 12:45. [PMID: 22937814 PMCID: PMC3505163 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2415-12-45] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2012] [Accepted: 08/13/2012] [Indexed: 03/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Glaucoma has been shown to lead to disability in many daily tasks including visual search. This study aims to determine whether the saccadic eye movements of people with glaucoma differ from those of people with normal vision, and to investigate the association between eye movements and impaired visual search. Methods Forty patients (mean age: 67 [SD: 9] years) with a range of glaucomatous visual field (VF) defects in both eyes (mean best eye mean deviation [MD]: –5.9 (SD: 5.4) dB) and 40 age-related people with normal vision (mean age: 66 [SD: 10] years) were timed as they searched for a series of target objects in computer displayed photographs of real world scenes. Eye movements were simultaneously recorded using an eye tracker. Average number of saccades per second, average saccade amplitude and average search duration across trials were recorded. These response variables were compared with measurements of VF and contrast sensitivity. Results The average rate of saccades made by the patient group was significantly smaller than the number made by controls during the visual search task (P = 0.02; mean reduction of 5.6% (95% CI: 0.1 to 10.4%). There was no difference in average saccade amplitude between the patients and the controls (P = 0.09). Average number of saccades was weakly correlated with aspects of visual function, with patients with worse contrast sensitivity (PR logCS; Spearman’s rho: 0.42; P = 0.006) and more severe VF defects (best eye MD; Spearman’s rho: 0.34; P = 0.037) tending to make less eye movements during the task. Average detection time in the search task was associated with the average rate of saccades in the patient group (Spearman’s rho = −0.65; P < 0.001) but this was not apparent in the controls. Conclusions The average rate of saccades made during visual search by this group of patients was fewer than those made by people with normal vision of a similar average age. There was wide variability in saccade rate in the patients but there was an association between an increase in this measure and better performance in the search task. Assessment of eye movements in individuals with glaucoma might provide insight into the functional deficits of the disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas D Smith
- Department of Optometry and Visual Science, City University London, London, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Plank T, Frolo J, Farzana F, Brandl-Rühle S, Renner AB, Greenlee MW. Neural correlates of visual search in patients with hereditary retinal dystrophies. Hum Brain Mapp 2012; 34:2607-23. [PMID: 22505353 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2011] [Revised: 02/13/2012] [Accepted: 03/07/2012] [Indexed: 11/11/2022] Open
Abstract
In patients with central visual field scotomata a large part of visual cortex is not adequately stimulated. We investigated evidence for possible upregulation in cortical responses in 22 patients (8 females, 14 males; mean age 41.5 years, range 12-65 years) with central visual field loss due to hereditary retinal dystrophies (Stargardt's disease, other forms of hereditary macular dystrophies and cone-rod dystrophy) and compared their results to those of 22 age-matched controls (11 females, 11 males; mean age, 42.4 years, range, 13-70 years). Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) we recorded differences in behavioral and BOLD signal distribution in retinotopic mapping and visual search tasks. Patients with an established preferred retinal locus (PRL) exhibited significantly higher activation in early visual cortex during the visual search task, especially on trials when the target stimuli fell in the vicinity of the PRL. Compared with those with less stable fixation, patients with stable eccentric fixation at the PRL exhibited greater performance levels and more brain activation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tina Plank
- Institute of Psychology, University of Regensburg, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Pijnacker J, Verstraten P, van Damme W, Vandermeulen J, Steenbergen B. Rehabilitation of reading in older individuals with macular degeneration: A review of effective training programs. AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2011; 18:708-32. [DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2011.613451] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Judith Pijnacker
- a Radboud University Nijmegen, Behavioural Science Institute , Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Peter Verstraten
- b Royal Dutch Visio, National Foundation for Blind and Partially Sighted People , The Netherlands
| | - Wim van Damme
- b Royal Dutch Visio, National Foundation for Blind and Partially Sighted People , The Netherlands
| | - Jo Vandermeulen
- b Royal Dutch Visio, National Foundation for Blind and Partially Sighted People , The Netherlands
| | - Bert Steenbergen
- a Radboud University Nijmegen, Behavioural Science Institute , Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Nguyen NX, Stockum A, Hahn GA, Trauzettel-Klosinski S. Training to improve reading speed in patients with juvenile macular dystrophy: a randomized study comparing two training methods. Acta Ophthalmol 2011; 89:e82-8. [PMID: 21272283 DOI: 10.1111/j.1755-3768.2010.02081.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE In this study, we examined the clinical application of two training methods for optimizing reading ability in patients with juvenile macular dystrophy with established eccentric preferred retinal locus and optimal use of low-vision aids. METHOD This randomized study included 36 patients with juvenile macular dystrophy (35 with Stargardt's disease and one with Best's disease). All patients have been using individually optimized low-vision aids. After careful ophthalmological examination, patients were randomized into two groups: Group 1: Training to read during rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) with elimination of eye movements as far as possible (n = 20); Group 2: Training to optimize reading eye movements (SM, sensomotoric training) (n = 16). Only patients with magnification requirement up to sixfold were included in the study. Training was performed for 4 weeks with an intensity of ½ hr per day and 5 days a week. Reading speed during page reading was measured before and after training. Eye movements during silent reading were recorded before and after training using a video eye tracker in 11 patients (five patients of SM and six of RSVP training group) and using an infrared reflection system in five patients (three patients from the SM and two patients of RSVP training group). RESULTS Age, visual acuity and magnification requirement did not differ significantly between the two groups. The median reading speed was 83 words per minute (wpm) (interquartile range 74-105 wpm) in the RSVP training group and 102 (interquartile range 63-126 wpm) in the SM group before training and increased significantly to 104 (interquartile range 81-124 wpm) and 122, respectively (interquartile range 102-137 wpm; p = 0.01 and 0.006) after training, i.e. patients with RSVP training increased their reading speed by a median of 21 wpm, while it was 20 wpm in the SM group. There were individual patients, who benefited strongly from the training. Eye movement recordings before and after training showed that in the RSVP group, increasing reading speed correlated with decreasing fixation duration (r = -0.75, p = 0.03), whereas in the SM group, increasing reading speed correlated with a decreasing number of forward saccades (r = -0.9, p = 0.01). CONCLUSION Although the median effect of both training methods was limited, individual patients benefited well. Our results may indicate a difference in the training effect between both methods on the reading strategy: the RSVP method reduces fixation duration, the SM method decreases the number of forward saccades. Patients can apply their newly learned reading strategy in the natural reading situation, e.g. in page reading without special presentation of the text. These results can be used as a basis for further improvement in training methods for optimizing reading performance in patients with a central scotoma.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nhung X Nguyen
- Low Vision Clinic and Research Laboratory, Centre for Ophthalmology, University of Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|