1
|
Kueser JB, Horvath S, Borovsky A. Two pathways in vocabulary development: Large-scale differences in noun and verb semantic structure. Cogn Psychol 2023; 143:101574. [PMID: 37209501 PMCID: PMC10832511 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2022] [Revised: 04/25/2023] [Accepted: 05/07/2023] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
In adults, nouns and verbs have varied and multilevel semantic interrelationships. In children, evidence suggests that nouns and verbs also have semantic interrelationships, though the timing of the emergence of these relationships and their precise impact on later noun and verb learning are not clear. In this work, we ask whether noun and verb semantic knowledge in 16-30-month-old children tend to be semantically isolated from one another or semantically interacting from the onset of vocabulary development. Early word learning patterns were quantified using network science. We measured the semantic network structure for nouns and verbs in 3,804 16-30-month-old children at several levels of granularity using a large, open dataset of vocabulary checklist data. In a cross-sectional approach in Experiment 1, early nouns and verbs exhibited stronger network relationships with other nouns and verbs than expected across multiple network levels. Using a longitudinal approach in Experiment 2, we examined patterns of normative vocabulary development over time. Initial noun and verb learning was supported by strong semantic connections to other nouns, whereas later-learned words exhibited strong connections to verbs. Overall, these two experiments suggest that nouns and verbs demonstrate early semantic interactions and that these interactions impact later word learning. Early verb and noun learning is affected by the emergence of noun and verb semantic networks during early lexical development.
Collapse
|
2
|
Kawai Y, Oshima Y, Sasamoto Y, Nagai Y, Asada M. A Computational Model for Child Inferences of Word Meanings via Syntactic Categories for Different Ages and Languages. IEEE Trans Cogn Dev Syst 2020. [DOI: 10.1109/tcds.2018.2883048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
3
|
Lohmann A. No Acoustic Correlates of Grammatical Class: A Critical Re-Examination of Sereno and Jongman (1995). PHONETICA 2020; 77:429-440. [PMID: 32172248 DOI: 10.1159/000506138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2019] [Accepted: 01/04/2020] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
The present article describes an extended replication of a reading study by Sereno and Jongman (1995) that reported acoustic differences between noun and verb pronunciations of English disyllabic, non-stress-shifting homophone pairs, for example, answer (v) versus answer (n). The original findings point to a gradual influence of the typical stress patterns of both grammatical categories, with noun readings exhibiting a tendency toward trochaic and verb readings toward iambic pronunciation. However, the effects found by Sereno and Jongman (1995) did not consistently reach statistical significance and were based on a very small sample size. Employing a considerably larger group of speakers, the current replication fails to find the reported effects of the grammatical category. The null result obtained can be accounted for within speech production models that assume identical metrical templates for the homophone pairs tested and no direct influence of the grammatical category on the phonetics of stress assignment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arne Lohmann
- Department of English and American Studies, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany,
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ulicheva A, Harvey H, Aronoff M, Rastle K. Skilled readers' sensitivity to meaningful regularities in English writing. Cognition 2018; 195:103810. [PMID: 30509872 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2018] [Revised: 08/31/2018] [Accepted: 09/22/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Substantial research has been undertaken to understand the relationship between spelling and sound, but we know little about the relationship between spelling and meaning in alphabetic writing systems. We present a computational analysis of English writing in which we develop new constructs to describe this relationship. Diagnosticity captures the amount of meaningful information in a given spelling, whereas specificity estimates the degree of dispersion of this meaning across different spellings for a particular sound sequence. Using these two constructs, we demonstrate that particular suffix spellings tend to be reserved for particular meaningful functions. We then show across three paradigms (nonword classification, spelling, and eye tracking during sentence reading) that this form of regularity between spelling and meaning influences the behaviour of skilled readers, and that the degree of this behavioural sensitivity mirrors the strength of spelling-to-meaning regularities in the writing system. We close by arguing that English spelling may have become fractionated such that the high degree of spelling-sound inconsistency maximises the transmission of meaningful information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Hannah Harvey
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, UK
| | - Mark Aronoff
- Department of Linguistics, Stony Brook University, USA
| | - Kathleen Rastle
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Christiansen MH, Monaghan P. Division of Labor in Vocabulary Structure: Insights From Corpus Analyses. Top Cogn Sci 2015; 8:610-24. [DOI: 10.1111/tops.12164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2013] [Revised: 03/11/2014] [Accepted: 05/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
6
|
Curtin S, Zamuner TS. Understanding the developing sound system: interactions between sounds and words. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2015; 5:589-602. [PMID: 26308747 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2014] [Revised: 06/17/2014] [Accepted: 07/18/2014] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Over the course of the first 2 years of life, infants are learning a great deal about the sound system of their native language. Acquiring the sound system requires the infant to learn about sounds and their distributions, sound combinations, and prosodic information, such as syllables, rhythm, and stress. These aspects of the phonological system are being learned simultaneously as the infant experiences the language around him or her. What binds all of the phonological units is the context in which they occur, namely, words. In this review, we explore the development of phonetics and phonology by showcasing the interactive nature of the developing lexicon and sound system with a focus on perception. We first review seminal research in the foundations of phonological development. We then discuss early word recognition and learning followed by a discussion of phonological and lexical representations. We conclude by discussing the interactive nature of lexical and phonological representations and highlight some further directions for exploring the developing sound system. WIREs Cogn Sci 2014, 5:589-602. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1307 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The authors have declared no conflicts of interest for this article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Suzanne Curtin
- Department of Psycology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada
| | - Tania S Zamuner
- Department of Linguistics, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Monaghan P, Shillcock RC, Christiansen MH, Kirby S. How arbitrary is language? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2015; 369:20130299. [PMID: 25092667 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0299] [Citation(s) in RCA: 96] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
It is a long established convention that the relationship between sounds and meanings of words is essentially arbitrary--typically the sound of a word gives no hint of its meaning. However, there are numerous reported instances of systematic sound-meaning mappings in language, and this systematicity has been claimed to be important for early language development. In a large-scale corpus analysis of English, we show that sound-meaning mappings are more systematic than would be expected by chance. Furthermore, this systematicity is more pronounced for words involved in the early stages of language acquisition and reduces in later vocabulary development. We propose that the vocabulary is structured to enable systematicity in early language learning to promote language acquisition, while also incorporating arbitrariness for later language in order to facilitate communicative expressivity and efficiency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Padraic Monaghan
- Centre for Research in Human Development and Learning, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, UK
| | - Richard C Shillcock
- School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AD, UK School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AD, UK
| | - Morten H Christiansen
- Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
| | - Simon Kirby
- School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9AD, UK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Recchia G, Sahlgren M, Kanerva P, Jones MN. Encoding sequential information in semantic space models: comparing holographic reduced representation and random permutation. COMPUTATIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND NEUROSCIENCE 2015; 2015:986574. [PMID: 25954306 PMCID: PMC4405220 DOI: 10.1155/2015/986574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2014] [Accepted: 02/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Circular convolution and random permutation have each been proposed as neurally plausible binding operators capable of encoding sequential information in semantic memory. We perform several controlled comparisons of circular convolution and random permutation as means of encoding paired associates as well as encoding sequential information. Random permutations outperformed convolution with respect to the number of paired associates that can be reliably stored in a single memory trace. Performance was equal on semantic tasks when using a small corpus, but random permutations were ultimately capable of achieving superior performance due to their higher scalability to large corpora. Finally, "noisy" permutations in which units are mapped to other units arbitrarily (no one-to-one mapping) perform nearly as well as true permutations. These findings increase the neurological plausibility of random permutations and highlight their utility in vector space models of semantics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Pentti Kanerva
- Redwood Center for Theoretical Neuroscience, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Distributional structure in language: contributions to noun-verb difficulty differences in infant word recognition. Cognition 2014; 132:429-36. [PMID: 24908342 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2009] [Revised: 05/08/2014] [Accepted: 05/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
What makes some words easy for infants to recognize, and other words difficult? We addressed this issue in the context of prior results suggesting that infants have difficulty recognizing verbs relative to nouns. In this work, we highlight the role played by the distributional contexts in which nouns and verbs occur. Distributional statistics predict that English nouns should generally be easier to recognize than verbs in fluent speech. However, there are situations in which distributional statistics provide similar support for verbs. The statistics for verbs that occur with the English morpheme -ing, for example, should facilitate verb recognition. In two experiments with 7.5- and 9.5-month-old infants, we tested the importance of distributional statistics for word recognition by varying the frequency of the contextual frames in which verbs occur. The results support the conclusion that distributional statistics are utilized by infant language learners and contribute to noun-verb differences in word recognition.
Collapse
|
10
|
|
11
|
Sundara M, Demuth K, Kuhl PK. Sentence-position effects on children's perception and production of English third person singular -s. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2011; 54:55-71. [PMID: 20705740 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0056)] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Two-year-olds produce third person singular -s more accurately on verbs in sentence-final position as compared with verbs in sentence-medial position. This study was designed to determine whether these sentence-position effects can be explained by perceptual factors. METHOD For this purpose, the authors compared 22- and 27-month-olds' perception and elicited production of third person singular -s in sentence-medial versus-final position. The authors assessed perception by measuring looking/listening times to a 1-screen display of a cartoon paired with a grammatical versus an ungrammatical sentence (e.g., She eats now vs. She eat now). RESULTS Children at both ages demonstrated sensitivity to the presence/absence of this inflectional morpheme in sentence-final, but not sentence-medial, position. Children were also more accurate at producing third person singular -s sentence finally, and production accuracy was predicted by vocabulary measures as well as by performance on the perception task. CONCLUSIONS These results indicate that children's more accurate production of third person singular -s in sentence-final position cannot be explained by articulatory factors alone but that perceptual factors play an important role in accounting for early patterns of production. The findings also indicate that perception and production of inflectional morphemes may be more closely related than previously thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megha Sundara
- Department of Linguistics, University of California, Los Angeles, 3125 Campbell Hall, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1543, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
More data trumps smarter algorithms: Comparing pointwise mutual information with latent semantic analysis. Behav Res Methods 2009; 41:647-56. [PMID: 19587174 DOI: 10.3758/brm.41.3.647] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
13
|
Christiansen MH, Onnis L, Hockema SA. The secret is in the sound: from unsegmented speech to lexical categories. Dev Sci 2009; 12:388-95. [PMID: 19371361 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00824.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
When learning language, young children are faced with many seemingly formidable challenges, including discovering words embedded in a continuous stream of sounds and determining what role these words play in syntactic constructions. We suggest that knowledge of phoneme distributions may play a crucial part in helping children segment words and determine their lexical category, and we propose an integrated model of how children might go from unsegmented speech to lexical categories. We corroborated this theoretical model using a two-stage computational analysis of a large corpus of English child-directed speech. First, we used transition probabilities between phonemes to find words in unsegmented speech. Second, we used distributional information about word edges--the beginning and ending phonemes of words--to predict whether the segmented words from the first stage were nouns, verbs, or something else. The results indicate that discovering lexical units and their associated syntactic category in child-directed speech is possible by attending to the statistics of single phoneme transitions and word-initial and final phonemes. Thus, we suggest that a core computational principle in language acquisition is that the same source of information is used to learn about different aspects of linguistic structure.
Collapse
|
14
|
Orthographic influences in spoken word recognition: the consistency effect in semantic and gender categorization tasks. Psychon Bull Rev 2009; 16:363-8. [PMID: 19293108 DOI: 10.3758/pbr.16.2.363] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
According to current models, spoken word recognition is driven by the phonological properties of the speech signal. However, several studies have suggested that orthographic information also influences recognition in adult listeners. In particular, it has been repeatedly shown that, in the lexical decision task, words that include rimes with inconsistent spellings (e.g., /-ip/ spelled -eap or -eep) are disadvantaged, as compared with words with consistent rime spelling. In the present study, we explored whether the orthographic consistency effect extends to tasks requiring people to process words beyond simple lexical access. Two different tasks were used: semantic and gender categorization. Both tasks produced reliable consistency effects. The data are discussed as suggesting that orthographic codes are activated during word recognition, or that the organization of phonological representations of words is affected by orthography during literacy acquisition.
Collapse
|