1
|
Mousley VL, MacSweeney M, Mercure E. Revisiting perceptual sensitivity to non-native speech in a diverse sample of bilinguals. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 76:101959. [PMID: 38781790 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2024] [Revised: 04/18/2024] [Accepted: 05/12/2024] [Indexed: 05/25/2024]
Abstract
Werker and Tees (1984) prompted decades of research attempting to detail the paths infants take towards specialisation for the sounds of their native language(s). Most of this research has examined the trajectories of monolingual children. However, it has also been proposed that bilinguals, who are exposed to greater phonetic variability than monolinguals and must learn the rules of two languages, may remain perceptually open to non-native language sounds later into life than monolinguals. Using a visual habituation paradigm, the current study tests this question by comparing 15- to 18-month-old monolingual and bilingual children's developmental trajectories for non-native phonetic consonant contrast discrimination. A novel approach to the integration of stimulus presentation software with eye-tracking software was validated for objective measurement of infant looking time. The results did not support the hypothesis of a protracted period of sensitivity to non-native phonetic contrasts in bilingual compared to monolingual infants. Implications for diversification of perceptual narrowing research and implementation of increasingly sensitive measures are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria L Mousley
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom.
| | - Mairéad MacSweeney
- Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London, London WC1H 0PD, United Kingdom; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AZ, United Kingdom.
| | - Evelyne Mercure
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London SE14 6NW, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kachergis G, Masek LR, Gonzalez SL, Soska KC, Herzberg O, Xu M, Adolph KE, Gilmore RO, Bornstein MH, Casasola M, Fausey CM, Frank MC, Goldin-Meadow S, Gros-Louis J, Hirsh-Pasek K, Iverson J, Lew-Williams C, MacWhinney B, Marchman VA, Naigles L, Namy L, Perry LK, Rowe M, Sheya A, Soderstrom M, Song L, Walle E, Warlaumont AS, Yoshida H, Yu C, Yurovsky D. Comparing apples to manzanas and oranges to naranjas: A new measure of English-Spanish vocabulary for dual language learners. INFANCY 2024; 29:302-326. [PMID: 38217508 PMCID: PMC11019594 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12571] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2023] [Revised: 11/20/2023] [Accepted: 11/21/2023] [Indexed: 01/15/2024]
Abstract
The valid assessment of vocabulary development in dual-language-learning infants is critical to developmental science. We developed the Dual Language Learners English-Spanish (DLL-ES) Inventories to measure vocabularies of U.S. English-Spanish DLLs. The inventories provide translation equivalents for all Spanish and English items on Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) short forms; extended inventories based on CDI long forms; and Spanish language-variety options. Item-Response Theory analyses applied to Wordbank and Web-CDI data (n = 2603, 12-18 months; n = 6722, 16-36 months; half female; 1% Asian, 3% Black, 2% Hispanic, 30% White, 64% unknown) showed near-perfect associations between DLL-ES and CDI long-form scores. Interviews with 10 Hispanic mothers of 18- to 24-month-olds (2 White, 1 Black, 7 multi-racial; 6 female) provide a proof of concept for the value of the DLL-ES for assessing the vocabularies of DLLs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Marc H. Bornstein
- Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, MD, USA
- Institute for Fiscal Studies, UK
- UNICEF, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Laura Namy
- Institute for Education Sciences, Washington, DC, USA
| | | | | | - Adam Sheya
- University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA
| | | | | | - Eric Walle
- University of California Merced, Merced, CA, USA
| | | | | | - Chen Yu
- University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Kaushanskaya M. Combining Languages in Bilingual Input: Using Experimental Evidence to Formulate Bilingual Exposure Strategies. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023; 66:4771-4784. [PMID: 37732839 PMCID: PMC11361785 DOI: 10.1044/2023_jslhr-23-00181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2023] [Revised: 06/07/2023] [Accepted: 06/20/2023] [Indexed: 09/22/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Traditional approaches to studying bilingual language development through bilingual-monolingual comparisons are deeply flawed. They are also insufficient as the evidence base for informing advice to bilingual parents regarding the optimal bilingual exposure strategy and for supporting the formulation of bilingual intervention approaches. The purpose of this review article is to provide an overview of empirical studies that have queried the question of how different types of dual-language input shape learning and language outcomes in bilingual children. METHOD We rely on tightly controlled experimental studies of word learning in Spanish-English bilingual children, where we contrast children's learning in dual-language conditions (defined as distributed exposure and code-switched input) to a single-language condition in a within-subjects design. RESULTS Word-learning studies querying the role of distributed exposure indicated that distribution of exposures across Spanish and English reduced children's performance in comparison to English-only exposure. However, this effect was rooted in the abrupt switch from Spanish to English rather than distributed exposure itself. In contrast, an experiment designed to test the role of code-switched context on children's word learning revealed that code-switched context where switches resembled naturalistic code-switching behaviors enhanced learning in Spanish-English bilingual children. Notably, across different studies, children with weaker language skills (developmental language disorder) were no more affected by dual-language input than children with typical language skills. CONCLUSIONS Together, experimental studies of word learning indicate that bilingual children can effectively learn from dual-language input but that different ways of combining languages in the input to bilingual children can have distinct consequences for learning. Ultimately, word-learning experiments, beyond answering critical questions regarding bilingual learning, can serve as an effective bridge between laboratory-based work and intervention studies whose goal it is to discover the optimal way of combining languages in the input to bilingual children with communication impairments. PRESENTATION VIDEO https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.23929515.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margarita Kaushanskaya
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin–Madison
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin–Madison
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Tsui RKY, Kosie JE, Fibla L, Lew-Williams C, Byers-Heinlein K. Patterns of language switching and bilingual children's word learning: An experiment across two communities. TRANSLATIONAL ISSUES IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2023; 9:323-337. [PMID: 38405269 PMCID: PMC10883668 DOI: 10.1037/tps0000353] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/27/2024]
Abstract
Language switching is common in bilingual environments, including those of many bilingual children. Some bilingual children hear rapid switching that involves immediate translation of words (an 'immediate-translation' pattern), while others hear their languages most often in long blocks of a single language (a 'one-language-at-a-time' pattern). Our two-site experimental study compared two groups of developing bilinguals from different communities, and investigated whether differences in the timing of language switching impose different demands on bilingual children's learning of novel nouns in their two languages: do children learn differently if they hear a translation immediately vs. if they hear translations more separated in time? Using an at-home online tablet word learning task, data were collected asynchronously from 3- to 5-year-old bilinguals from French-English bilingual families in Montreal, Canada (N = 31) and Spanish-English bilingual families in New Jersey, USA (N = 22). Results showed that bilingual children in both communities readily learned new words, and their performance was similar across the immediate-translation and one-language-at-a-time conditions. Our findings highlight that different types of bilingual interactions can provide equal learning opportunities for bilingual children's vocabulary development.
Collapse
|
5
|
Ruan Y, Byers-Heinlein K, Orena AJ, Polka L. Mixed-Language Input and Infant Volubility: Friend or Foe? BILINGUALISM (CAMBRIDGE, ENGLAND) 2023; 26:1051-1066. [PMID: 38187471 PMCID: PMC10769107 DOI: 10.1017/s1366728923000287] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2024]
Abstract
Language mixing is a common feature of many bilingually-raised children's input. Yet how it is related to their language development remains an open question. The current study investigated mixed-language input indexed by observed (30-second segment) counts and proportions in day-long recordings as well as parent-reported scores, in relation to infant vocal activeness (i.e., volubility) when infants were 10 and 18 months old. Results suggested infants who received a higher score or proportion of mixed input in one-on-one social contexts were less voluble. However, within contexts involving language mixing, infants who heard more words were also the ones who produced more vocalizations. These divergent associations between mixed input and infant vocal development point for a need to better understand the causal factors that drive these associations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yufang Ruan
- School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
- Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Krista Byers-Heinlein
- Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, Montréal, Québec, Canada
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
- Centre for Research in Human Development, Concordia University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| | - Adriel John Orena
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Linda Polka
- School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada
- Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, Montréal, Québec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Zettersten M, Yurovsky D, Xu TL, Uner S, Tsui ASM, Schneider RM, Saleh AN, Meylan SC, Marchman VA, Mankewitz J, MacDonald K, Long B, Lewis M, Kachergis G, Handa K, deMayo B, Carstensen A, Braginsky M, Boyce V, Bhatt NS, Bergey CA, Frank MC. Peekbank: An open, large-scale repository for developmental eye-tracking data of children's word recognition. Behav Res Methods 2023; 55:2485-2500. [PMID: 36002623 PMCID: PMC9950292 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-022-01906-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/27/2022] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
The ability to rapidly recognize words and link them to referents is central to children's early language development. This ability, often called word recognition in the developmental literature, is typically studied in the looking-while-listening paradigm, which measures infants' fixation on a target object (vs. a distractor) after hearing a target label. We present a large-scale, open database of infant and toddler eye-tracking data from looking-while-listening tasks. The goal of this effort is to address theoretical and methodological challenges in measuring vocabulary development. We first present how we created the database, its features and structure, and associated tools for processing and accessing infant eye-tracking datasets. Using these tools, we then work through two illustrative examples to show how researchers can use Peekbank to interrogate theoretical and methodological questions about children's developing word recognition ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin Zettersten
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, 218 Peretsman Scully Hall, Princeton, NJ, 08540, USA.
| | - Daniel Yurovsky
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Tian Linger Xu
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
| | - Sarp Uner
- Data Science Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
| | | | - Rose M Schneider
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Annissa N Saleh
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
| | - Stephan C Meylan
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | | | | | | | - Bria Long
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Molly Lewis
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - George Kachergis
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | | | - Benjamin deMayo
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, 218 Peretsman Scully Hall, Princeton, NJ, 08540, USA
| | | | - Mika Braginsky
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Veronica Boyce
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Naiti S Bhatt
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
| | | | - Michael C Frank
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Mousley VL, MacSweeney M, Mercure E. Bilingual toddlers show increased attention capture by static faces compared to monolinguals. BILINGUALISM (CAMBRIDGE, ENGLAND) 2023; 26:835-844. [PMID: 37636491 PMCID: PMC7614981 DOI: 10.1017/s136672892200092x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/29/2023]
Abstract
Bilingual infants rely differently than monolinguals on facial information, such as lip patterns, to differentiate their native languages. This may explain, at least in part, why young monolinguals and bilinguals show differences in social attention. For example, in the first year, bilinguals attend faster and more often to static faces over non-faces than do monolinguals (Mercure et al., 2018). However, the developmental trajectories of these differences are unknown. In this pre-registered study, data were collected from 15- to 18-month-old monolinguals (English) and bilinguals (English and another language) to test whether group differences in face-looking behaviour persist into the second year. We predicted that bilinguals would orient more rapidly and more often to static faces than monolinguals. Results supported the first but not the second hypothesis. This suggests that, even into the second year of life, toddlers' rapid visual orientation to static social stimuli is sensitive to early language experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria L Mousley
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
- Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Mairéad MacSweeney
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
- Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Evelyne Mercure
- Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
- Goldsmiths, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Potter CE, Lew-Williams C. Frequent vs. infrequent words shape toddlers' real-time sentence comprehension. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2023:1-11. [PMID: 37401467 PMCID: PMC10764636 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000923000387] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/05/2023]
Abstract
We examined how noun frequency and the typicality of surrounding linguistic context contribute to children's real-time comprehension. Monolingual English-learning toddlers viewed pairs of pictures while hearing sentences with typical or atypical sentence frames (Look at the… vs. Examine the…), followed by nouns that were higher- or lower-frequency labels for a referent (horse vs. pony). Toddlers showed no significant differences in comprehension of nouns in typical and atypical sentence frames. However, they were less accurate in recognizing lower-frequency nouns, particularly among toddlers with smaller vocabularies. We conclude that toddlers can recognize nouns in diverse sentence contexts, but their representations develop gradually.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine E Potter
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at El Paso, USA
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Kaushanskaya M, Crespo K, Neveu A. Does code-switching influence novel word learning? Dev Sci 2023; 26:e13292. [PMID: 35639763 PMCID: PMC10163668 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13292] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2021] [Revised: 03/24/2022] [Accepted: 04/29/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Code-switching occurs regularly in the input to bilingual children. Yet, the effect of code-switched input on language development is unclear. To test whether word learning would be affected by code-switching, Spanish-English bilingual children (N = 45, 19 boys, MeanAge = 5.05 years; ethnicity: 37 Hispanic/Latino, six Non-Hispanic/Latino, two unreported) were taught English-like novel words in two conditions. In the English-only condition, definitions for novel words were provided entirely in English. In the code-switch condition, definitions for novel words were provided in English and Spanish, incorporating code-switches. Children required fewer exposures to retain novel words in the code-switch than the English-only condition and this effect was not moderated by children's language ability or exposure to code-switching, suggesting that code-switched input does not pose word-learning risks to bilingual children, including children with lower levels of language ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Anne Neveu
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Shi Z, Xiao F, Yan H, Guo J. Cost or advantage? Intra-sentential language switching could facilitate L2 emotional words' comprehension in auditory modality. Int J Psychophysiol 2023; 184:51-63. [PMID: 36584902 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.12.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2022] [Revised: 12/20/2022] [Accepted: 12/22/2022] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
Language switching is encountered commonly and inevitably in bilingual society and often induces costs for target language's production. However, for auditory words' comprehension at sentence level, the limited research showed divergent findings. Some research observed comprehension costs when the language of target words was switched with the code of sentential contexts kept constant; but a potential switch advantage was also showed in non-dominant targets' processing when sentential context switches occurred. Additionally, it's well documented that the words' emotional connotations play a key role in both L1 and L2 word comprehension. Therefore, we aimed to explore which switch effect would occur when bilinguals comprehended L1/L2 emotional target nouns in auditory modality at behavioral and neural level through a visual object selection task. Behaviorally, switch related costs occurred in L1 targets' comprehension, whereas advantage effects were found in L2. Moreover, greater switch advantage occurred for positive and negative targets than for neutral ones. Consistently, larger LPC (Late Positive Component) defection was elicited for L2-Switch trials relative to L2-Nonswitch trials and the differences of LPC's amplitude could predict the behavioral advantageous effects of switching in nondominant targets' comprehension, which suggest that language switching lead to deeper re-analyses for emotional words. Taken together, it's suggested that bilinguals can adaptively utilize top-down (sentential prediction) and bottom-up (words' emotional information) cues to access weaker L2 representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhengwei Shi
- Shaanxi Provincial Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China
| | - Feng Xiao
- Shaanxi Provincial Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China
| | - Hao Yan
- Key Laboratory for Artificial Intelligence and Cognitive Neuroscience of Language, Xi'an International Studies University, Xi'an, China
| | - Jingjing Guo
- Shaanxi Provincial Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Cychosz M. LANGUAGE EXPOSURE PREDICTS CHILDREN'S PHONETIC PATTERNING: EVIDENCE FROM LANGUAGE SHIFT. LANGUAGE 2022; 98:461-509. [PMID: 37034148 PMCID: PMC10079255 DOI: 10.1353/lan.0.0269] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/19/2023]
Abstract
Although understanding the role of the environment is central to language acquisition theory, rarely has this been studied for children's phonetic development, and receptive and expressive language experiences in the environment are not distinguished. This last distinction may be crucial for child speech production in particular, because production requires coordination of low-level speech-motor planning with high-level linguistic knowledge. In this study, the role of the environment is evaluated in a novel way-by studying phonetic development in a bilingual community undergoing rapid language shift. This sociolinguistic context provides a naturalistic gradient of the amount of children's exposure to two languages and the ratio of expressive to receptive experiences. A large-scale child language corpus encompassing over 500 hours of naturalistic South Bolivian Quechua and Spanish speech was efficiently annotated for children's and their caregivers' bilingual language use. These estimates were correlated with children's patterns in a series of speech production tasks. The role of the environment varied by outcome: children's expressive language experience best predicted their performance on a coarticulation-morphology measure, while their receptive experience predicted performance on a lower-level measure of vowel variability. Overall these bilingual exposure effects suggest a pathway for children's role in language change whereby language shift can result in different learning outcomes within a single speech community. Appropriate ways to model language exposure in development are discussed.
Collapse
|
12
|
Erel Y, Potter CE, Jaffe‐Dax S, Lew‐Williams C, Bermano AH. iCatcher: A neural network approach for automated coding of young children's eye movements. INFANCY 2022; 27:765-779. [PMID: 35416378 PMCID: PMC9320879 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12468] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2021] [Revised: 03/02/2022] [Accepted: 03/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Infants' looking behaviors are often used for measuring attention, real-time processing, and learning-often using low-resolution videos. Despite the ubiquity of gaze-related methods in developmental science, current analysis techniques usually involve laborious post hoc coding, imprecise real-time coding, or expensive eye trackers that may increase data loss and require a calibration phase. As an alternative, we propose using computer vision methods to perform automatic gaze estimation from low-resolution videos. At the core of our approach is a neural network that classifies gaze directions in real time. We compared our method, called iCatcher, to manually annotated videos from a prior study in which infants looked at one of two pictures on a screen. We demonstrated that the accuracy of iCatcher approximates that of human annotators and that it replicates the prior study's results. Our method is publicly available as an open-source repository at https://github.com/yoterel/iCatcher.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yotam Erel
- School of Computer ScienceTel Aviv UniversityTel AvivIsrael
| | - Christine E. Potter
- Department of PsychologyPrinceton UniversityPrincetonNew JerseyUSA
- Department of PsychologyThe University of Texas at El PasoEl PasoTexasUSA
| | - Sagi Jaffe‐Dax
- Department of PsychologyPrinceton UniversityPrincetonNew JerseyUSA
- School of Psychological Sciences and Segol School for NeuroscienceTel Aviv UniversityTel AvivIsrael
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
How Many Palabras? Codeswitching and Lexical Diversity in Spanish-English Picture Books. LANGUAGES 2022. [DOI: 10.3390/languages7010069] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Bilingual picture books have been growing in popularity, with caregivers, teachers, and researchers increasingly interested in understanding how picture books might be able to support the learning of words in two languages. In this study, we present the first evaluation of the quantity and quality of text contained within bilingual picture books in English and Spanish targeted to children ages 0–9 and available to parents in the United States. We focus specifically on a sample of codeswitching books (N = 45) which present text in one language embedded in another language. All books were transcribed and evaluated for (1) the number of words and utterances presented in each language; (2) the quality and complexity of text presented in each language; and (3) how switching occurred between the two languages. Results showed that although picture books in our sample presented predominantly English text and more complex English sentences, relatively more unique words were presented in Spanish. Furthermore, picture books in our sample presented frequent switching between languages, particularly within utterances. We suggest that bilingual picture books provide children with potentially enriching yet asymmetrical opportunities for learning in each language.
Collapse
|
14
|
Brouillard M, Dubé D, Byers‐Heinlein K. Reading to bilingual preschoolers: An experimental study of two book formats. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daphnée Dubé
- Department of Psychology Concordia University Montreal Quebec Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Byers-Heinlein K, Jardak A, Fourakis E, Lew-Williams C. Effects of language mixing on bilingual children's word learning. BILINGUALISM (CAMBRIDGE, ENGLAND) 2022; 25:55-69. [PMID: 35399292 PMCID: PMC8992731 DOI: 10.1017/s1366728921000699] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
Language mixing is common in bilingual children's learning environments. Here, we investigated effects of language mixing on children's learning of new words. We tested two groups of 3-year-old bilinguals: French-English (Experiment 1) and Spanish-English (Experiment 2). Children were taught two novel words, one in single-language sentences ("Look! Do you see the dog on the teelo?") and one in mixed-language sentences with a mid-sentence language switch ("Look! Do you see the chien/perro on the walem?"). During the learning phase, children correctly identified novel targets when hearing both single-language and mixed-language sentences. However, at test, French-English bilinguals did not successfully recognize the word encountered in mixed-language sentences. Spanish-English bilinguals failed to recognize either word, which underscores the importance of examining multiple bilingual populations. This research suggests that language mixing may sometimes hinder children's encoding of novel words that occur downstream, but leaves open several possible underlying mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Amel Jardak
- Concordia University, Department of Psychology, Montreal, Canada
| | - Eva Fourakis
- Princeton University, Department of Psychology, Princeton, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Schott E, Mastroberardino M, Fourakis E, Lew-Williams C, Byers-Heinlein K. Fine-tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants' detection of language switching. INFANCY 2021; 26:1037-1056. [PMID: 34482624 PMCID: PMC8530864 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12429] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2020] [Revised: 06/13/2021] [Accepted: 07/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The ability to differentiate between two languages sets the stage for bilingual learning. Infants can discriminate languages when hearing long passages, but language switches often occur on short time scales with few cues to language identity. As bilingual infants begin learning sequences of sounds and words, how do they detect the dynamics of two languages? In two studies using the head-turn preference procedure, we investigated whether infants (n = 44) can discriminate languages at the level of individual words. In Study 1, bilingual and monolingual 8- to 12-month-olds were tested on their detection of single-word language switching in lists of words (e.g., "dog… lait [fr. milk]"). In Study 2, they were tested on language switching within sentences (e.g., "Do you like the lait?"). We found that infants were unable to detect language switching in lists of words, but the results were inconclusive about infants' ability to detect language switching within sentences. No differences were observed between bilinguals and monolinguals. Given that bilingual proficiency eventually requires detection of sound sequences across two languages, more research will be needed to conclusively understand when and how this skill emerges. Materials, data, and analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/9dtwn/.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esther Schott
- Concordia University
- Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Cychosz M, Villanueva A, Weisleder A. Efficient Estimation of Children's Language Exposure in Two Bilingual Communities. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2021; 64:3843-3866. [PMID: 34520232 PMCID: PMC9132038 DOI: 10.1044/2021_jslhr-20-00755] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/10/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The language that children hear early in life is associated with their speech-language outcomes. This line of research relies on naturalistic observations of children's language input, often captured with daylong audio recordings. However, the large quantity of data that daylong recordings generate requires novel analytical tools to feasibly parse thousands of hours of naturalistic speech. This study outlines a new approach to efficiently process and sample from daylong audio recordings made in two bilingual communities, Spanish-English in the United States and Quechua-Spanish in Bolivia, to derive estimates of children's language exposure. Method We employed a general sampling with replacement technique to efficiently estimate two key elements of children's early language environments: (a) proportion of child-directed speech (CDS) and (b) dual language exposure. Proportions estimated from random sampling of 30-s segments were compared to those from annotations over the entire daylong recording (every other segment), as well as parental report of dual language exposure. Results Results showed that approximately 49 min from each recording or just 7% of the overall recording was required to reach a stable proportion of CDS and bilingual exposure. In both speech communities, strong correlations were found between bilingual language estimates made using random sampling and all-day annotation techniques. A strong association was additionally found for CDS estimates in the United States, but this was weaker at the Bolivian site, where CDS was less frequent. Dual language estimates from the audio recordings did not correspond well to estimates derived from parental report collected months apart. Conclusions Daylong recordings offer tremendous insight into children's daily language experiences, but they will not become widely used in developmental research until data processing and annotation time substantially decrease. We show that annotation based on random sampling is a promising approach to efficiently estimate ambient characteristics from daylong recordings that cannot currently be estimated via automated methods.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margaret Cychosz
- Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of Maryland, College Park
- Center for Comparative and Evolutionary Biology of Hearing, College Park, MD
| | - Anele Villanueva
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL
| | - Adriana Weisleder
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Gade M, Declerck M, Philipp AM, Rey-Mermet A, Koch I. Assessing the Evidence for Asymmetrical Switch Costs and Reversed Language Dominance Effects - A Meta-Analysis. J Cogn 2021; 4:55. [PMID: 34611575 PMCID: PMC8447966 DOI: 10.5334/joc.186] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2020] [Accepted: 08/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Two seemingly counterintuitive phenomena - asymmetrical language switch costs and the reversed language dominance effect - prove to be particularly controversial in the literature on language control. Asymmetrical language switch costs refer to the larger costs for switching into the dominant language compared to switching into the less dominant language, both relative to staying in either one language. The reversed language dominance effect refers to longer reaction times when in the more dominant of the two languages in situations that require frequent language switching (i.e., mixed-language blocks). The asymmetrical language switch costs are commonly taken as an index for processes of transient, reactive inhibitory language control, whereas the reversed language dominance effect is taken as an index for sustained, proactive inhibitory language control. In the present meta-analysis, we set out to establish the empirical evidence for these two phenomena using a Bayesian linear mixed effects modelling approach. Despite the observation of both phenomena in some studies, our results suggest that overall, there is little evidence for the generality and robustness of these two effects, and this holds true even when conditions - such as language proficiency and preparation time manipulations - were included as moderators of these phenomena. We conclude that asymmetrical switch costs and the reversed language dominance effect are important for theory development, but their utility for theory testing is limited due to their lack of robustness and the absence of confirmed moderatory variables.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miriam Gade
- Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt, Department of Psychology, General Psychology
- Medical School Berlin, Department of Sciences, Berlin, Germany
| | - Mathieu Declerck
- Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Department of Linguistics and Literary Studies, Brussels, Belgium
| | | | | | - Iring Koch
- RWTH Aachen University, Department of Psychology, Aachen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Gross MC, Patel H, Kaushanskaya M. Processing of Code-Switched Sentences in Noise by Bilingual Children. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2021; 64:1283-1302. [PMID: 33788593 PMCID: PMC8608215 DOI: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-20-00388] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2020] [Revised: 10/26/2020] [Accepted: 12/15/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The purpose of the current study was to examine the effects of code-switching on bilingual children's online processing and offline comprehension of sentences in the presence of noise. In addition, the study examined individual differences in language ability and cognitive control skills as moderators of children's ability to process code-switched sentences in noise. Method The participants were 50 Spanish-English bilingual children, ages 7;0-11;8 (years;months). Children completed an auditory moving window task to examine whether they processed sentences with code-switching more slowly and less accurately than single-language sentences in the presence of noise. They completed the Dimensional Change Card Sort task to index cognitive control and standardized language measures in English and Spanish to index relative language dominance and overall language ability. Results Children were significantly less accurate in answering offline comprehension questions about code-switched sentences presented in noise compared to single-language sentences, especially for their dominant language. They also tended to exhibit slower processing speed, but costs did not reach significance. Language ability had an overall effect on offline comprehension but did not moderate the effects of code-switching. Cognitive control moderated the extent to which offline comprehension costs were affected by language dominance. Conclusions The findings of the current study suggest that code-switching, especially in the presence of background noise, may place additional demands on children's ability to comprehend sentences. However, it may be the processing of the nondominant language, rather than code-switching per se, that is especially difficult in the presence of noise.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan C Gross
- Department of Communication Disorders, University of Massachusetts Amherst
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Tsui ASM, Erickson LC, Mallikarjunn A, Thiessen ED, Fennell CT. Dual language statistical word segmentation in infancy: Simulating a language-mixing bilingual environment. Dev Sci 2020; 24:e13050. [PMID: 33063938 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2019] [Revised: 08/24/2020] [Accepted: 10/08/2020] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Infants are sensitive to syllable co-occurrence probabilities when segmenting words from fluent speech. However, segmenting two languages overlapping at the syllabic level is challenging because the statistical cues across the languages are incongruent. Successful segmentation, thus, relies on infants' ability to separate language inputs and track the statistics of each language. Here, we report three experiments investigating how infants statistically segment words from two overlapping languages in a simulated language-mixing bilingual environment. In the first two experiments, we investigated whether 9.5-month-olds can use French and English phonetic markers to segment words from two overlapping artificial languages produced by one individual. After showing that infants could segment the languages when the languages were presented in isolation (Experiment 1), we presented infants with two interleaved languages differing in phonetic cues (Experiment 2). Both monolingual and bilingual infants successfully segmented words from one of the two languages-the language heard last during familiarization. In Experiment 3, a conceptual replication, we replicated the findings of Experiment 2 with a different population and with different cues. As before, when 12-month-old monolingual infants heard two interleaved languages differing in English and Finnish phonetic cues, they learned only the last language heard during familiarization. Together, our findings suggest that segmenting words in a language-mixing environment is challenging, but infants possess a nascent ability to recruit phonetic cues to segment words from one of two overlapping languages in a bilingual-like environment. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=92pNcpxZguw.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angeline Sin Mei Tsui
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada.,Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | | | | | - Erik D Thiessen
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Gross MC, Lopez E, Buac M, Kaushanskaya M. Processing of code-switched sentences by bilingual children: Cognitive and linguistic predictors. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2019; 52. [PMID: 31885416 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100821] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Production studies of language switching have identified costs in the speed and/or accuracy of word production, but it is unclear whether processing costs are experienced by listeners as well. A related question is whether language control during comprehension recruits domain-general cognitive control. The current study examined processing of code-switching in Spanish-English bilingual children (ages 6;0-11;10) using an auditory moving window paradigm. Cognitive control was indexed by the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Children exhibited significant costs in processing speed when listening to code-switched sentences, but no costs in a measure of offline comprehension. The extent to which cognitive control skills moderated processing costs depended on the robustness of the language system: children with higher language skills exhibited a greater moderating effect of cognitive control. Taken together, the findings provide limited support for a role of cognitive control in children's code-switching processing and suggest that the processing costs incurred may be transitory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan C Gross
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Language Acquisition & Bilingualism Lab, 1500 Highland Avenue, Room 476, Madison, WI 53705, United States
| | - Eva Lopez
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Language Acquisition & Bilingualism Lab, 1500 Highland Avenue, Room 476, Madison, WI 53705, United States
| | - Milijana Buac
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Language Acquisition & Bilingualism Lab, 1500 Highland Avenue, Room 476, Madison, WI 53705, United States
| | - Margarita Kaushanskaya
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Language Acquisition & Bilingualism Lab, 1500 Highland Avenue, Room 476, Madison, WI 53705, United States
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
MORINI G, NEWMAN RS. Dónde está la ball? Examining the effect of code switching on bilingual children's word recognition. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2019; 46:1238-1248. [PMID: 31405393 PMCID: PMC7592264 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000400] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/19/2023]
Abstract
Hearing words in sentences facilitates word recognition in monolingual children. Many children grow up receiving input in multiple languages - including exposure to sentences that 'mix' the languages. We explored Spanish-English bilingual toddlers' (n = 24) ability to identify familiar words in three conditions: (i) single word (ball!); (ii) same-language sentence (Where's the ball?); or (iii) mixed-language sentence (Dónde está la ball?). Children successfully identified words across conditions; however, the advantage linked to hearing words in sentences was present only in the same-language condition. This work hence suggests that language mixing plays an important role on bilingual children's ability to recognize spoken words.
Collapse
|