1
|
The Babytwins Study Sweden (BATSS): A Multi-Method Infant Twin Study of Genetic and Environmental Factors Influencing Infant Brain and Behavioral Development. Twin Res Hum Genet 2021; 24:217-227. [PMID: 34521499 DOI: 10.1017/thg.2021.34] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
Twin studies can help us understand the relative contributions of genes and environment to phenotypic trait variation, including attentional and brain activation measures. In terms of applying methodologies such as electroencephalography (EEG) and eye tracking, which are key methods in developmental neuroscience, infant twin studies are almost nonexistent. Here, we describe the Babytwins Study Sweden (BATSS), a multi-method longitudinal twin study of 177 MZ and 134 DZ twin pairs (i.e., 622 individual infants) covering the 5-36 month time period. The study includes EEG, eye tracking and genetics, together with more traditional measures based on in-person testing, direct observation and questionnaires. The results show that interest in participation in research among twin parents is high, despite the comprehensive protocol. DNA analysis from saliva samples was possible in virtually all participants, allowing for both zygosity confirmation and polygenic score analyses. Combining a longitudinal twin design with advanced technologies in developmental cognitive neuroscience and genomics, BATSS represents a new approach in infancy research, which we hope to have impact across multiple disciplines in the coming years.
Collapse
|
2
|
The 100-plus Study of cognitively healthy centenarians: rationale, design and cohort description. Eur J Epidemiol 2018; 33:1229-1249. [PMID: 30362018 PMCID: PMC6290855 DOI: 10.1007/s10654-018-0451-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2018] [Accepted: 10/03/2018] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Although the incidence of dementia increases exponentially with age, some individuals reach more than 100 years with fully retained cognitive abilities. To identify the characteristics associated with the escape or delay of cognitive decline, we initiated the 100-plus Study (www.100plus.nl). The 100-plus Study is an on-going prospective cohort study of Dutch centenarians who self-reported to be cognitively healthy, their first-degree family members and their respective partners. We collect demographics, life history, medical history, genealogy, neuropsychological data and blood samples. Centenarians are followed annually until death. PET–MRI scans and feces donation are optional. Almost 30% of the centenarians agreed to post-mortem brain donation. To date (September 2018), 332 centenarians were included in the study. We analyzed demographic statistics of the first 300 centenarians (25% males) included in the cohort. Centenarians came from higher socio-economic classes and had higher levels of education compared to their birth cohort; alcohol consumption of centenarians was similar, and most males smoked during their lifetime. At baseline, the centenarians had a median MMSE score of 25 points (IQR 22.0–27.5); most centenarians lived independently, retained hearing and vision abilities and were independently mobile. Mortality was associated with cognitive functioning: centenarians with a baseline MMSE score ≥ 26 points had a mortality percentage of 17% per annual year in the second year after baseline, while centenarians with a baseline MMSE score < 26 points had a mortality of 42% per annual year (p = 0.003). The cohort was 2.1-fold enriched with the neuroprotective APOE-ε2 allele relative to 60–80 year-old population controls (p = 4.8 × 10−7), APOE-ε3 was unchanged and the APOE-ε4 allele was 2.3-fold depleted (p = 6.3 × 10−7). Comprehensive characterization of the 100-plus cohort of cognitively healthy centenarians might reveal protective factors that explain the physiology of long-term preserved cognitive health.
Collapse
|
3
|
Schwabe I, Boomsma D, van den Berg S. Increased environmental sensitivity in high mathematics performance. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
4
|
Barral S, Cosentino S, Costa R, Andersen SL, Christensen K, Eckfeldt JH, Newman AB, Perls TT, Province MA, Hadley EC, Rossi WK, Mayeux R. Exceptional memory performance in the Long Life Family Study. Neurobiol Aging 2013; 34:2445-8. [PMID: 23759147 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2013] [Accepted: 05/01/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Research to understand variability at the highest end of the cognitive performance distribution has been scarce. Our aim was to define a cognitive endophenotype based on exceptional episodic memory (EM) performance and to investigate familial aggregation of EM in families from the Long Life Family Study (LLFS). Using a sample of 1911 nondemented offspring of long-lived probands, we created a quantitative phenotype, EM (memory z ≥ 1.5), and classified LLFS families as EM and non-EM families based on the number of EM offspring. We then assessed differences in memory performance between LLFS relatives in the parental generation of EM families and those in non-EM families using multivariate analysis adjusted for APOE Apolipoprotein E genotype. LLFS relatives in the proband generation from EM families showed better EM performance than those from non-EM families (β = 0.74, standard error = 0.19, p = 1.4 × 10(-4)). We demonstrated that there is a familial correlation of the EM endophenotype, suggesting that genetic variants might influence memory performance in long-lived families.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra Barral
- Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University Medical Center, Department of Neurology, New York, NY 10032, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Yoon U, Perusse D, Evans AC. Mapping genetic and environmental influences on cortical surface area of pediatric twins. Neuroscience 2012; 220:169-78. [PMID: 22728098 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.06.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2012] [Revised: 06/11/2012] [Accepted: 06/13/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Cortical surface area has been largely overlooked in genetic studies of human brain morphometry, even though phylogenetic differences in cortical surface area between individuals are known to be influenced by differences in genetic endowment. In this study, we examined the relative contribution of genetic and environmental influences on cortical surface areas in both the native and stereotaxic spaces for a cohort of homogeneously-aged healthy pediatric twins. Bilateral hemispheric surface and all lobar surface areas except the occipital lobes in native space showed high heritable estimates, while the common environmental effect on bilateral occipital lobes reached statistical significance. The proportion of genetic variance for cortical surface areas measured in stereotaxic space was lower than that measured in native space, whereas the unique environmental influences increased. This is reasonable since whole brain volume is also known to be heritable itself and so removing that component of areal variance due to overall brain size via stereotaxic transformation will reduce the genetic proportion. These findings further suggest that cortical surface areas involved in cognitive, attention and emotional processing, as well as in creating and retaining of long-term memories are likely to be more useful for examining the relationship between genotype and behavioral phenotypes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- U Yoon
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Catholic University of Daegu, 13-13 Hayang-ro, Hayang-eup, Gyeongsan-si, Gyeongsangbuk-do 712-702, South Korea.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Kirkpatrick RM, McGue M, Iacono WG. Shared-environmental contributions to high cognitive ability. Behav Genet 2009; 39:406-16. [PMID: 19377874 PMCID: PMC2751599 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-009-9265-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2009] [Accepted: 03/17/2009] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Using a combined sample of adolescent twins, biological siblings, and adoptive siblings, we estimated and compared the differential shared-environmentality for high cognitive ability and the shared-environmental variance for the full range of ability during adolescence. Estimates obtained via multiple methods were in the neighborhood of 0.20, and suggest a modest effect of the shared environment on both high and full-range ability. We then examined the association of ability with three measures of the family environment in a subsample of adoptive siblings: parental occupational status, parental education, and disruptive life events. Only parental education showed significant (albeit modest) association with ability in both the biological and adoptive samples. We discuss these results in terms of the need for cognitive-development research to combine genetically sensitive designs and modern statistical methods with broad, thorough environmental measurement.
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
In this article, the author reviews the characteristics of developmentally appropriate criteria for the identification of early childhood mental health symptoms and disorders and the key components of a comprehensive, empirically based, psychiatric assessment of young children and their families. In the first section, the author discusses the infant/early childhood mental health field's perspectives on mental health and mental health problems in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. The author then provides an overview of the objections to diagnosis of psychiatric disorders in young children and different approaches to the definition of early childhood psychopathology, including descriptive, dimensional, and categorical approaches. In the second section, the author describes the six essential components of a comprehensive mental health assessment of young children: (1) multiple sessions (2) multiple informants (3) a multidisciplinary approach (4) a multicultural perspective (5) multiple modes of assessment, and (6) a multiaxial diagnostic formulation and treatment plan. The author ends with a discussion of the challenges of diagnosing and assessing mental health symptoms and disorders in children younger than 2 years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Helen Link Egger
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Center for Developmental Epidemiology, Duke University Medical Center, Box 3454, Durham, NC 27710, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Brant AM, Haberstick BC, Corley RP, Wadsworth SJ, DeFries JC, Hewitt JK. The developmental etiology of high IQ. Behav Genet 2009; 39:393-405. [PMID: 19377873 PMCID: PMC3086674 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-009-9268-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/24/2008] [Accepted: 03/27/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The genetic and environmental trends in IQ development were assessed in 483 same-sex twin pairs in the Colorado longitudinal twin study using maximum-likelihood model-fitting analysis. The twins were assessed periodically from ages 1 to 16. Results show a decreasing influence of shared environment and an increasing influence of heritability across development, with large and increasing age to age stability of genetic influences. Non-shared environment contributes almost exclusively to age to age change. Similar analyses were conducted designating the top 15% of the sample as having high IQ at each age. The developmental etiology of high IQ did not significantly differ from that found for the continuous measure in this relatively novel analysis. These results demonstrate early stability in etiological influences on IQ and have potential implications for gene-finding efforts, suggesting that samples selected for high IQ can be used to find genetic variation that will be applicable to the full range of the IQ distribution, although conclusive demonstration that the same genes are indeed involved was beyond the scope of this study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angela M Brant
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309-0447, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Petrill SA, Kovas Y, Hart SA, Thompson LA, Plomin R. The genetic and environmental etiology of high math performance in 10-year-old twins. Behav Genet 2009; 39:371-9. [PMID: 19247827 PMCID: PMC2913421 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-009-9258-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2008] [Accepted: 01/20/2009] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
The genetic and environmental etiology of high math performance (at or above the 85%tile) was examined in a population-based sample of 10-year-old twins (nMZ = 1,279, nDZ = 2,305). Math skills were assessed using a web-based battery of math performance tapping skills related to the UK National Math Curriculum. Probandwise concordance rates and liability threshold models indicated that genetic and shared environmental influences were significant, and that these estimates were generally similar to those obtained across the normal range of ability and did not vary significantly by gender. These results suggest that the genetic and environmental influences at the high end of ability are likely to be continuous with those that affect the entire range of math performance across all children irrespective of gender.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen A Petrill
- Department of Human Development and Family Science, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Haworth CM, Wright MJ, Martin NW, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, Posthuma D, Davis OS, Brant AM, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, Iacono WG, McGue M, Thompson LA, Hart SA, Petrill SA, Lubinski D, Plomin R. A twin study of the genetics of high cognitive ability selected from 11,000 twin pairs in six studies from four countries. Behav Genet 2009; 39:359-70. [PMID: 19381794 PMCID: PMC2740717 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-009-9262-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2008] [Accepted: 03/03/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Although much genetic research has addressed normal variation in intelligence, little is known about the etiology of high cognitive abilities. Using data from 11,000 twin pairs (age range = 6-71 years) from the genetics of high cognitive abilities consortium, we investigated the genetic and environmental etiologies of high general cognitive ability (g). Age-appropriate psychometric cognitive tests were administered to the twins and used to create g scores standardized within each study. Liability-threshold model fitting was used to estimate genetic and environmental parameters for the top 15% of the distribution of g. Genetic influence for high g was substantial (0.50, with a 95% confidence interval of 0.41-0.60). Shared environmental influences were moderate (0.28, 0.19-0.37). We conclude that genetic variation contributes substantially to high g in Australia, the Netherlands, the United Kingdom and the United States.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claire M.A. Haworth
- Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
| | - Margaret J. Wright
- Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Nicolas W. Martin
- Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Nicholas G. Martin
- Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Dorret I Boomsma
- Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Meike Bartels
- Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Danielle Posthuma
- Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Section Medical Genomics, VU Medical Centre Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Section Functional Genomics, Faculty Earth and Life Science, VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Oliver S.P. Davis
- Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
| | - Angela M. Brant
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder, Colorado, USA
| | - Robin P. Corley
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder, Colorado, USA
| | - John K. Hewitt
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado at Boulder, Colorado, USA
| | - William G. Iacono
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Matthew McGue
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Lee A. Thompson
- Department of Psychology, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, USA
| | - Sara A. Hart
- Human Development and Family Science, Ohio State University, Ohio, USA
| | | | - David Lubinski
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
| | - Robert Plomin
- Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Genetic Covariation Between the Author Recognition Test and Reading and Verbal Abilities: What Can We Learn from the Analysis of High Performance? Behav Genet 2009; 39:417-26. [DOI: 10.1007/s10519-009-9275-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2008] [Accepted: 04/20/2009] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
|
12
|
Salt A, Freeman K, Prusa A, Ferret N, Buffolano W, Malm G, Schmidt D, Tan HK, Gilbert RE. Determinants of response to a parent questionnaire about development and behaviour in 3 year olds: European multicentre study of congenital toxoplasmosis. BMC Pediatr 2005; 5:21. [PMID: 15998464 PMCID: PMC1190190 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2431-5-21] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2004] [Accepted: 07/05/2005] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND We aimed to determine how response to a parent-completed postal questionnaire measuring development, behaviour, impairment, and parental concerns and anxiety, varies in different European centres. METHODS Prospective cohort study of 3 year old children, with and without congenital toxoplasmosis, who were identified by prenatal or neonatal screening for toxoplasmosis in 11 centres in 7 countries. Parents were mailed a questionnaire that comprised all or part of existing validated tools. We determined the effect of characteristics of the centre and child on response, age at questionnaire completion, and response to child drawing tasks. RESULTS The questionnaire took 21 minutes to complete on average. 67% (714/1058) of parents responded. Few parents (60/1058) refused to participate. The strongest determinants of response were the score for organisational attributes of the study centre (such as direct involvement in follow up and access to an address register), and infection with congenital toxoplasmosis. Age at completion was associated with study centre, presence of neurological abnormalities in early infancy, and duration of prenatal treatment. Completion rates for individual questions exceeded 92% except for child completed drawings of a man (70%), which were completed more by girls, older children, and in certain centres. CONCLUSION Differences in response across European centres were predominantly related to the organisation of follow up and access to correct addresses. The questionnaire was acceptable in all six countries and offers a low cost tool for assessing development, behaviour, and parental concerns and anxiety, in multinational studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Salt
- The Neurodisability Service, Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children and Institute of Child Health, London, UK
| | - K Freeman
- Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, New York, U.S.A
| | - A Prusa
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Neonatology and Intensive Care, Medical University of Vienna, Austria
| | - N Ferret
- CHU de NICE, Service Parasitologie – Mycologie, Hopital L'Archet II, BP 3079, 06202 NICE Cedex 3
| | - W Buffolano
- Perinatal Infection Unit, Dept of Pediatrics, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
| | - G Malm
- Karolinska University Hospital, Huddinge, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - D Schmidt
- Department of Parasitology, Staten Seruminstitut, Copenhagen, Denmark
| | - HK Tan
- Centre for Paediatric Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Institute of Child Health, London, UK
| | - RE Gilbert
- Centre for Paediatric Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Institute of Child Health, London, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Petrill SA, Deater-Deckard K. Task orientation, parental warmth and SES account for a significant proportion of the shared environmental variance in general cognitive ability in early childhood: evidence from a twin study. Dev Sci 2004; 7:25-32. [PMID: 15323115 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00319.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Prior research suggests shared environmental influences on cognitive performance are important in early childhood. However, few studies have attempted to identify the factors comprising this shared environmental variance. To address this issue, we examined the covariance between task orientation, parental warmth, socioeconomic status and general cognitive ability in a British twin study of 125 pairs of identical and same-sex fraternal twins. Task orientation correlated r =.41 with general cognitive ability Bivariate genetic analyses suggested that this correlation was mediated by shared environmental influences. Additional analyses suggested that SES and parental warmth mediated about two-thirds of the shared environmental covariance between task engagement and cognitive skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen A Petrill
- Department of Biobehavioral Health, Center for Developmental and Health Genetics, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park 16802, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Kuo AY, Reiss AL, Freund LS, Huffman LC. Family environment and cognitive abilities in girls with fragile-X syndrome. JOURNAL OF INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY RESEARCH : JIDR 2002; 46:328-339. [PMID: 12000584 DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00396.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND There remains some variance in cognitive ability that is unexplained in children with fragile-X syndrome (FXS). Studies in typically developing children suggest that family environment might be one contributor to this unexplained variance. However, the effect of family environment in relation to cognition in atypical children with FXS has been relatively unexplored to date. METHODS The present authors examined the putative genetic and environmental factors associated with cognition in a group of age-matched children consisting of 26 females with FXS and 31 typically developing children. All subjects were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; and the subjects' parents were administered the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, and completed the Hollingshead Index of Social Status and the Moos & Moos Family Environment Scale. RESULTS Using a multiple regression analytic strategy, the present authors found that family environment contributed significantly to cognitive abilities in typically developing girls, but did not have a unique contribution to cognitive abilities in girls with FXS. There was a suggestion that, for girls with FXS, socio-economic status, a measure of sociocultural environment, was correlated with IQ. CONCLUSIONS The present study provides a basis for future research on the environmental contributions to cognitive abilities, particularly work related to verbal cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Y Kuo
- The Children's Health Council, 650 Clark Way, Palo Alto, CA 94304, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|