1
|
Piantadosi ST. The algorithmic origins of counting. Child Dev 2023; 94:1472-1490. [PMID: 37984061 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2022] [Revised: 09/16/2023] [Accepted: 09/19/2023] [Indexed: 11/22/2023]
Abstract
The study of how children learn numbers has yielded one of the most productive research programs in cognitive development, spanning empirical and computational methods, as well as nativist and empiricist philosophies. This paper provides a tutorial on how to think computationally about learning models in a domain like number, where learners take finite data and go far beyond what they directly observe or perceive. To illustrate, this paper then outlines a model which acquires a counting procedure using observations of sets and words, extending the proposal of Piantadosi et al. (2012). This new version of the model responds to several critiques of the original work and outlines an approach which is likely appropriate for acquiring further aspects of mathematics.
Collapse
|
2
|
Chen CC, Jang S, Piazza M, Hyde DC. Characterizing exact arithmetic abilities before formal schooling. Cognition 2023; 238:105481. [PMID: 37182405 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105481] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2022] [Revised: 03/07/2023] [Accepted: 05/03/2023] [Indexed: 05/16/2023]
Abstract
Children appear to have some arithmetic abilities before formal instruction in school, but the extent of these abilities as well as the mechanisms underlying them are poorly understood. Over two studies, an initial exploratory study of preschool children in the U.S. (N = 207; Age = 2.89-4.30 years) and a pre-registered replication of preschool children in Italy (N = 130; Age = 3-6.33 years), we documented some basic behavioral signatures of exact arithmetic using a non-symbolic subtraction task. Furthermore, we investigated the underlying mechanisms by analyzing the relationship between individual differences in exact subtraction and assessments of other numerical and non-numerical abilities. Across both studies, children performed above chance on the exact non-symbolic arithmetic task, generally showing better performance on problems involving smaller quantities compared to those involving larger quantities. Furthermore, individual differences in non-verbal approximate numerical abilities and exact cardinal number knowledge were related to different aspects of subtraction performance. Specifically, non-verbal approximate numerical abilities were related to subtraction performance in older but not younger children. Across both studies we found evidence that cardinal number knowledge was related to performance on subtraction problems where the answer was zero (i.e., subtractive negation problems). Moreover, subtractive negation problems were only solved above chance by children who had a basic understanding of cardinality. Together these finding suggest that core non-verbal numerical abilities, as well as emerging knowledge of symbolic numbers provide a basis for some, albeit limited, exact arithmetic abilities before formal schooling.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chi-Chuan Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA
| | - Selim Jang
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA
| | - Manuela Piazza
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CiMEC), University of Trento, Italy
| | - Daniel C Hyde
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA; Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
O’Shaughnessy DM, Cruz Cordero T, Mollica F, Boni I, Jara-Ettinger J, Gibson E, Piantadosi ST. Diverse mathematical knowledge among indigenous Amazonians. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2023; 120:e2215999120. [PMID: 37603761 PMCID: PMC10469040 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2215999120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2022] [Accepted: 05/12/2023] [Indexed: 08/23/2023] Open
Abstract
We investigate number and arithmetic learning among a Bolivian indigenous people, the Tsimane', for whom formal schooling is comparatively recent in history and variable in both extent and consistency. We first present a large-scale meta-analysis on child number development involving over 800 Tsimane' children. The results emphasize the impact of formal schooling: Children are only found to be full counters when they have attended school, suggesting the importance of cultural support for early mathematics. We then test especially remote Tsimane' communities and document the development of specialized arithmetical knowledge in the absence of direct formal education. Specifically, we describe individuals who succeed on arithmetic problems involving the number five-which has a distinct role in the local economy-even though they do not succeed on some lower numbers. Some of these participants can perform multiplication with fives at greater accuracy than addition by one. These results highlight the importance of cultural factors in early mathematics and suggest that psychological theories of number where quantities are derived from lower numbers via repeated addition (e.g., a successor function) are unlikely to explain the diversity of human mathematical ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Francis Mollica
- School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh, EdinburghEH8 9AB, United Kingdom
| | - Isabelle Boni
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA94720-1650
| | | | - Edward Gibson
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA02139-4307
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Abstract
It is popular in psychology to hypothesize that representations of exact number are innately determined-in particular, that biology has endowed humans with a system for manipulating quantities which forms the primary representational substrate for our numerical and mathematical concepts. While this perspective has been important for advancing empirical work in animal and child cognition, here we examine six natural predictions of strong numerical nativism from a multidisciplinary perspective, and find each to be at odds with evidence from anthropology and developmental science. In particular, the history of number reveals characteristics that are inconsistent with biological determinism of numerical concepts, including a lack of number systems across some human groups and remarkable variability in the form of numerical systems that do emerge. Instead, this literature highlights the importance of economic and social factors in constructing fundamentally new cognitive systems to achieve culturally specific goals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
5
|
Jacobs C, Flowers M, Aboody R, Maier M, Jara-Ettinger J. Not just what you did, but how: Children see distributors that count as more fair than distributors who don't. Cognition 2022; 225:105128. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105128] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2021] [Revised: 04/08/2022] [Accepted: 04/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
6
|
Silver AM, Libertus ME. Environmental influences on mathematics performance in early childhood. NATURE REVIEWS PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 1:407-418. [PMID: 36330081 PMCID: PMC9624502 DOI: 10.1038/s44159-022-00061-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/15/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
Math skills relate to lifelong career, health, and financial outcomes. Individuals' own cognitive abilities predict math performance and there is growing recognition that environmental influences including differences in culture and variability in math engagement also impact math skills. In this Review, we summarize evidence indicating that differences between languages, exposure to math-focused language, socioeconomic status, attitudes and beliefs about math, and engagement with math activities influence young children's math performance. These influences play out at the community and individual level. However, research on the role of these environmental influences for foundational number skills, including understanding of number words, is limited. Future research is needed to understand individual differences in the development of early emerging math skills such as number word skills, examining to what extent different types of environmental input are necessary and how children's cognitive abilities shape the impact of environmental input.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex M. Silver
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Schröder E, Gredebäck G, Forssman L, Lindskog M. Predicting children's emerging understanding of numbers. Dev Sci 2021; 25:e13207. [PMID: 34870876 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2020] [Revised: 10/26/2021] [Accepted: 11/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
How do children construct a concept of natural numbers? Past research addressing this question has mainly focused on understanding how children come to acquire the cardinality principle. However, at that point children already understand the first number words and have a rudimentary natural number concept in place. The question therefore remains; what gets children's number learning off the ground? We therefore, based on previous empirical and theoretical work, tested which factors predict the first stages of children's natural number understanding. We assessed if children's expressive vocabulary, visuospatial working memory, and ANS (Approximate number system) acuity at 18 months of age could predict their natural number knowledge at 2.5 years of age. We found that early expressive vocabulary and visuospatial working memory were important for later number knowledge. The results of the current study add to a growing body of literature showing the importance of language in children's learning about numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elin Schröder
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | | | - Linda Forssman
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Marcus Lindskog
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Gouet C, Jin W, Naiman DQ, Peña M, Halberda J. Bias and noise in proportion estimation: A mixture psychophysical model. Cognition 2021; 213:104805. [PMID: 34172265 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104805] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2020] [Revised: 06/04/2021] [Accepted: 06/05/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The importance of proportional reasoning has long been recognized by psychologists and educators, yet we still do not have a good understanding of how humans mentally represent proportions. In this paper we present a psychophysical model of proportion estimation, extending previous approaches. We assumed that proportion representations are formed by representing each magnitude of a proportion stimuli (the part and its complement) as Gaussian activations in the mind, which are then mentally combined in the form of a proportion. We next derived the internal representation of proportions, including bias and internal noise parameters -capturing respectively how our estimations depart from true values and how variable estimations are. Methodologically, we introduced a mixture of components to account for contaminating behaviors (guessing and reversal of responses) and framed the model in a hierarchical way. We found empirical support for the model by testing a group of 4th grade children in a spatial proportion estimation task. In particular, the internal density reproduced the asymmetries (skewedness) seen in this and in previous reports of estimation tasks, and the model accurately described wide variations between subjects in behavior. Bias estimates were in general smaller than by using previous approaches, due to the model's capacity to absorb contaminating behaviors. This property of the model can be of especial relevance for studies aimed at linking psychophysical measures with broader cognitive abilities. We also recovered higher levels of noise than those reported in discrimination of spatial magnitudes and discuss possible explanations for it. We conclude by illustrating a concrete application of our model to study the effects of scaling in proportional reasoning, highlighting the value of quantitative models in this field of research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camilo Gouet
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA; Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Avenida Vicuña Mackenna 4860, Macul, Santiago, Chile.
| | - Wei Jin
- Department of Applied Mathematics and Statistics, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA.
| | - Daniel Q Naiman
- Department of Applied Mathematics and Statistics, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA.
| | - Marcela Peña
- Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Avenida Vicuña Mackenna 4860, Macul, Santiago, Chile.
| | - Justin Halberda
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Jacobs C, Flowers M, Jara-Ettinger J. Children's understanding of the abstract logic of counting. Cognition 2021; 214:104790. [PMID: 34090035 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104790] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2020] [Revised: 05/20/2021] [Accepted: 05/26/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
When children learn to count, do they understand its logic independent of the number list that they learned to count with? Here we tested CP-knowers' (ages three to five) understanding of how counting reveals a set's cardinality, even when non-numerical lists are used to count. Participants watched an agent count unobservable objects in two boxes and were asked to identify the larger set. Participants successfully identified the box with more objects when the agent counted using their familiar number list (Experiment 1) and when the agent counted using a non-numeric ordered list, as long as the items in the list were not linguistically used as number words (Experiments 2-3). Additionally, children's performance was strongly influenced by visual cues that helped them link the list's order to representations of magnitude (Experiment 4). Our findings suggest that three- to six-year-olds who can count also understand how counting reveals a set's cardinality, but they require additional time to understand how symbols on any arbitrary ordered list can be used as numerals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Colin Jacobs
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, United States of America
| | - Madison Flowers
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, United States of America
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Carey S, Barner D. Ontogenetic Origins of Human Integer Representations. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:823-835. [PMID: 31439418 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2019] [Revised: 07/15/2019] [Accepted: 07/16/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Do children learn number words by associating them with perceptual magnitudes? Recent studies argue that approximate numerical magnitudes play a foundational role in the development of integer concepts. Against this, we argue that approximate number representations fail both empirically and in principle to provide the content required of integer concepts. Instead, we suggest that children's understanding of integer concepts proceeds in two phases. In the first phase, children learn small exact number word meanings by associating words with small sets. In the second phase, children learn the meanings of larger number words by mastering the logic of exact counting algorithms, which implement the successor function and Hume's principle (that one-to-one correspondence guarantees exact equality). In neither phase do approximate number representations play a foundational role.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan Carey
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.
| | - David Barner
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA; University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Braithwaite DW, Leib ER, Siegler RS, McMullen J. Individual differences in fraction arithmetic learning. Cogn Psychol 2019; 112:81-98. [PMID: 31132666 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2019] [Revised: 04/24/2019] [Accepted: 04/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Understanding fractions is critical to mathematical development, yet many children struggle with fractions even after years of instruction. Fraction arithmetic is particularly challenging. The present study employed a computational model of fraction arithmetic learning, FARRA (Fraction Arithmetic Reflects Rules and Associations; Braithwaite, Pyke, and Siegler, 2017), to investigate individual differences in children's fraction arithmetic. FARRA predicted four qualitatively distinct patterns of performance, as well as differences in math achievement among the four patterns. These predictions were confirmed in analyses of two datasets using two methods to classify children's performance-a theory-based method and a data-driven method, Latent Profile Analysis. The findings highlight three dimensions of individual differences that may affect learning in fraction arithmetic, and perhaps other domains as well: effective learning after committing errors, behavioral consistency versus variability, and presence or absence of initial bias. Methodological and educational implications of the findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David W Braithwaite
- Florida State University, Department of Psychology, 1107 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL 32306, United States.
| | - Elena R Leib
- University of California, Berkeley, Department of Psychology, 2121 Berkeley Way, Berkeley, CA 94720, United States.
| | - Robert S Siegler
- Teacher's College, Columbia University, 525 West 120th Street, New York, NY 10027, United States; The Siegler Center for Innovative Learning (SCIL), Advanced Technology Center, Beijing Normal University, China.
| | - Jake McMullen
- University of Turku, Department of Teacher Education, FIN-20014 Turun Yliopisto, Finland.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Gibson DJ, Gunderson EA, Spaepen E, Levine SC, Goldin-Meadow S. Number gestures predict learning of number words. Dev Sci 2019; 22:e12791. [PMID: 30566755 PMCID: PMC6470030 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12791] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2016] [Revised: 11/14/2018] [Accepted: 11/14/2018] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
When asked to explain their solutions to a problem, children often gesture and, at times, these gestures convey information that is different from the information conveyed in speech. Children who produce these gesture-speech "mismatches" on a particular task have been found to profit from instruction on that task. We have recently found that some children produce gesture-speech mismatches when identifying numbers at the cusp of their knowledge, for example, a child incorrectly labels a set of two objects with the word "three" and simultaneously holds up two fingers. These mismatches differ from previously studied mismatches (where the information conveyed in gesture has the potential to be integrated with the information conveyed in speech) in that the gestured response contradicts the spoken response. Here, we ask whether these contradictory number mismatches predict which learners will profit from number-word instruction. We used the Give-a-Number task to measure number knowledge in 47 children (Mage = 4.1 years, SD = 0.58), and used the What's on this Card task to assess whether children produced gesture-speech mismatches above their knower level. Children who were early in their number learning trajectories ("one-knowers" and "two-knowers") were then randomly assigned, within knower level, to one of two training conditions: a Counting condition in which children practiced counting objects; or an Enriched Number Talk condition containing counting, labeling set sizes, spatial alignment of neighboring sets, and comparison of these sets. Controlling for counting ability, we found that children were more likely to learn the meaning of new number words in the Enriched Number Talk condition than in the Counting condition, but only if they had produced gesture-speech mismatches at pretest. The findings suggest that numerical gesture-speech mismatches are a reliable signal that a child is ready to profit from rich number instruction and provide evidence, for the first time, that cardinal number gestures have a role to play in number-learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dominic J Gibson
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
| | | | - Elizabet Spaepen
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
| | - Susan C Levine
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Opfer J, Kim D, Young CJ, Marciani F. Linear Spatial-Numeric Associations Aid Memory for Single Numbers. Front Psychol 2019; 10:146. [PMID: 30778318 PMCID: PMC6369359 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00146] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2017] [Accepted: 01/16/2019] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Memory for numbers improves with age. One source of this improvement may be learning linear spatial–numeric associations, but previous evidence for this hypothesis likely confounded memory span with quality of numerical magnitude representations and failed to distinguish spatial–numeric mappings from other numeric abilities, such as counting or number word-cardinality mapping. To obviate the influence of memory span on numerical memory, we examined 39 3- to 5-year-olds’ ability to recall one spontaneously produced number (1–20) after a delay, and the relation between numeric recall (controlling for non-numeric recall) and quality of mapping between symbolic and non-symbolic quantities using number-line estimation, give-a-number estimation, and counting tasks. Consistent with previous reports, mapping of numerals to space, to discrete quantities, and to numbers in memory displayed a logarithmic-to-linear shift. Also, linearity of spatial–numeric mapping correlated strongly with multiple measures of numeric recall (percent correct and percent absolute error), even when controlling for age and non-numeric memory. Results suggest that linear spatial–numeric mappings may aid memory for number over and above children’s other numeric skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John Opfer
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
| | - Dan Kim
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Slusser E, Ribner A, Shusterman A. Language
counts
: Early language mediates the relationship between parent education and children's math ability. Dev Sci 2018; 22:e12773. [DOI: 10.1111/desc.12773] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2016] [Accepted: 10/19/2018] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Slusser
- Department of Child & Adolescent Development San Jose State University San Jose California USA
| | - Andrew Ribner
- Department of Applied Psychology New York University New York New York USA
| | - Anna Shusterman
- Department of Psychology Wesleyan University Middletown Connecticut USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Wagner K, Chu J, Barner D. Do children's number words begin noisy? Dev Sci 2018; 22:e12752. [PMID: 30230138 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12752] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2017] [Accepted: 07/25/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
How do children acquire exact meanings for number words like three or forty-seven? In recent years, a lively debate has probed the cognitive systems that support learning, with some arguing that an evolutionarily ancient "approximate number system" drives early number word meanings, and others arguing that learning is supported chiefly by representations of small sets of discrete individuals. This debate has centered around the findings generated by Wynn's (, ) Give-a-Number task, which she used to categorize children into discrete "knower level" stages. Early reports confirmed Wynn's analysis, and took these stages to support the "small sets" hypothesis. However, more recent studies have disputed this analysis, and have argued that Give-a-Number data reveal a strong role for approximate number representations. In the present study, we use previously collected Give-a-Number data to replicate the analyses of these past studies, and to show that differences between past studies are due to assumptions made in analyses, rather than to differences in data themselves. We also show how Give-a-Number data violate the assumptions of parametric tests used in past studies. Based on simple non-parametric tests and model simulations, we conclude that (a) before children learn exact meanings for words like one, two, three, and four, they first acquire noisy preliminary meanings for these words, (b) there is no reliable evidence of preliminary meanings for larger meanings, and (c) Give-a-Number cannot be used to readily identify signatures of the approximate number system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Junyi Chu
- MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Cambridge, Massachussetts
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
What counts in preschool number knowledge? A Bayes factor analytic approach toward theoretical model development. J Exp Child Psychol 2017; 166:116-133. [PMID: 28888192 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2016] [Revised: 05/29/2017] [Accepted: 07/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Preschool children vary tremendously in their numerical knowledge, and these individual differences strongly predict later mathematics achievement. To better understand the sources of these individual differences, we measured a variety of cognitive and linguistic abilities motivated by previous literature to be important and then analyzed which combination of these variables best explained individual differences in actual number knowledge. Through various data-driven Bayesian model comparison and selection strategies on competing multiple regression models, our analyses identified five variables of unique importance to explaining individual differences in preschool children's symbolic number knowledge: knowledge of the count list, nonverbal approximate numerical ability, working memory, executive conflict processing, and knowledge of letters and words. Furthermore, our analyses revealed that knowledge of the count list, likely a proxy for explicit practice or experience with numbers, and nonverbal approximate numerical ability were much more important to explaining individual differences in number knowledge than general cognitive and language abilities. These findings suggest that children use a diverse set of number-specific, general cognitive, and language abilities to learn about symbolic numbers, but the contribution of number-specific abilities may overshadow that of more general cognitive abilities in the learning process.
Collapse
|
17
|
Sella F, Berteletti I, Lucangeli D, Zorzi M. Preschool children use space, rather than counting, to infer the numerical magnitude of digits: Evidence for a spatial mapping principle. Cognition 2017; 158:56-67. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2015] [Revised: 10/06/2016] [Accepted: 10/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
18
|
|
19
|
Jara-Ettinger J, Piantadosi S, Spelke ES, Levy R, Gibson E. Mastery of the logic of natural numbers is not the result of mastery of counting: evidence from late counters. Dev Sci 2016; 20. [PMID: 27546566 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12459] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2015] [Accepted: 04/26/2016] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
To master the natural number system, children must understand both the concepts that number words capture and the counting procedure by which they are applied. These two types of knowledge develop in childhood, but their connection is poorly understood. Here we explore the relationship between the mastery of counting and the mastery of exact numerical equality (one central aspect of natural number) in the Tsimane', a farming-foraging group whose children master counting at a delayed age and with higher variability than do children in industrialized societies. By taking advantage of this variation, we can better understand how counting and exact equality relate to each other, while controlling for age and education. We find that the Tsimane' come to understand exact equality at later and variable ages. This understanding correlates with their mastery of number words and counting, controlling for age and education. However, some children who have mastered counting lack an understanding of exact equality, and some children who have not mastered counting have achieved this understanding. These results suggest that understanding of counting and of natural number concepts are at least partially distinct achievements, and that both draw on inputs and resources whose distribution and availability differ across cultures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julian Jara-Ettinger
- Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA
| | - Steve Piantadosi
- Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, USA
| | | | - Roger Levy
- Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA
| | - Edward Gibson
- Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Number-specific and general cognitive markers of preschoolers’ math ability profiles. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 147:1-21. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2015] [Revised: 01/26/2016] [Accepted: 02/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
21
|
Wagner K, Kimura K, Cheung P, Barner D. Why is number word learning hard? Evidence from bilingual learners. Cogn Psychol 2015; 83:1-21. [PMID: 26413888 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2015] [Revised: 08/11/2015] [Accepted: 08/24/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Young children typically take between 18 months and 2 years to learn the meanings of number words. In the present study, we investigated this developmental trajectory in bilingual preschoolers to examine the relative contributions of two factors in number word learning: (1) the construction of numerical concepts, and (2) the mapping of language specific words onto these concepts. We found that children learn the meanings of small number words (i.e., one, two, and three) independently in each language, indicating that observed delays in learning these words are attributable to difficulties in mapping words to concepts. In contrast, children generally learned to accurately count larger sets (i.e., five or greater) simultaneously in their two languages, suggesting that the difficulty in learning to count is not tied to a specific language. We also replicated previous studies that found that children learn the counting procedure before they learn its logic - i.e., that for any natural number, n, the successor of n in the count list denotes the cardinality n+1. Consistent with past studies, we found that children's knowledge of successors is first acquired incrementally. In bilinguals, we found that this knowledge exhibits item-specific transfer between languages, suggesting that the logic of the positive integers may not be stored in a language-specific format. We conclude that delays in learning the meanings of small number words are mainly due to language-specific processes of mapping words to concepts, whereas the logic and procedures of counting appear to be learned in a format that is independent of a particular language and thus transfers rapidly from one language to the other in development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katie Wagner
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, United States.
| | - Katherine Kimura
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, United States; Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, United States
| | - Pierina Cheung
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, United States; Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Canada
| | - David Barner
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, United States; Department of Linguistics, University of California, San Diego, United States
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
Many studies rely on estimation of Weber ratios (W) in order to quantify the acuity an individual's approximate number system. This paper discusses several problems encountered in estimating W using the standard methods, most notably low power and inefficiency. Through simulation, this work shows that W can best be estimated in a Bayesian framework that uses an inverse (1/W) prior. This beneficially balances a bias/variance trade-off and, when used with MAP estimation is extremely simple to implement. Use of this scheme substantially improves statistical power in examining correlates of W.
Collapse
|
23
|
Piantadosi ST, Kidd C, Aslin R. Rich analysis and rational models: inferring individual behavior from infant looking data. Dev Sci 2014; 17:321-37. [PMID: 24750256 PMCID: PMC3996510 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12083] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2012] [Accepted: 11/29/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Studies of infant looking times over the past 50 years have provided profound insights about cognitive development, but their dependent measures and analytic techniques are quite limited. In the context of infants' attention to discrete sequential events, we show how a Bayesian data analysis approach can be combined with a rational cognitive model to create a rich data analysis framework for infant looking times. We formalize (i) a statistical learning model, (ii) a parametric linking between the learning model's beliefs and infants' looking behavior, and (iii) a data analysis approach and model that infers parameters of the cognitive model and linking function for groups and individuals. Using this approach, we show that recent findings from Kidd, Piantadosi and Aslin (iv) of a U-shaped relationship between look-away probability and stimulus complexity even holds within infants and is not due to averaging subjects with different types of behavior. Our results indicate that individual infants prefer stimuli of intermediate complexity, reserving attention for events that are moderately predictable given their probabilistic expectations about the world.
Collapse
|
24
|
Piantadosi ST, Jara-Ettinger J, Gibson E. Children's learning of number words in an indigenous farming-foraging group. Dev Sci 2014; 17:553-63. [PMID: 24766463 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2012] [Accepted: 02/26/2013] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
We show that children in the Tsimane', a farming-foraging group in the Bolivian rain-forest, learn number words along a similar developmental trajectory to children from industrialized countries. Tsimane' children successively acquire the first three or four number words before fully learning how counting works. However, their learning is substantially delayed relative to children from the United States, Russia, and Japan. The presence of a similar developmental trajectory likely indicates that the incremental stages of numerical knowledge - but not their timing - reflect a fundamental property of number concept acquisition which is relatively independent of language, culture, age, and early education.
Collapse
|
25
|
Sarnecka BW, Wright CE. The idea of an exact number: children's understanding of cardinality and equinumerosity. Cogn Sci 2013; 37:1493-506. [PMID: 23672476 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2010] [Revised: 10/16/2012] [Accepted: 10/17/2012] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Understanding what numbers are means knowing several things. It means knowing how counting relates to numbers (called the cardinal principle or cardinality); it means knowing that each number is generated by adding one to the previous number (called the successor function or succession), and it means knowing that all and only sets whose members can be placed in one-to-one correspondence have the same number of items (called exact equality or equinumerosity). A previous study (Sarnecka & Carey, 2008) linked children's understanding of cardinality to their understanding of succession for the numbers five and six. This study investigates the link between cardinality and equinumerosity for these numbers, finding that children either understand both cardinality and equinumerosity or they understand neither. This suggests that cardinality and equinumerosity (along with succession) are interrelated facets of the concepts five and six, the acquisition of which is an important conceptual achievement of early childhood.
Collapse
|
26
|
Sarnecka BW, Negen J. A number of options: rationalist, constructivist, and Bayesian insights into the development of exact-number concepts. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2012. [PMID: 23205414 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-397919-3.00009-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The question of how human beings acquire exact-number concepts has interested cognitive developmentalists since the time of Piaget. The answer will owe something to both the rationalist and constructivist traditions. On the one hand, some aspects of numerical cognition (e.g. approximate number estimation and the ability to track small sets of one to four individuals) are innate or early-developing and are shared widely among species. On the other hand, only humans create representations of exact, large numbers such as 42, as distinct from both 41 and 43. These representations seem to be constructed slowly, over a period of months or years during early childhood. The task for researchers is to distinguish the innate representational resources from those that are constructed, and to characterize the construction process. Bayesian approaches can be useful to this project in at least three ways: (1) As a way to analyze data, which may have distinct advantages over more traditional methods (e.g. making it possible to find support for a nuli hypothesis); (2) as a way of modeling children's performance on specific tasks: Peculiarities of the task are captured as a prior; the child's knowledge is captured in the way the prior is updated; and behavior is captured as a posterior distribution; and (3) as a way of modeling learning itself, by providing a formal account of how learners might choose among alternative hypotheses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara W Sarnecka
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-5100, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Yurovsky D, Hidaka S, Wu R. Quantitative linking hypotheses for infant eye movements. PLoS One 2012; 7:e47419. [PMID: 23110071 PMCID: PMC3482231 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047419] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2012] [Accepted: 09/13/2012] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The study of cognitive development hinges, largely, on the analysis of infant looking. But analyses of eye gaze data require the adoption of linking hypotheses: assumptions about the relationship between observed eye movements and underlying cognitive processes. We develop a general framework for constructing, testing, and comparing these hypotheses, and thus for producing new insights into early cognitive development. We first introduce the general framework--applicable to any infant gaze experiment--and then demonstrate its utility by analyzing data from a set of experiments investigating the role of attentional cues in infant learning. The new analysis uncovers significantly more structure in these data, finding evidence of learning that was not found in standard analyses and showing an unexpected relationship between cue use and learning rate. Finally, we discuss general implications for the construction and testing of quantitative linking hypotheses. MATLAB code for sample linking hypotheses can be found on the first author's website.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Yurovsky
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
Number-knower levels are a series of stages of number concept development in early childhood. A child's number-knower level is typically assessed using the give-N task. Although the task procedure has been highly refined, the standard ways of analyzing give-N data remain somewhat crude. Lee and Sarnecka (Cogn Sci 34:51-67, 2010, in press) have developed a Bayesian model of children's performance on the give-N task that allows knower level to be inferred in a more principled way. However, this model requires considerable expertise and computational effort to implement and apply to data. Here, we present an approximation to the model's inference that can be computed with Microsoft Excel. We demonstrate the accuracy of the approximation and provide instructions for its use. This makes the powerful inferential capabilities of the Bayesian model accessible to developmental researchers interested in estimating knower levels from give-N data.
Collapse
|
29
|
Abstract
How is number-concept acquisition related to overall language development? Experiments 1 and 2 measured number-word knowledge and general vocabulary in a total of 59 children, ages 30-60 months. A strong correlation was found between number-word knowledge and vocabulary, independent of the child's age, contrary to previous results (D. Ansari et al., 2003). This result calls into question arguments that (a) the number-concept creation process is scaffolded mainly by visuo-spatial development and (b) that language only becomes integrated after the concepts are created (D. Ansari et al., 2003). Instead, this may suggest that having a larger nominal vocabulary helps children learn number words. Experiment 3 shows that the differences with previous results are likely due to changes in how the data were analyzed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James Negen
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92617-5100, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Piantadosi ST, Tenenbaum JB, Goodman ND. Bootstrapping in a language of thought: a formal model of numerical concept learning. Cognition 2012; 123:199-217. [PMID: 22284806 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2010] [Revised: 11/02/2011] [Accepted: 11/11/2011] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
In acquiring number words, children exhibit a qualitative leap in which they transition from understanding a few number words, to possessing a rich system of interrelated numerical concepts. We present a computational framework for understanding this inductive leap as the consequence of statistical inference over a sufficiently powerful representational system. We provide an implemented model that is powerful enough to learn number word meanings and other related conceptual systems from naturalistic data. The model shows that bootstrapping can be made computationally and philosophically well-founded as a theory of number learning. Our approach demonstrates how learners may combine core cognitive operations to build sophisticated representations during the course of development, and how this process explains observed developmental patterns in number word learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven T Piantadosi
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, United States.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Slusser EB, Sarnecka BW. Find the picture of eight turtles: a link between children's counting and their knowledge of number word semantics. J Exp Child Psychol 2011; 110:38-51. [PMID: 21524422 PMCID: PMC3105118 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2010] [Revised: 02/21/2011] [Accepted: 03/11/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
An essential part of understanding number words (e.g., eight) is understanding that all number words refer to the dimension of experience we call numerosity. Knowledge of this general principle may be separable from knowledge of individual number word meanings. That is, children may learn the meanings of at least a few individual number words before realizing that all number words refer to numerosity. Alternatively, knowledge of this general principle may form relatively early and proceed to guide and constrain the acquisition of individual number word meanings. The current article describes two experiments in which 116 children (2½- to 4-year-olds) were given a Word Extension task as well as a standard Give-N task. Results show that only children who understood the cardinality principle of counting successfully extended number words from one set to another based on numerosity-with evidence that a developing understanding of this concept emerges as children approach the cardinality principle induction. These findings support the view that children do not use a broad understanding of number words to initially connect number words to numerosity but rather make this connection around the time that they figure out the cardinality principle of counting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily B Slusser
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT 06457, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Abstract
AbstractJones & Love (J&L) should have given more attention to Agnostic uses of Bayesian methods for the statistical analysis of models and data. Reliance on the frequentist analysis of Bayesian models has retarded their development and prevented their full evaluation. The Ecumenical integration of Bayesian statistics to analyze Bayesian models offers a better way to test their inferential and predictive capabilities.
Collapse
|
33
|
Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition. Behav Brain Sci 2011; 34:169-88; disuccsion 188-231. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x10003134] [Citation(s) in RCA: 272] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
AbstractThe prominence of Bayesian modeling of cognition has increased recently largely because of mathematical advances in specifying and deriving predictions from complex probabilistic models. Much of this research aims to demonstrate that cognitive behavior can be explained from rational principles alone, without recourse to psychological or neurological processes and representations. We note commonalities between this rational approach and other movements in psychology – namely, Behaviorism and evolutionary psychology – that set aside mechanistic explanations or make use of optimality assumptions. Through these comparisons, we identify a number of challenges that limit the rational program's potential contribution to psychological theory. Specifically, rational Bayesian models are significantly unconstrained, both because they are uninformed by a wide range of process-level data and because their assumptions about the environment are generally not grounded in empirical measurement. The psychological implications of most Bayesian models are also unclear. Bayesian inference itself is conceptually trivial, but strong assumptions are often embedded in the hypothesis sets and the approximation algorithms used to derive model predictions, without a clear delineation between psychological commitments and implementational details. Comparing multiple Bayesian models of the same task is rare, as is the realization that many Bayesian models recapitulate existing (mechanistic level) theories. Despite the expressive power of current Bayesian models, we argue they must be developed in conjunction with mechanistic considerations to offer substantive explanations of cognition. We lay out several means for such an integration, which take into account the representations on which Bayesian inference operates, as well as the algorithms and heuristics that carry it out. We argue this unification will better facilitate lasting contributions to psychological theory, avoiding the pitfalls that have plagued previous theoretical movements.
Collapse
|
34
|
Abstract
In The Price Is Right game show, players compete to win a prize, by placing bids on its price. We ask whether it is possible to achieve a "wisdom of the crowd" effect, by combining the bids to produce an aggregate price estimate that is superior to the estimates of individual players. Using data from the game show, we show that a wisdom of the crowd effect is possible, especially by using models of the decision-making processes involved in bidding. The key insight is that, because of the competitive nature of the game, what people bid is not necessarily the same as what they know. This means better estimates are formed by aggregating latent knowledge than by aggregating observed bids. We use our results to highlight the usefulness of models of cognition and decision-making in studying the wisdom of the crowd, which are often approached only from non-psychological statistical perspectives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael D Lee
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697-5100, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Number-knower levels in young children: insights from Bayesian modeling. Cognition 2010; 120:391-402. [PMID: 21109239 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2010] [Revised: 09/27/2010] [Accepted: 10/11/2010] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Lee and Sarnecka (2010) developed a Bayesian model of young children's behavior on the Give-N test of number knowledge. This paper presents two new extensions of the model, and applies the model to new data. In the first extension, the model is used to evaluate competing theories about the conceptual knowledge underlying children's behavior. One, the knower-levels theory, is basically a "stage" theory involving real conceptual change. The other, the approximate-meanings theory, assumes that the child's conceptual knowledge is relatively constant, although performance improves over time. In the second extension, the model is used to ask whether the same latent psychological variable (a child's number-knower level) can simultaneously account for behavior on two tasks (the Give-N task and the Fast-Cards task) with different performance demands. Together, these two demonstrations show the potential of the Bayesian modeling approach to improve our understanding of the development of human cognition.
Collapse
|
36
|
Kruschke JK. What to believe: Bayesian methods for data analysis. Trends Cogn Sci 2010; 14:293-300. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 152] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2009] [Revised: 05/01/2010] [Accepted: 05/04/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
37
|
Emergent and structured cognition in Bayesian models: comment on Griffiths et al. and McClelland et al. Trends Cogn Sci 2010; 14:345-6. [PMID: 20561813 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2010] [Accepted: 05/27/2010] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
38
|
Sarnecka BW, Lee MD. Levels of number knowledge during early childhood. J Exp Child Psychol 2009; 103:325-37. [PMID: 19345956 PMCID: PMC3127737 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2008] [Revised: 02/06/2009] [Accepted: 02/27/2009] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Researchers have long disagreed about whether number concepts are essentially continuous (unchanging) or discontinuous over development. Among those who take the discontinuity position, there is disagreement about how development proceeds. The current study addressed these questions with new quantitative analyses of children's incorrect responses on the Give-N task. Using data from 280 children, ages 2 to 4 years, this study showed that most wrong answers were simply guesses, not counting or estimation errors. Their mean was unrelated to the target number, and they were lower-bounded by the numbers children actually knew. In addition, children learned the number-word meanings one at a time and in order; they treated the number words as mutually exclusive; and once they figured out the cardinal principle of counting, they generalized this principle to the rest of their count list. Findings support the 'discontinuity' account of number development in general and the 'knower-levels' account in particular.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara W Sarnecka
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|