1
|
Dizdarević D, Leskovar R, Vukovič G. Primus inter pares effect in high schools. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1382062. [PMID: 39463457 PMCID: PMC11502392 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1382062] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2024] [Accepted: 09/24/2024] [Indexed: 10/29/2024] Open
Abstract
Primus inter pares effect or better-than-average effect is cognitive bias known as illusory superiority in which individuals overestimate their positive abilities and traits in comparison to others. Overestimation and bias are often accompanied with various dangers on a personal, organizational or even societal level. We investigated the presence of overestimation among high school graduates in Slovenia in areas of teamwork, interpersonal skills, emotional intelligence, problem solving, and decision making. Although overestimation was present in all areas, results have also shown indications of indecisiveness. Overestimation was highest in the area of interpersonal skills, which is more of a social skill area in comparison with decision making or problem solving. Individuals probably receive more feedback over the course of high school in decision making, problem solving, and teamwork than in interpersonal skills, as those can directly impact grades while interpersonal skills less so.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Denis Dizdarević
- Faculty of Organizational Sciences, University of Maribor, Maribor, Slovenia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Tuckwell GA, Gupta CC, Vincent GE, Vandelanotte C, Duncan MJ, Ferguson SA. Calibrated to drive: Measuring self-assessed driving ability and perceived workload after prolonged sitting and sleep restriction. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2024; 202:107609. [PMID: 38701560 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2024.107609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2023] [Revised: 04/08/2024] [Accepted: 04/28/2024] [Indexed: 05/05/2024]
Abstract
Self-assessed driving ability may differ from actual driving performance, leading to poor calibration (i.e., differences between self-assessed driving ability and actual performance), increased risk of accidents and unsafe driving behaviour. Factors such as sleep restriction and sedentary behaviour can impact driver workload, which influences driver calibration. This study aims to investigate how sleep restriction and prolonged sitting impact driver workload and driver calibration to identify strategies that can lead to safer and better calibrated drivers. Participants (n = 84, mean age = 23.5 ± 4.8, 49 % female) undertook a 7-day laboratory study and were randomly allocated to a condition: sitting 9-h sleep opportunity (Sit9), breaking up sitting 9-h sleep opportunity (Break9), sitting 5-h sleep opportunity (Sit5) and breaking up sitting 5-h sleep opportunity (Break5). Break9 and Break5 conditions completed 3-min of light-intensity walking on a treadmill every 30 min between 09:00-17:00 h, while participants in Sit9 and Sit5 conditions remained seated. Each participant completed a 20-min simulated commute in the morning and afternoon each day and completed subjective assessments of driving ability and perceived workload before and after each commute. Objective driving performance was assessed using a driving simulator measuring speed and lane performance metrics. Driver calibration was analysed using a single component and 3-component Brier Score. Correlational matrices were conducted as an exploratory analysis to understand the strength and direction of the relationship between subjective and objective driving outcomes. Analyses revealed participants in Sit9 and Break9 were significantly better calibrated for lane variability, lane position and safe zone-lane parameters at both time points (p < 0.0001) compared to Sit5 and Break5. Break5 participants were better calibrated for safe zone-speed and combined safe zone parameters (p < 0.0001) and speed variability at both time points (p = 0.005) compared to all other conditions. Analyses revealed lower perceived workload scores at both time points for Sit9 and Break9 participants compared to Sit5 and Break5 (p = <0.001). Breaking up sitting during the day may reduce calibration errors compared to sitting during the day for speed keeping parameters. Future studies should investigate if different physical activity frequency and intensity can reduce calibration errors, and better align a driver's self-assessment with their actual performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Georgia A Tuckwell
- Central Queensland University, Appleton Institute, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Adelaide, Australia.
| | - Charlotte C Gupta
- Central Queensland University, Appleton Institute, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Adelaide, Australia
| | - Grace E Vincent
- Central Queensland University, Appleton Institute, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Adelaide, Australia
| | - Corneel Vandelanotte
- Central Queensland University, Appleton Institute, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Adelaide, Australia
| | - Mitch J Duncan
- The University of Newcastle, School of Medicine & Public Health, Callaghan, Australia; Active Living Research Program, Hunter Medical Research Institute, New Lambton Heights, NSW 2305, Australia
| | - Sally A Ferguson
- Central Queensland University, Appleton Institute, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, Adelaide, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sun L, Hu WJ, Cheng L, Zhang CL. Effects of hazard type and confidence level on hazard perception in young male drivers: an ERP study. Neuroreport 2024; 35:299-305. [PMID: 38305147 DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000002007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2024]
Abstract
Hazard perception refers to a driver's ability to identify potential hazards on the road. Although studies have shown that overconfidence affects drivers' ability to perceive hazards, the neural processing of hazard perception in overconfident drivers under different hazard types has rarely been investigated. A mixed experimental design of 2 (hazard type: hazards, no hazards) × 2 (driver group: overconfident driver, control group) was used. A total of 120 images (60 with hazards and 60 without hazards) were presented to 25 overconfident drivers and 25 drivers in the control group. Reaction time, response accuracy, P200 (200-250 ms), and N2 (280-330 ms) components were measured via event-related potential (ERP) technology. Response times of overconfident drivers are slower than those of the control group regardless of hazard types. The P200 latency of overconfident drivers was longer than that of their peers in hazard situations, while the P200 latency was similar between the two groups in no-hazard situations. Although a significant interaction was found on the N2 amplitude, no significant differences were found between the two groups in both hazard and no-hazard situations. Overconfident drivers have worse hazard perception performance in hazard situations with slower reaction times and greater P200 latencies than their peers. More importantly, P200 amplitude and N2 amplitude are sensitive to hazard types, while P200 amplitude and N2 latency can discriminate between driver groups. The findings help understand the negative impact of overconfidence on young drivers and contribute to the development and training of hazard perception tests.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Long Sun
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian
| | - Wen-Jing Hu
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian
| | - Liang Cheng
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian
| | - Chang-Lu Zhang
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Zhaoyang H, Feng C, Mei F, Jingjing L, Jiyang P. Negative perfectionism and sleep quality in Chinese international students under COVID-19 epidemic: A moderated mediation. Front Psychol 2022; 13:937816. [PMID: 35978773 PMCID: PMC9377220 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.937816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2022] [Accepted: 07/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective This study used a moderated mediation model to test the mediating effect of anxiety on the relationship between negative perfectionism and sleep quality and the moderating effect of COVID-19 epidemic risk perception during the COVID-19 pandemic in Chinese international students. Materials and methods A sample of 239 Chinese international students from the south of China, was surveyed with the Negative and Positive Perfectionism Scale, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, the General Anxiety Disorder Scale, and the COVID-19 Epidemic Risk Perception Inventory. Version 23.0 of SPSS and version 3.4 of PROCESS were used to perform the correlation analyses, mediation analysis, and moderated mediation analysis. Results (1) Negative perfectionism was significantly correlated with anxiety (r = 0.371, p < 0.01) and poor sleep quality (r = 0.291, p < 0.01). Anxiety was significantly correlated with poor sleep quality (r = 0.594, p < 0.01). (2) The mediating effect test showed that anxiety had a mediating effect between negative perfectionism and poor sleep quality (β = 0.157, p < 0.01). (3) Epidemic risk perception moderated the mediating effect of anxiety between negative perfectionism and poor sleep quality (β = 0.070, p < 0.01). Conclusion Negative perfectionism affected sleep quality indirectly through anxiety. In particular, COVID-19 epidemic risk perception moderated the relationship between anxiety and sleep quality, such that the association was stronger when the COVID-19 epidemic risk perception was high. These results provide a more comprehensive understanding of the negative link between negative perfectionism and poor sleep quality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Huang Zhaoyang
- Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
- School of Management, Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Chen Feng
- Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Fan Mei
- Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Lin Jingjing
- Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Pan Jiyang
- Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
- *Correspondence: Pan Jiyang,
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Unverricht J, Yamani Y, Chen J, Horrey WJ. Minding the Gap: Effects of an Attention Maintenance Training Program on Driver Calibration. HUMAN FACTORS 2022; 64:890-903. [PMID: 33054386 DOI: 10.1177/0018720820965293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The present study examines the effect of an existing driver training program, FOrward Concentration and Attention Learning (FOCAL) on young drivers' calibration, drivers' ability to estimate the length of their in-vehicle glances while driving, using two different measures, normalized difference scores and Brier Scores. BACKGROUND Young drivers are poor at maintaining attention to the forward roadway while driving a vehicle. Additionally, drivers may overestimate their attention maintenance abilities. Driver training programs such as FOCAL may train target skills such as attention maintenance but also might serve as a promising way to reduce errors in drivers' calibration of their self-perceived attention maintenance behaviors in comparison to their actual performance. METHOD Thirty-six participants completed either FOCAL or a Placebo training program, immediately followed by driving simulator evaluations of their attention maintenance performance. In the evaluation drive, participants navigated four driving simulator scenarios during which their eyes were tracked. In each scenario, participants performed a map task on a tablet simulating an in-vehicle infotainment system. RESULTS FOCAL-trained drivers maintained their attention to the forward roadway more and reported better calibration using the normalized difference measure than Placebo-trained drivers. However, the Brier scores did not distinguish the two groups on their calibration. CONCLUSION The study implies that FOCAL has the potential to improve not only attention maintenance skills but also calibration of the skills for young drivers. APPLICATION Driver training programs may be designed to train not only targeted higher cognitive skills but also driver calibration-both critical for driving safety in young drivers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Yusuke Yamani
- 6042 Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia, USA
| | - Jing Chen
- 6042 Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
A Comprehensive Review of Driving Style Evaluation Approaches and Product Designs Applied to Vehicle Usage-Based Insurance. SUSTAINABILITY 2022. [DOI: 10.3390/su14137705] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Vehicle insurance is a very important source of income for insurance companies, and it is closely related to the driving style performed by driving behavior. Different driving styles can better reflect the driving risk than the number of violations, claims, and other static statistic data. Subdivide the vehicle insurance market according to the personal characteristics and driving habits of the insured vehicles, and studying the personalized vehicle insurance products, will help the insurance companies to improve their income, help the drivers to change their bad driving habits, and thus help to realize the healthy development of the vehicle insurance industry. In the past 20 to 30 years, more and more insurance companies around the world have launched vehicle usage-based insurance (UBI) products based on driving style analysis. However, up to now, there are few comprehensive reports on commercial vehicle UBI products and their core driving risk assessment methods. On the basis of literature indexing on the Web of Science and other academic platforms by using the keywords involved in vehicle UBI, over 100 relevant works of literature were screened in this paper, and a detailed and comprehensive discussion on the driving style evaluation methods and the design of commercial vehicle UBI products during the past 20 to 30 years has been made, hoping to get a full understanding of the possible factors affecting driving style and the collectible data that can reflect these factors, and to get a full grasp of the developing status, challenges and future trends in vehicle insurance branch of the Internet of Vehicles (IoV) industry.
Collapse
|
7
|
Prohn MJ, Herbig B. Traffic safety knowledge gain of ambulance drivers after simulator-based training. BMC MEDICAL EDUCATION 2022; 22:216. [PMID: 35354466 PMCID: PMC8969364 DOI: 10.1186/s12909-022-03279-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2021] [Accepted: 03/21/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Compared to other road users, ambulance drivers are at a higher accident risk while driving with warning lights and sirens. No standard exists for training or education for emergency medical service employees driving ambulances. Training programs should positively influence knowledge. However, knowledge gain can be influenced by several different factors. This study developed a knowledge test for ambulance drivers to determine influencing factors on knowledge and its gain by simulator-based training. METHODS Two parallel knowledge test forms with 20 questions each were designed in several steps and tested on up to 174 participants. Questionnaires were used to study associated and influencing factors, such as objective experience, subjective attitudes, personality, motivation and demographic data. RESULTS Test construction showed good overall parallelism of the two tests as well as reliability and sensitivity. There was no correlation between subjective and objective knowledge gain, but participants with higher subjective knowledge gain showed a higher variation in objective knowledge. Younger age, higher qualification, higher number of license classes, fewer traffic violations, and more traffic safety trainings were positively associated with knowledge, whereas less yearly driving mileage, more traffic safety trainings, and higher risk sensitivity positively influenced knowledge gain through the training. CONCLUSION Knowledge and its gain through training are very low. Reasons for the lack of predictive power of some variables, such as motivation, personality and attitudes, are discussed. This study presents a new tool for testing knowledge on driving with warning lights and sirens. It shows the need for objective testing and for further research in this special area.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria J. Prohn
- Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Ziemssenstr. 5, 80336 Munich, Germany
| | - Britta Herbig
- Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Ziemssenstr. 5, 80336 Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Mills L, Freeman J, Truelove V, Davey J, Delhomme P. Comparative judgements of crash risk and driving ability for speeding behaviours. JOURNAL OF SAFETY RESEARCH 2021; 79:68-75. [PMID: 34848021 DOI: 10.1016/j.jsr.2021.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2020] [Revised: 04/13/2021] [Accepted: 08/11/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Preliminary research has indicated that numerous drivers perceive their risk of traffic crash to be less than other drivers, while perceiving their driving ability to be better. This phenomenon is referred to as 'comparative optimism' (CO) and may prove to inhibit the safe adoption of driving behaviors and/or dilute perceptions of negative outcomes. The objective of this study was to investigate comparative judgments regarding crash risk and driving ability, and how these judgments relate to self-reported speeding. METHOD There were 760 Queensland motorists comprised of 51.6% males and 48.2% females, aged 16-85 (M = 39.60). Participants completed either a paper or online version of a survey. Judgments of crash risk and driving ability were compared to two referents: the average same-age, same-sex driver, and the average same-age, same-sex V8 supercar champion. RESULTS Drivers displayed greater optimism when comparing their crash risk and driving ability to the average same-age, same- sex driver (respectively, 72%, 72.4%), than when comparing to a V8 supercar champion (respectively, 60%, 32.9%). When comparing judgements of crash risk and driving ability to a similar driver, it appears that participants in the present study are just about as optimistic about their risk of crash (i.e. 72%) as they are optimistic about their driving ability (i.e. 74.2%).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Mills
- Road Safety Research Collaboration, University of the Sunshine Coast, 90 Sippy Downs Dr, Sippy Downs, Queensland 4556, Australia.
| | - James Freeman
- Road Safety Research Collaboration, University of the Sunshine Coast, 90 Sippy Downs Dr, Sippy Downs, Queensland 4556, Australia
| | - Verity Truelove
- Road Safety Research Collaboration, University of the Sunshine Coast, 90 Sippy Downs Dr, Sippy Downs, Queensland 4556, Australia
| | - Jeremy Davey
- Road Safety Research Collaboration, University of the Sunshine Coast, 90 Sippy Downs Dr, Sippy Downs, Queensland 4556, Australia
| | - Patricia Delhomme
- Univ Gustave Eiffel, Université de Paris, LaPEA, F-78000 Versailles, France
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Kosuge R, Okamura K, Nakano Y, Fujita G. Characteristics of driving self-assessments and factors related to inaccurate self-assessment in Japanese older adults. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2021; 159:106235. [PMID: 34130059 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2021.106235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2020] [Revised: 05/07/2021] [Accepted: 05/30/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Older adults are considered to decide their driving behaviors based their own assessment of their driving performance, and thus it is important that these self-assessments be accurate. Therefore, this study aimed to clarify how older drivers assess their driving performance and examine factors related to the level of inaccuracy in their self-assessments. Japanese drivers aged 70 years or older (N = 181) were asked to assess their own driving performance by questionnaire and then to drive on a public road while wearing an electronic device that measured their actual driving behaviors. They were accompanied by a driving instructor who sat in the passenger seat and assessed their driving performance. The results showed that older drivers' self-assessments were significantly higher than the experts' assessments of their driving performance. This tendency applied to all driving competencies, including overall rating, speed, and scanning. In addition, there were greater discrepancies between self-assessments and expert assessments for drivers who were rated poorly by experts compared with those who were highly rated. Drivers with a greater rating discrepancy were likely to drive faster around an intersection with a stop sign. This discrepancy was also related to a low entropy rate (low randomness in head rotation) around a signalized T-junction. These findings based on on-road driving assessments provide credible evidence of performance overestimation by older drivers. Furthermore, the results suggest that making older drivers recognize the evaluative criteria might be effective for reducing the discrepancy between self-assessments and expert assessments. It was also suggested that improving the accuracy of one's self-assessment had the potential to improve driving behaviors such as choice of speed and scanning behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ritsu Kosuge
- National Research Institute of Police Science, 6-3-1, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa-shi, Chiba 277-0882, Japan.
| | - Kazuko Okamura
- National Research Institute of Police Science, 6-3-1, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa-shi, Chiba 277-0882, Japan
| | - Yukako Nakano
- National Research Institute of Police Science, 6-3-1, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa-shi, Chiba 277-0882, Japan
| | - Goro Fujita
- National Research Institute of Police Science, 6-3-1, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa-shi, Chiba 277-0882, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Horswill MS, Hill A, Silapurem L, Watson MO. A thousand years of crash experience in three hours: An online hazard perception training course for drivers. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2021; 152:105969. [PMID: 33497854 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2020.105969] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2020] [Revised: 11/19/2020] [Accepted: 12/26/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
A key goal of driver training is to teach drivers to avoid crashes. However, in traditional driver training, drivers are unlikely to see even a single example of the class of event that we want them to learn to avoid. We developed a six-session automated online hazard perception training course for drivers, which incorporates a range of evidence-based strategies and employs extensive video footage of real crashes. We evaluated this course in a randomized control trial by examining its effects on previously-validated computer-based measures of hazard perception, hazard prediction, speed choice, following distance, and gap acceptance propensity, as well as self-rated measures of driver skill, safety, and real world transfer. We found that the course resulted in significant improvements in hazard perception response time and hazard prediction scores, and significantly longer vehicle following distances. Additionally, all participants in the trained group reported that their real world driving behaviour had improved. No significant training effects were found for the other measures. The results suggest that the course can improve key behaviours associated with crash risk.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark S Horswill
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia.
| | - Andrew Hill
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia; Minerals Industry Safety and Health Centre, Sustainable Minerals Institute, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia
| | - Likitha Silapurem
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia
| | - Marcus O Watson
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Prohn MJ, Herbig B. Evaluating the effects of a simulator-based training on knowledge, attitudes and driving profiles of German ambulance drivers. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2020; 138:105466. [PMID: 32087394 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2020.105466] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2019] [Revised: 02/04/2020] [Accepted: 02/05/2020] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Accident risk is increased for emergency responders driving with warning lights and sirens compared to other road users' driving. Currently no standards for education of ambulance drivers exist. Research shows that high order understanding trainings focusing on insight to avoid critical driving situations might be more helpful than trainings focusing on car handling. The present controlled intervention study evaluates a one-day simulator-based high order training program specifically designed for ambulance drivers. METHODS In a longitudinal design with three measurement times multiple methods were used to evaluate the training holistically targeting the levels of reaction to training, learning, behavior and results of training. Questionnaire, knowledge test and driving profile data were analyzed with repeated measures analysis of variance controlling for age and sex. Data of two intervention groups and one control-waiting group was collected between 2014 and 2017 in two German federal states. RESULTS 183 German paramedics (age: M = 33.1, SD = 9.4, 21.9 % female) participated in the study. 147 participants (80.3 %) completed post-training tests, and 30 participants (16.4 %) completed follow-up measurements six months after training. Participants' reaction to training was positive directly after the training, and dropped slightly over time. Intervention group participants gained traffic-relevant knowledge compared to control group participants. Risk sensitivity of regular driving situations was the only attitude variable positively affected by training. This effect was not sustained six months after training. Training led to a decrease of average and maximum speed in short- as well as long-term measurements but did not affect drivers' acceleration. Although speed was lower in post-tests, emergency response times did not differ. CONCLUSION The simulator-based training for paramedics has small but notable effects on drivers' knowledge, attitudes and real driving behavior. Although only very few measured variables showed positive training effects, no negative training effects were found. Speed was reduced in the long term which underlines the importance of such a training. More research is needed to determine effects on different types of participants and to elicit framework conditions for training integration in formal education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria J Prohn
- Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Ziemssenstraße 1, 80336 München, Germany.
| | - Britta Herbig
- Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Ziemssenstraße 1, 80336 München, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Risk Perception Sensitivity of Cyclists Based on the Cox Risk Perception Model. SUSTAINABILITY 2020. [DOI: 10.3390/su12072613] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
To promote the sustainable development and safety of bicycle traffic, survival analysis of the risk perception sensitivity of cyclists is proposed. The cumulative probability of survival serves as an index of risk perception sensitivity, and a Cox regression model is established. The proposed method is applied to middle school cyclists, and the factors of their risk perception are analyzed. Data are collected by questionnaire and traffic conflict survey and are quantified by factor analysis. The model results show that active and extroverted personality, negative peer influence, unsafe riding behavior intention, non-motor vehicle flow and speed, and a lack of separation facilities have negative correlations with risk perception sensitivity. Positive attitude towards traffic rules, good family education, heightened traffic safety awareness, motor vehicle flow and speed, pedestrian flow, and non-motorized lane width have positive correlations with risk perception sensitivity. The conflict type has no correlation with risk perception sensitivity. This study aims to improve the sensitivity of risk perception, prevent traffic conflicts and provide a theoretical basis for risk perception research on vulnerable traffic participants.
Collapse
|
13
|
Steinbakk RT, Ulleberg P, Sagberg F, Fostervold KI. Effects of roadwork characteristics and drivers' individual differences on speed preferences in a rural work zone. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2019; 132:105263. [PMID: 31525650 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2019.105263] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2018] [Revised: 08/06/2019] [Accepted: 08/06/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Work zone safety from a psychological perspective has received little attention in scientific literature. Therefore, the present study aims to explore the influence of roadwork characteristics and drivers' individual differences in terms of personality traits and self-assessment of driving skills on speed preferences in a rural work zone. Eight hundred forty-five Norwegian drivers stated their preferred speed for ten pictures of a rural work zone with a 50 km/h reduced speed limit without knowing the speed limit. The results showed that the preferred speeds were greater than the actual reduced speed limit for all pictures. The standard deviations were quite high (from 11 to 14 km/h), indicating that drivers have a rather high variation in preferred speeds. A multilevel model was used to analyse the effects of the variables on speed preference. The results indicated that preferred speeds increased with age, higher scores on the normlessness scale, and higher self-assessment of own driving skills. As for the roadwork characteristics, speed increased with the presence of road markings by 11 km/h, while it decreased by 9 km/h with the presence of road delineators and by 5 km/h with barriers. Implications for respect for the reduced speed limits in work zones were discussed, and recommendations of other countermeasures were presented.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Renata Torquato Steinbakk
- Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Forskningsveien 3A, 0373, Oslo, Norway; Norwegian Public Roads Administration, Brynsengfaret 6A, 0667, Oslo, Norway.
| | - Pål Ulleberg
- Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Forskningsveien 3A, 0373, Oslo, Norway.
| | - Fridulv Sagberg
- Institute of Transport Economics, Oslo, Norway, Gaustadalléen 21, 0349, Oslo, Norway.
| | - Knut Inge Fostervold
- Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Forskningsveien 3A, 0373, Oslo, Norway.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Williams SE, Nelson R, Kashif R, Goff T, Simon L, Janse M. Demographic variation in the injury patterns of adult cyclists admitted to a large US healthcare network: A five-year review. TRAUMA-ENGLAND 2019. [DOI: 10.1177/1460408617740903] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Shanna E Williams
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of South Carolina School of Medicine Greenville, Greenville, SC, USA
| | - Rachel Nelson
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of South Carolina School of Medicine Greenville, Greenville, SC, USA
| | - Reema Kashif
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of South Carolina School of Medicine Greenville, Greenville, SC, USA
| | - Tyler Goff
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of South Carolina School of Medicine Greenville, Greenville, SC, USA
| | - Laura Simon
- Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of South Carolina School of Medicine Greenville, Greenville, SC, USA
| | - Melissa Janse
- Department of Emergency Medicine, Greenville Health System, Greenville, SC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Nees MA. Safer than the average human driver (who is less safe than me)? Examining a popular safety benchmark for self-driving cars. JOURNAL OF SAFETY RESEARCH 2019; 69:61-68. [PMID: 31235236 DOI: 10.1016/j.jsr.2019.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2018] [Revised: 11/20/2018] [Accepted: 02/12/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Although the level of safety required before drivers will accept self-driving cars is not clear, the criterion of being safer than a human driver has become pervasive in the discourse on vehicle automation. This criterion actually means "safer than the average human driver," because it is necessarily defined with respect to population-level data. At the level of individual risk assessment, a body of research has shown that most drivers perceive themselves to be safer than the average driver (the better-than-average effect). METHOD Using an online sample, this study examined U.S. drivers' ratings of their own ability to drive safely and their desired level of safety for self-driving vehicles. RESULTS This study replicated the better-than average effect and showed that most drivers stated a desire for self-driving cars that are safer than their own perceived ability to drive safely before they would: (1) feel reasonably safe riding in a self-driving vehicle; (2) buy a self-driving vehicle, all other things (cost, etc.) being equal; and (3) allow self-driving vehicles on public roads. CONCLUSIONS Since most drivers believe they are better than average drivers, the benchmark of achieving automation that is safer than a human driver (on average) may not represent acceptably safe performance of self-driving cars for most drivers. Practical applications: If perceived level of safety is an important contributor to acceptance of self-driving vehicles, the popular "safer than a human driver" benchmark may not be adequate for widespread acceptance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Nees
- Lafayette College, Department of Psychology, Oechsle Hall. 350 Hamilton Street, Easton, PA 18042, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Mouter N, van Cranenburgh S, van Wee B. The consumer-citizen duality: Ten reasons why citizens prefer safety and drivers desire speed. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2018; 121:53-63. [PMID: 30219726 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2018.08.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2017] [Revised: 07/04/2018] [Accepted: 08/28/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Cost-benefit analyses for transportation projects usually value impacts on safety and travel time through experiments in which consumers of mobility ('drivers') choose between routes which differ in safety and travel time. This approach has been criticized for failing to consider that private choices may not fully reflect citizens' preferences over public goods and means, a concept known as the consumer-citizen duality. Recent empirical evidence has established that individuals do indeed assign comparatively more value to safety in their role as citizens than in their role as drivers. Our study aims to provide explanations for this finding by presenting four stated choice experiments in which respondents were asked to make choices, both as citizens and as drivers, between routes that differed in travel time and safety. Subsequently, respondents were asked to provide reasons for their choices. We identify five cognitive and five normative explanations. The cognitive explanations suggest that individuals make diverging choices because their perceptions of accident risk differ between the two roles. Drivers will assign a relatively low value to mitigating accident risk because they believe that: (1) such risks are trivial on an individual level; (2) their personal risk is lower than the average risk; (3) their personal risk is controllable; (4) they would not be able to distinguish relative safety levels in real life; and (5) their choices for others are more risk-averse than choices for themselves and, unlike citizens, they are not explicitly evaluating risky choices for others. The normative explanations involve that individuals believe that the government should assign more value to safety compared to individual drivers because: (6) as citizen they are more prone to base their choices on social norms which prescribe risk-averse behaviour in this context; (7) governments have a duty of care concerning the safety of the transportation network; (8) drivers have a relatively high degree of responsibility to reduce their own travel times; (9) governments should account for drivers' tendencies to choose faster routes by building safer ones; and (10) governments should ensure the safety of the road network because this allows drivers to choose the fastest route without being concerned about the impact of their route choice on accident risk.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niek Mouter
- Delft University of Technology, Faculty of Technology, Policy and Management, Transport and Logistics Group, Jaffalaan 5, 2628 BX, Delft, The Netherlands.
| | - Sander van Cranenburgh
- Delft University of Technology, Faculty of Technology, Policy and Management, Transport and Logistics Group, Jaffalaan 5, 2628 BX, Delft, The Netherlands.
| | - Bert van Wee
- Delft University of Technology, Faculty of Technology, Policy and Management, Transport and Logistics Group, Jaffalaan 5, 2628 BX, Delft, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Schmidt JD, Lynall RC, Lempke LB, Weber ML, Devos H. Post-Concussion Driving Behaviors and Opinions: A Survey of Collegiate Student-Athletes. J Neurotrauma 2018; 35:2418-2424. [PMID: 29737227 DOI: 10.1089/neu.2018.5707] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Post-concussion driving restrictions are eminent, but we lack understanding of current behaviors and opinions about driving following concussion among populations at risk of concussion. We aimed to describe post-concussion driving behaviors and opinions among collegiate student-athletes. Student-athletes completed a survey (response rate = 45.3%, 223/492) regarding their post-concussion driving behaviors and opinions. Response frequencies and percentages are presented. Student-athletes self-reported a total of 169 lifetime concussions (0.76 ± 1.02 each). Of the 169 concussions, 52.1% (88/169) were diagnosed and 52.7% (89/169) occurred while the student-athlete possessed a valid driver's license. Student-athletes refrained from driving following 43.8% (39/89) of the concussive events. Student-athletes who refrained most commonly did so for only 24-48 h (20.5%, 8/39) and because a health care provider advised them to (33.3%, 13/39). Student-athletes most commonly reported that they would feel "very unsafe" driving a car immediately following injury (38.4%, 84/219). When asked whether driving restrictions would influence their decision to report the injury to a health care provider, 7.9% reported that it "definitely would" (17/214), 26.6% "probably would" (57/214), 17.8%"neutral" (38/214), 24.8% "probably would not" (53/214), and 22.9% "definitely would not" (49/214). Despite generally believing that driving immediately following a concussion is unsafe, a majority of student-athletes did not refrain from driving at any point following their previous concussions. Post-concussion driving restrictions may have some influence on student-athletes' decisions to report the injury to a health care provider. Health care providers play a critical role in post-concussion driving restriction, but lack standardized recommendations to guide their care.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Robert C Lynall
- 1 Department of Kinesiology, University of Georgia , Athens, Georgia
| | - Landon B Lempke
- 1 Department of Kinesiology, University of Georgia , Athens, Georgia
| | - Michelle L Weber
- 1 Department of Kinesiology, University of Georgia , Athens, Georgia
| | - Hannes Devos
- 2 Department of Physical Therapy and Rehabilitation Science, University of Kansas Medical Center , Kansas City, Kansas
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
|
19
|
Sadia R, Bekhor S, Polus A. Structural equations modelling of drivers' speed selection using environmental, driver, and risk factors. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2018; 116:21-29. [PMID: 28939098 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.08.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2017] [Revised: 07/08/2017] [Accepted: 08/24/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Drivers' speed has significant implications on road users' safety in general, and particularly so if a crash occurs. This paper explores the influence of environmental and road characteristics, situational factors, and individual characteristics on drivers' observed speed selection in a simulator experiment. The paper presents a theoretical framework for drivers' speed selection, and applies structural equation modeling for the various factors examined. The simulator experiments collected data of 111 drivers driving in 4 different scenarios composed of 22 segments for each scenario. The dataset was analyzed in several resolutions: Driver level, Trip level, and Segment level. The three models revealed that gender, age, and driving frequency are all significant in determining drivers' perceptions and attitudes, which in turn influence speed selection. Situational factors such as traffic speed, enforcement, and time-saving-benefits are also related to speed selection, as well as infrastructure characteristics. These findings demonstrate that structural equations provide a flexible modeling tool able to concurrently analyze the variety of factors that relate to speed selection. As a result, Structural Equations Modeling provides more accurate and refined explanations for the combined effects of various factors on drivers' speed selection than previous research so far. These tools can be useful in developing speed management strategies to improve road safety.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Reut Sadia
- Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa 32000, Israel.
| | - Shlomo Bekhor
- Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa 32000, Israel.
| | - Abishai Polus
- Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa 32000, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Olmedilla A. Tendencia al riesgo y lesión deportiva en fútbol y fútbol sala femenino. REVISTA GUILLERMO DE OCKHAM 2018. [DOI: 10.21500/22563202.3846] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
La relación entre determinados factores psicológicos y la vulnerabilidad que presenta un deportista para sufrir una lesión ha sido estudiada en los últimos años. Uno de los aspectos a considerar es la influencia de las conductas de riesgo del deportista en la probabilidad de sufrir una lesión. El presente estudio pretende estudiar si la tendencia al riesgo es un factor que está relacionado con la lesión deportiva. Por tanto, el objetivo es determinar si existen diferencias en los niveles de tendencia al riesgo entre jugadoras de fútbol y fútbol sala lesionadas y no lesionadas. Para ello, un total de 102 jugadoras de fútbol y fútbol sala femenino, de edades comprendidas entre los 12 y los 37 años y pertenecientes a clubes federados de la Región de Murcia participaron en esta investigación. Para la obtención de los datos, se empleó la versión española del cuestionario DOSPERT (Domain Specific Risk Taking) para adultos (Blais & Weber, 2006). Los resultados obtenidos en este estudio no muestran diferencias estadísticamente significativas entre los dos grupos analizados (lesionadas y no lesionadas), aunque parece existir una tendencia a presentar puntuaciones superiores en la variable tendencia al riesgo entre las jugadoras pertenecientes al grupo lesionado.
Collapse
|
21
|
Thompson JP, Mackenzie JRR, Dutschke JK, Baldock MRJ, Raftery SJ, Wall J. A trial of retrofitted advisory collision avoidance technology in government fleet vehicles. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2018; 115:34-40. [PMID: 29544135 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2018.02.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2017] [Revised: 02/23/2018] [Accepted: 02/27/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
In-vehicle collision avoidance technology (CAT) has the potential to prevent crash involvement. In 2015, Transport for New South Wales undertook a trial of a Mobileye 560 CAT system that was installed in 34 government fleet vehicles for a period of seven months. The system provided headway monitoring, lane departure, forward collision and pedestrian collision warnings, using audio and visual alerts. The purpose of the trial was to determine whether the technology could change the driving behaviour of fleet vehicle drivers and improve their safety. The evaluation consisted of three components: (1) analysis of objective data to examine effects of the technology on driving behaviour, (2) analysis of video footage taken from a sample of the vehicles to examine driving circumstances that trigger headway monitoring and forward collision warnings, and (3) a survey completed by 122 of the 199 individuals who drove the trial vehicles to examine experiences with, and attitudes to, the technology. Analysis of the objective data found that the system resulted in changes in behaviour with increased headway and improved lane keeping, but that these improvements dissipated once the warning alerts were switched off. Therefore, the system is capable of altering behaviour but only when it is actively providing alerts. In-vehicle video footage revealed that over a quarter of forward collision warnings were false alarms, in which a warning event was triggered despite there being no vehicle travelling ahead. The surveyed drivers recognised that the system could improve safety but most did not wish to use it themselves as they found it to be distracting and felt that it would not prevent them from having a crash. The results demonstrate that collision avoidance technology can improve driving behaviour but drivers may need to be educated about the potential benefits for their driving in order to accept the technology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James P Thompson
- Centre for Automotive Safety Research, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, SA 5005, Australia.
| | - Jamie R R Mackenzie
- Centre for Automotive Safety Research, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, SA 5005, Australia.
| | - Jeffrey K Dutschke
- Centre for Automotive Safety Research, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, SA 5005, Australia.
| | - Matthew R J Baldock
- Centre for Automotive Safety Research, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, SA 5005, Australia.
| | - Simon J Raftery
- Centre for Automotive Safety Research, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, SA 5005, Australia.
| | - John Wall
- Centre for Road Safety, Transport for NSW, Level 3, 84 Crown St, Wollongong, NSW 2500, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Joshi MS, Maclean M, Stevens C. Accident frequency and unrealistic optimism: Children's assessment of risk. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2018; 111:142-146. [PMID: 29202322 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.11.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2016] [Revised: 07/18/2017] [Accepted: 11/24/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Accidental injury is a major cause of mortality and morbidity among children, warranting research on their risk perceptions. Three hundred and seven children aged 10-11 years assessed the frequency, danger and personal risk likelihood of 8 accidents. Two social-cognitive biases were manifested. The frequency of rare accidents (e.g. drowning) was overestimated, and the frequency of common accidents (e.g. bike accidents) underestimated; and the majority of children showed unrealistic optimism tending to see themselves as less likely to suffer these accidents in comparison to their peers, offering superior skills or parental control of the environment as an explanation. In the case of pedestrian accidents, children recognised their seriousness, underestimated the frequency of this risk and regarded their own road crossing skill as protection. These findings highlight the challenging task facing safety educators who, when teaching conventional safety knowledge and routines, also need to alert children to the danger of over-confidence without disabling them though fear.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mary Sissons Joshi
- Department of Psychology, Health and Professional Development, Faculty of Health & Life Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK.
| | - Morag Maclean
- Department of Psychology, Health and Professional Development, Faculty of Health & Life Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK.
| | - Claire Stevens
- Research Department of Behavioural Science and Health, UCL Institute of Epidemiology and Health Care, London WC1E 6BT, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Davis JJ, Conlon EG. Identifying compensatory driving behavior among older adults using the situational avoidance questionnaire. JOURNAL OF SAFETY RESEARCH 2017; 63:47-55. [PMID: 29203023 DOI: 10.1016/j.jsr.2017.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2017] [Revised: 07/29/2017] [Accepted: 08/17/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Driving self-regulation is considered a means through which older drivers can compensate for perceived declines in driving skill or more general feelings of discomfort on the road. One form of driving self-regulation is situational avoidance, the purposeful avoidance of situations perceived as challenging or potentially hazardous. This study aimed to validate the Situational Avoidance Questionnaire (SAQ, Davis, Conlon, Ownsworth, & Morrissey, 2016) and identify the point on the scale at which drivers practicing compensatory avoidance behavior could be distinguished from those whose driving is unrestricted, or who are avoiding situations for other, non-compensatory reasons (e.g., time or convenience). METHOD Seventy-nine Australian drivers (Mage=71.48, SD=7.16, range: 55 to 86years) completed the SAQ and were classified as a compensatory-restricted or a non-restricted driver based on a semi-structured interview designed to assess the motivations underlying avoidance behavior reported on the SAQ. RESULTS Using receiver-operator characteristic (ROC) analysis, the SAQ was found to have high diagnostic accuracy (sensitivity: 85%, specificity: 82%) in correctly classifying the driver groups. Group comparisons confirmed that compensatory-restricted drivers were self-regulating their driving behavior to reduce the perceived demands of the driving task. This group had, on average, slower hazard perception reaction times, and reported greater difficulty with driving, more discomfort when driving due to difficulty with hazard perception skills, and greater changes in cognition over the past five years. CONCLUSIONS The SAQ is a psychometrically sound measure of situational avoidance for drivers in baby boomer and older adult generations. PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS Use of validated measures of driving self-regulation that distinguish between compensatory and non-compensatory behavior, such as the SAQ, will advance our understanding of the driving self-regulation construct and its potential safety benefits for older road users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica J Davis
- School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Australia.
| | - Elizabeth G Conlon
- School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
|
25
|
Payyanadan RP, Maus A, Sanchez FA, Lee JD, Miossi L, Abera A, Melvin J, Wang X. Using trip diaries to mitigate route risk and risky driving behavior among older drivers. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2017; 106:480-491. [PMID: 27720427 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.09.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2016] [Revised: 08/29/2016] [Accepted: 09/22/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
To reduce exposure to risky and challenging driving situations and prolong mobility and independence, older drivers self-regulate their driving behavior. But self-regulation can be challenging because it depends on drivers' ability to assess their limitations. Studies using self-reports, survey data, and hazard and risk perception tests have shown that driving behavior feedback can help older drivers assess their limitations and adjust their driving behavior. But only limited work has been conducted in developing feedback technology interventions tailored to meet the information needs of older drivers, and the impact these interventions have in helping older drivers self-monitor their driving behavior and risk outcomes. The vehicles of 33 drivers 65 years and older were instrumented with OBD2 devices. Older drivers were provided access to customized web-based Trip Diaries that delivered post-trip feedback of the routes driven, low-risk route alternatives, and frequency of their risky driving behaviors. Data were recorded over four months, with baseline driving behavior collected for one month. Generalized linear mixed effects regression models assessed the effects of post-trip feedback on the route risk and driving behaviors of older drivers. Results showed that post-trip feedback reduced the estimated route risk of older drivers by 2.9% per week, and reduced their speeding frequency on average by 0.9% per week. Overall, the Trip Diary feedback reduced the expected crash rate from 1 in 6172 trips to 1 in 7173 trips, and the expected speeding frequency from 46% to 39%. Thus providing older drivers with tailored feedback of their driving behavior and crash risk could help them appropriately self-regulate their driving behavior, and improve their crash risk outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rashmi P Payyanadan
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA.
| | - Adam Maus
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| | - Fabrizzio A Sanchez
- Department of Statistics, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| | - John D Lee
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| | - Lillian Miossi
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| | - Amsale Abera
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| | - Jacob Melvin
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| | - Xufan Wang
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, 53706, USA
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Richardson S, Mishuris R, O'Connell A, Feldstein D, Hess R, Smith P, McCullagh L, McGinn T, Mann D. "Think aloud" and "Near live" usability testing of two complex clinical decision support tools. Int J Med Inform 2017; 106:1-8. [PMID: 28870378 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2017.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2016] [Revised: 06/16/2017] [Accepted: 06/20/2017] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Low provider adoption continues to be a significant barrier to realizing the potential of clinical decision support. "Think Aloud" and "Near Live" usability testing were conducted on two clinical decision support tools. Each was composed of an alert, a clinical prediction rule which estimated risk of either group A Streptococcus pharyngitis or pneumonia and an automatic order set based on risk. The objective of this study was to further understanding of the facilitators of usability and to evaluate the types of additional information gained from proceeding to "Near Live" testing after completing "Think Aloud". METHODS This was a qualitative observational study conducted at a large academic health care system with 12 primary care providers. During "Think Aloud" testing, participants were provided with written clinical scenarios and asked to verbalize their thought process while interacting with the tool. During "Near Live" testing participants interacted with a mock patient. Morae usability software was used to record full screen capture and audio during every session. Participant comments were placed into coding categories and analyzed for generalizable themes. Themes were compared across usability methods. RESULTS "Think Aloud" and "Near Live" usability testing generated similar themes under the coding categories visibility, workflow, content, understand-ability and navigation. However, they generated significantly different themes under the coding categories usability, practical usefulness and medical usefulness. During both types of testing participants found the tool easier to use when important text was distinct in its appearance, alerts were passive and appropriately timed, content was up to date, language was clear and simple, and each component of the tool included obvious indicators of next steps. Participant comments reflected higher expectations for usability and usefulness during "Near Live" testing. For example, visit aids, such as automatically generated order sets, were felt to be less useful during "Near-Live" testing because they would not be all inclusive for the visit. CONCLUSIONS These complementary types of usability testing generated unique and generalizable insights. Feedback during "Think Aloud" testing primarily helped to improve the tools' ease of use. The additional feedback from "Near Live" testing, which mimics a real clinical encounter, was helpful for eliciting key barriers and facilitators to provider workflow and adoption.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - David Feldstein
- University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, United States.
| | - Rachel Hess
- University of Utah School of Medicine, United States.
| | - Paul Smith
- University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, United States.
| | | | - Thomas McGinn
- Hofstra Northwell School of Medicine, United States.
| | - Devin Mann
- New York University School of Medicine, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Horswill MS, Garth M, Hill A, Watson MO. The effect of performance feedback on drivers' hazard perception ability and self-ratings. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2017; 101:135-142. [PMID: 28226254 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.02.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2015] [Revised: 06/26/2016] [Accepted: 02/09/2017] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Drivers' hazard perception ability has been found to predict crash risk, and novice drivers appear to be particularly poor at this skill. This competency appears to develop only slowly with experience, and this could partially be a result of poor quality performance feedback. We report an experiment in which we provided high-quality artificial feedback on individual drivers' performance in a validated video-based hazard perception test via either: (1) a graph-based comparison of hazard perception response times between the test-taker, the average driver, and an expert driver; (2) a video-based comparison between the same groups; or (3) both. All three types of feedback resulted in both an improvement in hazard perception performance and a reduction in self-rated hazard perception skill, compared with a no-feedback control group. Video-based and graph-based feedback combined resulted in a greater improvement in hazard perception performance than either of the individual components, which did not differ from one another. All three types of feedback eliminated participants' self-enhancement bias for hazard perception skill. Participants judged both interventions involving video feedback to be significantly more likely to improve their real-world driving than the no feedback control group. While all three forms of feedback had some value, the combined video and graph feedback intervention appeared to be the most effective across all outcome measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark S Horswill
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane QLD 4072, Australia.
| | - Megan Garth
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Andrew Hill
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Marcus O Watson
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane QLD 4072, Australia; School of Medicine, The University of Queensland, Herston, Brisbane, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
UNLABELLED Bright sunlight may create visual illusions that lead to driver error, including fallible distance judgment from aerial perspective. We tested whether the risk of a life-threatening motor vehicle crash was increased when driving in bright sunlight.This longitudinal, case-only, paired-comparison analysis evaluated patients hospitalized because of a motor vehicle crash between January 1, 1995 and December 31, 2014. The relative risk of a crash associated with bright sunlight was estimated by evaluating the prevailing weather at the time and place of the crash compared with the weather at the same hour and location on control days a week earlier and a week later.The majority of patients (n = 6962) were injured during daylight hours and bright sunlight was the most common weather condition at the time and place of the crash. The risk of a life-threatening crash was 16% higher during bright sunlight than normal weather (95% confidence interval: 9-24, P < 0.001). The increased risk was accentuated in the early afternoon, disappeared at night, extended to patients with different characteristics, involved crashes with diverse features, not apparent with cloudy weather, and contributed to about 5000 additional patient-days in hospital. The increased risk extended to patients with high crash severity as indicated by ambulance involvement, surgical procedures, length of hospital stay, intensive care unit admission, and patient mortality. The increased risk was not easily attributed to differences in alcohol consumption, driving distances, or anomalies of adverse weather.Bright sunlight is associated with an increased risk of a life-threatening motor vehicle crash. An awareness of this risk might inform driver education, trauma staffing, and safety warnings to prevent a life-threatening motor vehicle crash. LEVEL OF EVIDENCE Epidemiologic Study, level III.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Donald A. Redelmeier
- Department of Medicine, University of Toronto
- Evaluative Clinical Sciences, Sunnybrook Research Institute
- Institute of Clinical Evaluative Sciences (ICES)
- Institute for Health Policy Management and Evaluation
- Division of General Internal Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Sheharyar Raza
- Department of Medicine, University of Toronto
- Evaluative Clinical Sciences, Sunnybrook Research Institute
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Watson JM, Memmott MG, Moffitt CC, Coleman J, Turrill J, Fernández Á, Strayer DL. On Working Memory and a Productivity Illusion in Distracted Driving. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.06.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
30
|
Cordellieri P, Baralla F, Ferlazzo F, Sgalla R, Piccardi L, Giannini AM. Gender Effects in Young Road Users on Road Safety Attitudes, Behaviors and Risk Perception. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1412. [PMID: 27729877 PMCID: PMC5037216 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01412] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2016] [Accepted: 09/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In the present study, we investigated gender-related effects on road safety attitudes in 2681 young drivers (1458 males, 54.4%; aged 18-22) who filled out several scales assessing attitudes toward road safety issues, driving behavior in specific hypothetical situations, accident risk perception, and concerns about such a risk. We focused only on young drivers to better understand the role of gender in road safety attitudes in a period of life in which risky behaviors are widespread for males and females. Indeed, there is still no agreement as to the nature of these gender differences. According to some authors, the effects of gender on being involved in a crash due to driving skills are either non-existent or largely explained by differences in alcohol consumption. In our study, we found gender differences in road safety attitudes (i.e., "negative attitude toward traffic rules and risky driving"; "negative attitude toward drugs and alcohol" and "tolerance toward speeding") and in driver behavior (i.e., "errors in inattentive driving" and "driving violations"). This result is consistent in all drivers coming from nine different European countries. Our analyses yielded an important finding concerning risk perception. The results indicate that the level of risk perception during driving is the same for males and females. However, these two groups differ in the level of concern about this risk, with males being less concerned about the risk of a road accident. This suggests that the main difference between these two groups is not strictly related to judgment of the perceived risk probability but rather to the level of concern experienced about the consequences of the risk. This difference between risk perception and worry could explain differences in the frequency of car accidents in the two groups. The present findings may provide new insights for the development of gender-based prevention programs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Fabio Ferlazzo
- Department of Psychology, "Sapienza" University of Rome Italy, Rome
| | - Roberto Sgalla
- Department of Public Security, Ministry of Interior Italy, Rome
| | - Laura Piccardi
- Department of Life, Health and Environmental Sciences, L'Aquila UniversityL'Aquila, Italy, Rome; Neuropsychology Unit, IRCCS Santa Lucia FoundationItaly, Rome
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Stavrinos D, Heaton K, Welburn SC, McManus B, Griffin R, Fine PR. Commercial Truck Driver Health and Safety. Workplace Health Saf 2016; 64:369-76. [DOI: 10.1177/2165079915620202] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Reducing distracters detrimental to commercial truck driving is a critical component of improving the safety performance of commercial drivers, and makes the highways safer for all drivers. This study used a driving simulator to examine effects of cell phone, texting, and email distractions as well as self-reported driver optimism bias on the driving performance of commercial truck drivers. Results revealed that more visually demanding tasks were related to poorer driving performance. However, the cell phone task resulted in less off-the-road eye glances. Drivers reporting being “very skilled” displayed poorer driving performance than those reporting being “skilled.” Onboard communication devices provide a practical, yet visually and manually demanding, solution for connecting drivers and dispatchers. Trucking company policies should minimize interaction between dispatchers and drivers when the truck is in motion. Training facilities should integrate driving simulators into the instruction of commercial drivers, targeting over-confident drivers.
Collapse
|
32
|
Sanbonmatsu DM, Strayer DL, Behrends AA, Ward N, Watson JM. Why drivers use cell phones and support legislation to restrict this practice. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2016; 92:22-33. [PMID: 27035396 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.03.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/26/2015] [Revised: 03/09/2016] [Accepted: 03/14/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The use of cell phones while driving is ubiquitous, particularly in countries where the practice is legal. However, surveys indicate that most drivers favor legislation to limit the use of mobile devices during the operation of a vehicle. A study was conducted to understand this inconsistency between what drivers do and what they advocate for others. Participants completed a survey about their driving attitudes, abilities, and behaviors. Following previous research, drivers reported using cell phones for benefits such as getting work done. The hypocrisy of using cell phones while advocating restrictions appears to stem from differences in the perceived safety risks of self vs. others' use of cell phones. Many if not most drivers believe they can drive safely while using mobile devices. However, they lack confidence in others' ability to drive safely while distracted and believe that others' use of cell phones is dangerous. The threat to public safety of others' usage of mobile devices was one of the strongest independent predictors of support for legislation to restrict cell phone use.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - David L Strayer
- Department of Psychology, University of Utah, United States.
| | | | - Nathan Ward
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana, Champaign, United States
| | - Jason M Watson
- Department of Psychology, University of Colorado Denver, United States
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Roberts SC, Horrey WJ, Liang Y. Measurement of driver calibration and the impact of feedback on drivers' estimates of performance. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2016; 88:150-158. [PMID: 26771893 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.12.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2015] [Revised: 10/13/2015] [Accepted: 12/14/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
Recent studies focused on driver calibration show that drivers are often miscalibrated, either over confident or under confident, and the magnitude of this miscalibration changes under different conditions. Previous work has demonstrated behavioral and performance benefits of feedback, yet these studies have not explicitly examined the issue of calibration. The objective of this study was to examine driver calibration, i.e., the degree to which drivers are accurately aware of their performance, and determine whether feedback alters driver calibration. Twenty-four drivers completed a series of driving tasks (pace clocks, traffic light, speed maintenance, and traffic cones) on a test track. Drivers drove three different blocks around the test track: (1) baseline block, where no participants received feedback; (2) feedback block, where half of the participants received performance feedback while the other half received no feedback; (3) a no feedback block, where no participants received feedback. Results indicated that across two different calibration measures, drivers were sufficiently calibrated to the pace clocks, traffic light, and traffic cone tasks. Drivers were not accurately aware of their performance regarding speed maintenance, though receiving feedback on this task improved calibration. Proper and accurate measurements of driver calibration are needed before designing performance feedback to improve calibration as these feedback systems may not always yield the intended results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - William J Horrey
- Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety, 71 Frankland Road, Hopkinton, MA 01748, USA.
| | - Yulan Liang
- Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety, 71 Frankland Road, Hopkinton, MA 01748, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Ivers RQ, Sakashita C, Senserrick T, Elkington J, Lo S, Boufous S, de Rome L. Does an on-road motorcycle coaching program reduce crashes in novice riders? A randomised control trial. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2016; 86:40-46. [PMID: 26513335 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.10.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2015] [Revised: 10/13/2015] [Accepted: 10/15/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Motorcycle riding is increasing globally and confers a high risk of crash-related injury and death. There is community demand for investment in rider training programs but no high-quality evidence about its effectiveness in preventing crashes. This randomised trial of an on-road rider coaching program aimed to determine its effectiveness in reducing crashes in novice motorcycle riders. METHODS Between May 2010 and October 2012, 2399 newly-licensed provisional riders were recruited in Victoria, Australia and completed a telephone interview before randomisation to intervention or control groups. Riders in the intervention group were offered an on-road motorcycle rider coaching program which involved pre-program activities, 4h riding and facilitated discussion in small groups with a riding coach. Outcome measures were collected for all participants via telephone interviews at 3 and 12 months after program delivery (or equivalent for controls), and via linkage to police-recorded crash and offence data. The primary outcome was a composite measure of police-recorded and self-reported crashes; secondary outcomes included traffic offences, near crashes, riding exposure, and riding behaviours and motivations. RESULTS Follow-up was 89% at 3 months and 88% at 12 months; 60% of the intervention group completed the program. Intention-to-treat analyses conducted in 2014 indicated no effect on crash risk at 3 months (adjusted OR 0.90, 95% CI: 0.65-1.27) or 12 months (adjusted OR 1.00, 95% CI: 0.78-1.29). Riders in the intervention group reported increased riding exposure, speeding behaviours and rider confidence. CONCLUSIONS There was no evidence that this on-road motorcycle rider coaching program reduced the risk of crash, and we found an increase in crash-related risk factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca Q Ivers
- The George Institute for Global Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia.
| | - Chika Sakashita
- The George Institute for Global Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Teresa Senserrick
- Transport and Road Safety Research, The University of NSW, Sydney, Australia
| | - Jane Elkington
- The George Institute for Global Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia; New York University, Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Serigne Lo
- The George Institute for Global Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
| | - Soufiane Boufous
- Transport and Road Safety Research, The University of NSW, Sydney, Australia
| | - Liz de Rome
- The George Institute for Global Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia; Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
DiMenichi BC, Tricomi E. The power of competition: Effects of social motivation on attention, sustained physical effort, and learning. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1282. [PMID: 26388801 PMCID: PMC4554955 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01282] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/27/2015] [Accepted: 08/11/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Competition has often been implicated as a means to improve effort-based learning and attention. Two experiments examined the effects of competition on effort and memory. In Experiment 1, participants completed a physical effort task in which they were rewarded for winning an overall percentage, or for winning a competition they believed was against another player. In Experiment 2, participants completed a memory task in which they were rewarded for remembering an overall percentage of shapes, or more shapes than a “competitor.” We found that, in the physical effort task, participants demonstrated faster reaction times (RTs)—a previous indicator of increased attention—in the competitive environment. Moreover, individual differences predicted the salience of competition’s effect. Furthermore, male participants showed faster RTs and greater sustained effort as a result of a competitive environment, suggesting that males may be more affected by competition in physical effort tasks. However, in Experiment 2, participants remembered fewer shapes when competing, and later recalled less of these shapes during a post-test, suggesting that competition was harmful in our memory task. The different results from these two experiments suggest that competition can improve attention in a physical effort task, yet caution the use of competition in memory tasks.
Collapse
|
36
|
Boele-Vos MJ, de Craen S. A randomized controlled evaluation study of the effects of a one-day advanced rider training course. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2015; 79:152-159. [PMID: 25827607 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.03.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2013] [Revised: 02/10/2015] [Accepted: 03/14/2015] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Motorcyclists have a relative high risk of crash involvement. As a consequence there is an on-going search for safety measures to improve road safety for motorcyclists. One popular measure is motorcycle training. Although intuitively sound, there are only few thorough studies on rider training courses and they do not always show a positive safety effect. The aim of this study was to assess the effects of the advanced rider training course 'Risk'. Through random assignment motorcyclists (N=222) were assigned to an experimental and control condition. At pre- and post-test, participants completed a questionnaire and their riding behaviour was assessed in an on-road ride. Furthermore, a selection of participants took a hazard perception test at post-test. Participants in the experimental condition (n=137) followed the advanced training course 'Risk' between pre- and post-test. RESULT s indicated that trained participants were rated higher on safe riding than the control group. A positive effect was also found for riding behaviour, i.e., speed and position on the road if it needed to be adapted to increase visibility and in reaction to potential hazard. The training did not affect riders' assessment of their own riding behaviour. Overall the trained riders performed better on the hazard perception test. This study is a step forward to demonstrate that motorcyclists' traffic behaviour can be positively influenced by the right training course. Crucial for this training course is that it did not lead to overconfidence, while it quantifiably improved traffic behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marjolein J Boele-Vos
- SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research, PO Box 93113, NL-2509 AC Den Haag, The Netherlands.
| | - Saskia de Craen
- SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research, PO Box 93113, NL-2509 AC Den Haag, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Horrey WJ, Lesch MF, Mitsopoulos-Rubens E, Lee JD. Calibration of skill and judgment in driving: development of a conceptual framework and the implications for road safety. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2015; 76:25-33. [PMID: 25560901 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2014.12.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2014] [Revised: 12/16/2014] [Accepted: 12/16/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Humans often make inflated or erroneous estimates of their own ability or performance. Such errors in calibration can be due to incomplete processing, neglect of available information or due to improper weighing or integration of the information and can impact our decision-making, risk tolerance, and behaviors. In the driving context, these outcomes can have important implications for safety. The current paper discusses the notion of calibration in the context of self-appraisals and self-competence as well as in models of self-regulation in driving. We further develop a conceptual framework for calibration in the driving context borrowing from earlier models of momentary demand regulation, information processing, and lens models for information selection and utilization. Finally, using the model we describe the implications for calibration (or, more specifically, errors in calibration) for our understanding of driver distraction, in-vehicle automation and autonomous vehicles, and the training of novice and inexperienced drivers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William J Horrey
- Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety, 71 Frankland Road, Hopkinton, MA 01748, USA.
| | - Mary F Lesch
- Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety, 71 Frankland Road, Hopkinton, MA 01748, USA.
| | | | - John D Lee
- Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin, 1513 University Avenue, Madison, WI 53706, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Lheureux F, Auzoult L, Charlois C, Hardy-Massard S, Minary JP. Traffic Offences: Planned or Habitual? Using the Theory of Planned Behaviour and habit strength to explain frequency and magnitude of speeding and driving under the influence of alcohol. Br J Psychol 2015; 107:52-71. [DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2014] [Revised: 12/25/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Florent Lheureux
- UFR SLHS Laboratory of Psychology (EA3188); University of Franche-Comté; Besançon France
| | - Laurent Auzoult
- UFR SLHS Laboratory of Psychology (EA3188); University of Franche-Comté; Besançon France
| | - Colette Charlois
- UFR SLHS Laboratory of Psychology (EA3188); University of Franche-Comté; Besançon France
| | - Sandrine Hardy-Massard
- UFR SLHS Laboratory of Psychology (EA3188); University of Franche-Comté; Besançon France
| | - Jean-Pierre Minary
- UFR SLHS Laboratory of Psychology (EA3188); University of Franche-Comté; Besançon France
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Meng F, Li S, Cao L, Li M, Peng Q, Wang C, Zhang W. Driving fatigue in professional drivers: a survey of truck and taxi drivers. TRAFFIC INJURY PREVENTION 2015; 16:474-483. [PMID: 25357206 DOI: 10.1080/15389588.2014.973945] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Fatigue among truck drivers has been studied extensively; however, less is known regarding the fatigue experience of taxi drivers in heavily populated metropolitan areas. This study aimed to compare the differences and similarities between truck and taxi driver fatigue to provide implications for the fatigue management and education of professional drivers. METHODS A sample of 274 truck drivers and 286 taxi drivers in Beijing was surveyed via a questionnaire, which included items regarding work characteristics, fatigue experience, accident information, attitude toward fatigue, and methods of counteracting fatigue. RESULTS Driver fatigue was prevalent among professional drivers, and it was even more serious for taxi drivers. Taxi drivers reported more frequent fatigue experiences and were involved in more accidents. Among the contributing factors to fatigue, prolonged driving time was the most important factor identified by both driver groups. Importantly, the reason for the engagement in prolonged driving was neither due to the lack of awareness concerning the serious outcome of fatigue driving nor because of their poor detection of fatigue. The most probable reason was the optimism bias, as a result of which these professional drivers thought that fatigue was more serious for other drivers than for themselves, and they thought that they were effective in counteracting the effect of fatigue on their driving performance. Moreover, truck drivers tended to employ methods that require stopping to counteract fatigue, whereas taxi drivers preferred methods that were simultaneous with driving. Although both driver groups considered taking a nap as one of the most effective means to address fatigue, this method was not commonly used. Interestingly, these drivers were aware that the methods they frequently used were not the most effective means to counteract fatigue. CONCLUSIONS This study provides knowledge on truck and taxi drivers' characteristics in fatigue experience, fatigue attitude, and fatigue countermeasures, and these findings have practical implications for the fatigue management and education of professional drivers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fanxing Meng
- a State Key Laboratory of Automotive Safety and Energy, Department of Industrial Engineering , Tsinghua University , Beijing , China
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Carter PM, Bingham CR, Zakrajsek JS, Shope JT, Sayer TB. Social norms and risk perception: predictors of distracted driving behavior among novice adolescent drivers. J Adolesc Health 2014; 54:S32-41. [PMID: 24759439 PMCID: PMC7189891 DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.01.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 102] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2013] [Revised: 01/07/2014] [Accepted: 01/07/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE Adolescent drivers are at elevated crash risk due to distracted driving behavior (DDB). Understanding parental and peer influences on adolescent DDB may aid future efforts to decrease crash risk. We examined the influence of risk perception, sensation seeking, as well as descriptive and injunctive social norms on adolescent DDB using the theory of normative social behavior. METHODS 403 adolescents (aged 16-18 years) and their parents were surveyed by telephone. Survey instruments measured self-reported sociodemographics, DDB, sensation seeking, risk perception, descriptive norms (perceived parent DDB, parent self-reported DDB, and perceived peer DDB), and injunctive norms (parent approval of DDB and peer approval of DDB). Hierarchical multiple linear regression was used to predict the influence of descriptive and injunctive social norms, risk perception, and sensation seeking on adolescent DDB. RESULTS 92% of adolescents reported regularly engaging in DDB. Adolescents perceived that their parents and peers participated in DDB more frequently than themselves. Adolescent risk perception, parent DDB, perceived parent DDB, and perceived peer DDB were predictive of adolescent DDB in the regression model, but parent approval and peer approval of DDB were not predictive. Risk perception and parental DDB were stronger predictors among males, whereas perceived parental DDB was stronger for female adolescents. CONCLUSIONS Adolescent risk perception and descriptive norms are important predictors of adolescent distracted driving. More study is needed to understand the role of injunctive normative influences on adolescent DDB. Effective public health interventions should address parental role modeling, parental monitoring of adolescent driving, and social marketing techniques that correct misconceptions of norms related to around driver distraction and crash risk.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick M Carter
- University of Michigan Injury Center, Ann Arbor, Michigan; Department of Emergency Medicine, University of Michigan School of Medicine, Ann Arbor, Michigan; Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan School of Medicine, Ann Arbor, Michigan.
| | - C Raymond Bingham
- University of Michigan Injury Center, Ann Arbor, Michigan; Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan School of Medicine, Ann Arbor, Michigan; University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute, Ann Arbor, Michigan; Department of Health Behavior and Health Education, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, Michigan
| | | | - Jean T Shope
- University of Michigan Injury Center, Ann Arbor, Michigan; University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute, Ann Arbor, Michigan; Department of Health Behavior and Health Education, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, Michigan
| | - Tina B Sayer
- Toyota Engineering and Manufacturing North America, Ann Arbor, Michigan
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Amado S, Arıkan E, Kaça G, Koyuncu M, Turkan BN. How accurately do drivers evaluate their own driving behavior? An on-road observational study. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2014; 63:65-73. [PMID: 24269581 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.10.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2013] [Revised: 10/11/2013] [Accepted: 10/22/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Self-assessment of driving skills became a noteworthy research subject in traffic psychology, since by knowing one's strenghts and weaknesses, drivers can take an efficient compensatory action to moderate risk and to ensure safety in hazardous environments. The current study aims to investigate drivers' self-conception of their own driving skills and behavior in relation to expert evaluations of their actual driving, by using naturalistic and systematic observation method during actual on-road driving session and to assess the different aspects of driving via comprehensive scales sensitive to different specific aspects of driving. 19-63 years old male participants (N=158) attended an on-road driving session lasting approximately 80min (45km). During the driving session, drivers' errors and violations were recorded by an expert observer. At the end of the driving session, observers completed the driver evaluation questionnaire, while drivers completed the driving self-evaluation questionnaire and Driver Behavior Questionnaire (DBQ). Low to moderate correlations between driver and observer evaluations of driving skills and behavior, mainly on errors and violations of speed and traffic lights was found. Furthermore, the robust finding that drivers evaluate their driving performance as better than the expert was replicated. Over-positive appraisal was higher among drivers with higher error/violation score and with the ones that were evaluated by the expert as "unsafe". We suggest that the traffic environment might be regulated by increasing feedback indicators of errors and violations, which in turn might increase the insight into driving performance. Improving self-awareness by training and feedback sessions might play a key role for reducing the probability of risk in their driving activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonia Amado
- Ege University, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Letters, Bornova 35100, İzmir, Turkey.
| | - Elvan Arıkan
- Yaşar University, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Letters, Bornova 35100, İzmir, Turkey.
| | - Gülin Kaça
- Ege University, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Letters, Bornova 35100, İzmir, Turkey.
| | - Mehmet Koyuncu
- Ege University, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Letters, Bornova 35100, İzmir, Turkey.
| | - B Nilay Turkan
- Ege University, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Letters, Bornova 35100, İzmir, Turkey.
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Helman S, Kinnear NAD, McKenna FP, Allsop RE, Horswill MS. Changes in self-reported driving intentions and attitudes while learning to drive in Great Britain. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2013; 59:425-431. [PMID: 23896046 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.06.038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2013] [Revised: 06/26/2013] [Accepted: 06/30/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Novice drivers are overrepresented in traffic collisions, especially in their first year of solo driving. It is widely accepted that some driving behaviours (such as speeding and thrill-seeking) increase risk in this group. Increasingly research is suggesting that attitudes and behavioural intentions held in the pre-driver and learning stage are important in determining later driver behaviour in solo driving. In this study we examine changes in several self-reported attitudes and behavioural intentions across the learning stage in a sample of learner drivers in Great Britain. A sample of 204 learner drivers completed a self-report questionnaire near the beginning of their learning, and then again shortly after they passed their practical driving test. Results showed that self-reported intentions regarding speed choice, perceptions regarding skill level, and intentions regarding thrill-seeking (through driving) became less safe over this time period, while self-reported intentions regarding following distance and overtaking tendency became safer. The results are discussed with reference to models of driver behaviour that focus on task difficulty; it is suggested that the manner in which behind-the-wheel experience relates to the risk measures of interest may be the key determining factor in how these change over the course of learning to drive.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Helman
- Transport Research Laboratory, Crowthorne House, Nine Mile Ride, Wokingham, Berkshire RG40 3GA, UK.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Horswill MS, Taylor K, Newnam S, Wetton M, Hill A. Even highly experienced drivers benefit from a brief hazard perception training intervention. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2013; 52:100-110. [PMID: 23314117 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.12.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2012] [Revised: 09/20/2012] [Accepted: 12/09/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
We examined the proposal that hazard perception ability is suboptimal even in highly experienced mid-age drivers. First, we replicated previous findings in which police drivers significantly outperformed highly experienced drivers on a validated video-based hazard perception test, indicating that the ability of the experienced participants had not reached ceiling despite decades of driving. Second, we found that the highly experienced drivers' hazard perception test performance could be improved with a mere 20 min of video-based training, and this improvement remained evident after a delay of at least a week. One possible explanation as to why hazard perception skill may be suboptimal even in experienced drivers is a dearth of self-insight, potentially resulting in a lack of motivation to improve this ability. Consistent with this proposal, we found no significant relationships between self-ratings and objective measures of hazard perception ability in this group. We also found significant self-enhancement biases in the self-ratings and that participants who received training did not rate their performance (either in real driving or in the test) as having improved, contrary to what was indicated by their objective performance data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark S Horswill
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Molina JG, Sanmartín J, Keskinen E. Driver training interests of a Spanish sample of young drivers and its relationship with their self-assessment skills concerning risky driving behavior. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2013; 52:118-124. [PMID: 23333696 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.12.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2012] [Revised: 12/18/2012] [Accepted: 12/21/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
Poor driving self-assessment skills (e.g., over-confidence) have been pointed out as an important explanatory factor behind young drivers' accident involvement. This paper explores (1) what young drivers miss in their training as drivers in order to analyze whether an assessment of one's own driving skills plays an important role in their desire to improve as drivers, and (2) how these training interests are related to an estimate of their self-assessment skills concerning risky driving behavior. For this purpose, a study was conducted using a survey with a blocked sampling design of novice drivers. The survey solicited respondents' self-report about (1) the contents of training courses that they feel would improve their driving, (2) their risky driving behavior, and (3) their likelihood of being involved in a risky driving situation. From the initial sample invited to participate, of nearly 1300 people, we finally obtained complete data from 321 young Spanish drivers. Two main results were apparent from our data analysis: (1) the novice drivers were mainly interested in improving their ability to recognize their strengths and weaknesses as drivers (i.e., self-assessment skills); (2) a significant relationship was found between novice drivers' interests and their current self-assessment skills concerning risky driving behavior. Specifically, there was greater general interest expressed in post-license training by the under-confident self-assessors than the over-confident ones. These results provide a relevant input which should be taken into account when designing driver training programs for novice drivers. Moreover, the relationship between their training interests and their risky driving self-assessment skills introduces an additional factor to be considered in the implementation of these training programs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Gabriel Molina
- INTRAS, Universitat de Valencia, C. Serpis 29, 46022 Valencia, Spain.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Félonneau ML, Causse E, Constant A, Contrand B, Messiah A, Lagarde E. Gender stereotypes and superior conformity of the self in a sample of cyclists. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2013; 50:336-340. [PMID: 22673603 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.05.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2012] [Revised: 04/25/2012] [Accepted: 05/02/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
In the field of driving, people tend to think they are more competent and more cautious than others. This is the superior conformity of the self (SCS). Our main hypothesis was that, among cyclists, women would show a higher SCS on cautiousness, though men would show a higher SCS concerning competence. 1799 cyclists provided a self-assessment of their own cautiousness and of other people's cautiousness. The same procedure was used for competence. Consistent with the hypothesis, the SCS was gender-specific: it was more prominent for women concerning cautiousness and more prominent for men concerning competence. These results could explain why people tend to ignore the safety campaigns. They also indicate the importance of adapting messages concerning safety measures to gender.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marie-Line Félonneau
- Laboratoire Psychologie, Santé & Qualité de Vie, Université de Bordeaux, F-33076, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Redelmeier DA, Yarnell CJ, Thiruchelvam D, Tibshirani RJ. Physicians' warnings for unfit drivers and the risk of trauma from road crashes. N Engl J Med 2012; 367:1228-36. [PMID: 23013074 DOI: 10.1056/nejmsa1114310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 102] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Physicians' warnings to patients who are potentially unfit to drive are a medical intervention intended to prevent trauma from motor vehicle crashes. We assessed the association between medical warnings and the risk of subsequent road crashes. METHODS We identified consecutive patients who received a medical warning in Ontario, Canada, between April 1, 2006, and December 31, 2009, from a physician who judged them to be potentially unfit to drive. We excluded patients who were younger than 18 years of age, who were not residents of Ontario, or who lacked valid health-card numbers under universal health insurance. We analyzed emergency department visits for road crashes during a baseline interval before the warning and a subsequent interval after the warning. RESULTS A total of 100,075 patients received a medical warning from a total of 6098 physicians. During the 3-year baseline interval, there were 1430 road crashes in which the patient was a driver and presented to the emergency department, as compared with 273 road crashes during the 1-year subsequent interval, representing a reduction of approximately 45% in the annual rate of crashes per 1000 patients after the warning (4.76 vs. 2.73, P<0.001). The lower rate was observed across patients with diverse characteristics. No significant change was observed in subsequent crashes in which patients were pedestrians or passengers. Medical warnings were associated with an increase in subsequent emergency department visits for depression and a decrease in return visits to the responsible physician. CONCLUSIONS Physicians' warnings to patients who are potentially unfit to drive may contribute to a decrease in subsequent trauma from road crashes, yet they may also exacerbate mood disorders and compromise the doctor-patient relationship. (Funded by the Canada Research Chairs program and others.).
Collapse
|
47
|
Horswill MS, Anstey KJ, Hatherly C, Wood JM, Pachana NA. Older drivers' insight into their hazard perception ability. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2011; 43:2121-2127. [PMID: 21819842 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.05.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2011] [Revised: 05/23/2011] [Accepted: 05/26/2011] [Indexed: 05/31/2023]
Abstract
Even though the driving ability of older adults may decline with age, there is evidence that some individuals attempt to compensate for these declines using strategies such as restricting their driving exposure. Such compensatory mechanisms rely on drivers' ability to evaluate their own driving performance. This paper focuses on one key aspect of driver ability that is associated with crash risk and has been found to decline with age: hazard perception. Three hundred and seven drivers, aged 65-96, completed a validated video-based hazard perception test. There was no significant relationship between hazard perception test response latencies and drivers' ratings of their hazard perception test performance, suggesting that their ability to assess their own test performance was poor. Also, age-related declines in hazard perception latency were not reflected in drivers' self-ratings. Nonetheless, ratings of test performance were associated with self-reported regulation of driving, as was self-rated driving ability. These findings are consistent with the proposal that, while self-assessments of driving ability may be used by drivers to determine the degree to which they restrict their driving, the problem is that drivershave little insight into their own driving ability. This may impact on the potential road safety benefits of self-restriction of driving because drivers may not have the information needed to optimally self-restrict. Strategies for addressing this problem are discussed.
Collapse
|
48
|
White MJ, Cunningham LC, Titchener K. Young drivers' optimism bias for accident risk and driving skill: Accountability and insight experience manipulations. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2011; 43:1309-1315. [PMID: 21545859 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.01.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2010] [Revised: 01/27/2011] [Accepted: 01/31/2011] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
This study aimed to determine whether two brief, low cost interventions would reduce young drivers' optimism bias for their driving skills and accident risk perceptions. This tendency for such drivers to perceive themselves as more skillful and less prone to driving accidents than their peers may lead to less engagement in precautionary driving behaviours and a greater engagement in more dangerous driving behaviour. 243 young drivers (aged 17-25 years) were randomly allocated to one of three groups: accountability, insight or control. All participants provided both overall and specific situation ratings of their driving skills and accident risk relative to a typical young driver. Prior to completing the questionnaire, those in the accountability condition were first advised that their driving skills and accident risk would be later assessed via a driving simulator. Those in the insight condition first underwent a difficult computer-based hazard perception task designed to provide participants with insight into their potential limitations when responding to hazards in difficult and unpredictable driving situations. Participants in the control condition completed only the questionnaire. Results showed that the accountability manipulation was effective in reducing optimism bias in terms of participants' comparative ratings of their accident risk in specific situations, though only for less experienced drivers. In contrast, among more experienced males, participants in the insight condition showed greater optimism bias for overall accident risk than their counterparts in the accountability or control groups. There were no effects of the manipulations on drivers' skills ratings. The differential effects of the two types of manipulations on optimism bias relating to one's accident risk in different subgroups of the young driver sample highlight the importance of targeting interventions for different levels of experience. Accountability interventions may be beneficial for less experienced young drivers but the results suggest exercising caution with the use of insight type interventions, particularly hazard perception style tasks, for more experienced young drivers typically still in the provisional stage of graduated licensing systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melanie J White
- Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety Queensland, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, 170 Victoria Park Road, Kelvin Grove, Queensland 4059, Australia.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
49
|
Rhodes N, Pivik K. Age and gender differences in risky driving: the roles of positive affect and risk perception. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2011; 43:923-31. [PMID: 21376884 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.11.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 187] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2008] [Revised: 10/31/2010] [Accepted: 11/24/2010] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
A phone survey of 504 teen (age 16-20) and 409 adult (age 25-45) drivers in the US state of Alabama was conducted to examine the relationships among risk perception, positive affect and risky driving. Male drivers reported engaging in risky driving behaviors more frequently than female drivers and teen drivers reported engaging in risky driving behaviors more frequently than adult drivers. Positive affect (liking for risky driving behaviors) and perceived risk mediated the relationships of age and gender with risky driving. Affect and risk perception were independent predictors of risky driving behavior. Interactions of positive affect and perceived risk with gender and age showed that positive affect more strongly predicted risky driving for teen and male drivers than for adult and female drivers. These findings are interpreted in the context of dual process models of behavioral decision making. Future research into interventions designed to moderate the positive affect surrounding driving may have promise for reducing risky driving behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nancy Rhodes
- Department of Communication Studies, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis 46202, United States.
| | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Perrissol S, Smeding A, Laumond F, Le Floch V. Effect of a road safety training program on drivers' comparative optimism. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2011; 43:478-482. [PMID: 21094347 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.08.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2010] [Revised: 08/20/2010] [Accepted: 08/26/2010] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
Reducing comparative optimism regarding risk perceptions in traffic accidents has been proven to be particularly difficult (Delhomme, 2000). This is unfortunate because comparative optimism is assumed to impede preventive action. The present study tested whether a road safety training course could reduce drivers' comparative optimism in high control situations. Results show that the training course efficiently reduced comparative optimism in high control, but not in low control situations. Mechanisms underlying this finding and implications for the design of road safety training courses are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stéphane Perrissol
- Université Toulouse le Mirail, CLLE-LTC (Cognition Langues, Langage, & Ergonomie - Laboratoire Travail et Cognition, UMR 5263), 5 allée Antonio Machado, 31058 Toulouse Cedex 9, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|