1
|
Moskowitz HS, Sussman ES. Sound category habituation requires task-relevant attention. Front Neurosci 2023; 17:1228506. [PMID: 37942141 PMCID: PMC10628171 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1228506] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2023] [Accepted: 10/12/2023] [Indexed: 11/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Processing the wealth of sensory information from the surrounding environment is a vital human function with the potential to develop learning, advance social interactions, and promote safety and well-being. Methods To elucidate underlying processes governing these activities we measured neurophysiological responses to patterned stimulus sequences during a sound categorization task to evaluate attention effects on implicit learning, sound categorization, and speech perception. Using a unique experimental design, we uncoupled conceptual categorical effects from stimulus-specific effects by presenting categorical stimulus tokens that did not physically repeat. Results We found effects of implicit learning, categorical habituation, and a speech perception bias when the sounds were attended, and the listeners performed a categorization task (task-relevant). In contrast, there was no evidence of a speech perception bias, implicit learning of the structured sound sequence, or repetition suppression to repeated within-category sounds (no categorical habituation) when participants passively listened to the sounds and watched a silent closed-captioned video (task-irrelevant). No indication of category perception was demonstrated in the scalp-recorded brain components when participants were watching a movie and had no task with the sounds. Discussion These results demonstrate that attention is required to maintain category identification and expectations induced by a structured sequence when the conceptual information must be extracted from stimuli that are acoustically distinct. Taken together, these striking attention effects support the theoretical view that top-down control is required to initiate expectations for higher level cognitive processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Howard S. Moskowitz
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, United States
| | - Elyse S. Sussman
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, United States
- Department of Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, Unites States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Lin YS, Chen CC, Greenlee MW. The role of lateral modulation in orientation-specific adaptation effect. J Vis 2022; 22:13. [PMID: 35191948 PMCID: PMC8883160 DOI: 10.1167/jov.22.2.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Center-surround modulation in visual processing reflects a normalization process of contrast gain control in the responsive neurons. Prior adaptation to a clockwise (CW) tilted grating, for example, leads to the percept of counterclockwise tilt in a vertical grating, referred to as the tilt-aftereffect (TAE). We previously reported that the magnitude of the TAE is modulated by adding a same-orientation annular surround to an adapter, suggesting inhibitory lateral modulation. To further examine the property of this lateral modulation effect on the perception of a central target, we here used center-surround sinusoidal patterns as adapters and varied the adapter surround and center orientations independently. The target had the same spatial extent as the adapter center with no physical overlap with the adapter surround. Participants were asked to judge the target orientation as tilted either CW or counterclockwise from vertical after adaptation. Results showed that, when the surround orientation was held constant, the TAE magnitude was determined by the adapter center, peaking between 10° and 20° of tilt. More important, the adapter surround orientation modulated the adaptation effect such that the TAE magnitude first decreased and then increased as the surround orientation became increasingly more different from that of the center, suggesting that the surround modulation effect was indeed orientation specific. Our data can be accounted for by a divisive inhibition model, in which (1) the adaptation effect is represented by increasing the normalizing constant and (2) the surround modulation is captured by two multiplicative sensitivity parameters determined by the adapter surround orientation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yih-Shiuan Lin
- Institute of Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany.,
| | - Chien-Chung Chen
- Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.,Neurobiology and Cognitive Science Center, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.,
| | - Mark W Greenlee
- Institute of Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany.,
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Li L, Chan A, Iqbal SM, Goldreich D. An Adaptation-Induced Repulsion Illusion in Tactile Spatial Perception. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:331. [PMID: 28701936 PMCID: PMC5487416 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2017] [Accepted: 06/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Following focal sensory adaptation, the perceived separation between visual stimuli that straddle the adapted region is often exaggerated. For instance, in the tilt aftereffect illusion, adaptation to tilted lines causes subsequently viewed lines with nearby orientations to be perceptually repelled from the adapted orientation. Repulsion illusions in the nonvisual senses have been less studied. Here, we investigated whether adaptation induces a repulsion illusion in tactile spatial perception. In a two-interval forced-choice task, participants compared the perceived separation between two point-stimuli applied on the forearms successively. Separation distance was constant on one arm (the reference) and varied on the other arm (the comparison). In Experiment 1, we took three consecutive baseline measurements, verifying that in the absence of manipulation, participants’ distance perception was unbiased across arms and stable across experimental blocks. In Experiment 2, we vibrated a region of skin on the reference arm, verifying that this focally reduced tactile sensitivity, as indicated by elevated monofilament detection thresholds. In Experiment 3, we applied vibration between the two reference points in our distance perception protocol and discovered that this caused an illusory increase in the separation between the points. We conclude that focal adaptation induces a repulsion aftereffect illusion in tactile spatial perception. The illusion provides clues as to how the tactile system represents spatial information. The analogous repulsion aftereffects caused by adaptation in different stimulus domains and sensory systems may point to fundamentally similar strategies for dynamic sensory coding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lux Li
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster UniversityHamilton, ON, Canada
| | - Arielle Chan
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster UniversityHamilton, ON, Canada
| | - Shah M Iqbal
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster UniversityHamilton, ON, Canada
| | - Daniel Goldreich
- Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster UniversityHamilton, ON, Canada.,McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study, McMaster UniversityHamilton, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Li B, Yuan X, Chen Y, Liu P, Huang X. Visual duration aftereffect is position invariant. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1536. [PMID: 26500591 PMCID: PMC4598571 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01536] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2015] [Accepted: 09/22/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Adaptation to relatively long or short sensory events leads to a negative aftereffect, such that the durations of the subsequent events within a certain range appear to be contracted or expanded. The distortion in perceived duration is presumed to arise from the adaptation of duration detectors. Here, we focus on the positional sensitivity of those visual duration detectors by exploring whether the duration aftereffect may be constrained by the visual location of stimuli. We adopted two different paradigms, one that tests for transfer across visual hemifields, and the other that tests for simultaneous selectivity between visual hemifields. By employing these experimental designs, we show that the duration aftereffect strongly transfers across visual hemifields and is not contingent on them. The lack of position specificity suggests that duration detectors in the visual system may operate at a relatively later stage of sensory processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Baolin Li
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Xiangyong Yuan
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Youguo Chen
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Peiduo Liu
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Xiting Huang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Li B, Yuan X, Huang X. The aftereffect of perceived duration is contingent on auditory frequency but not visual orientation. Sci Rep 2015; 5:10124. [PMID: 26054927 PMCID: PMC4460570 DOI: 10.1038/srep10124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2014] [Accepted: 03/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent sensory history plays a critical role in duration perception. It has been established that after adapting to a particular duration, the test durations within a certain range appear to be distorted. To explore whether the aftereffect of perceived duration can be constrained by sensory modality and stimulus feature within a modality, the current study applied the technique of simultaneous sensory adaptation, by which observers were able to simultaneously adapt to two durations defined by two different stimuli. Using both simple visual and auditory stimuli, we found that the aftereffect of perceived duration is modality specific and contingent on auditory frequency but not visual orientation of the stimulus. These results demonstrate that there are independent timers responsible for the aftereffects of perceived duration in each sensory modality. Furthermore, the timer for the auditory modality may be located at a relatively earlier stage of sensory processing than the timer for the visual modality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Baolin Li
- Key laboratory of cognition and personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China
- Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Xiangyong Yuan
- Key laboratory of cognition and personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China
- Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Xiting Huang
- Key laboratory of cognition and personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China
- Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Magnussen S. How I Started Living a Double Life in Psychological Science. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.2977] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Svein Magnussen
- Cognitive Developmental Research Unit, Department of Psychology; University of Oslo; Oslo Norway
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Gheorghiu E, Bell J, Kingdom FAA. Line orientation adaptation: local or global? PLoS One 2013; 8:e73307. [PMID: 24023677 PMCID: PMC3758281 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2013] [Accepted: 07/19/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Prolonged exposure to an oriented line shifts the perceived orientation of a subsequently observed line in the opposite direction, a phenomenon known as the tilt aftereffect (TAE). Here we consider whether the TAE for line stimuli is mediated by a mechanism that integrates the local parts of the line into a single global entity prior to the site of adaptation, or the result of the sum of local TAEs acting separately on the parts of the line. To test between these two alternatives we used the fact the TAE transfers almost completely across luminance contrast polarity [1]. We measured the TAE using adaptor and test lines that (1) either alternated in luminance polarity or were of a single polarity, and (2) either alternated in local orientation or were of a single orientation. We reasoned that if the TAE was agnostic to luminance polarity and was parts-based, we should obtain large TAEs using alternating-polarity adaptors with single-polarity tests. However we found that (i) TAEs using one-alternating-polarity adaptors with all-white tests were relatively small, increased slightly for two-alternating-polarity adaptors, and were largest with all-white or all-black adaptors. (ii) however TAEs were relatively large when the test was one-alternating polarity, irrespective of the adaptor type. (iii) The results with orientation closely mirrored those obtained with polarity with the difference that the TAE transfer across orthogonal orientations was weak. Taken together, our results demonstrate that the TAE for lines is mediated by a global shape mechanism that integrates the parts of lines into whole prior to the site of orientation adaptation. The asymmetry in the magnitude of TAE depending on whether the alternating-polarity lines was the adaptor or test can be explained by an imbalance in the population of neurons sensitive to 1st-and 2nd-order lines, with the 2nd-order lines being encoded by a subset of the mechanisms sensitive to 1st-order lines.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gheorghiu
- University of Stirling, Department of Psychology, Stirling, Scotland, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Jason Bell
- Research School of Psychology, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
- School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Gheorghiu E, Kingdom FA, Witney E. Size and shape after-effects: Same or different mechanism? Vision Res 2010; 50:2127-36. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2010] [Revised: 07/30/2010] [Accepted: 08/04/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
9
|
Lin Z, He S. Seeing the invisible: the scope and limits of unconscious processing in binocular rivalry. Prog Neurobiol 2009; 87:195-211. [PMID: 18824061 PMCID: PMC2689366 DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 120] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/02/2008] [Revised: 07/03/2008] [Accepted: 09/02/2008] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
When an image is presented to one eye and a very different image is presented to the corresponding location of the other eye, the two images compete for conscious representations, such that only one image is visible at a time while the other is suppressed. Called binocular rivalry, this phenomenon and its deviants have been extensively exploited to study the mechanism and neural correlates of consciousness. In this paper, we propose a framework - the unconscious binding hypothesis - to distinguish unconscious processing from conscious processing. According to this framework, the unconscious mind not only encodes individual features but also temporally binds distributed features to give rise to cortical representations; unlike conscious binding, however, unconscious binding is fragile. Under this framework, we review evidence from psychophysical and neuroimaging studies and come to two important conclusions. First, processing of invisible features depends on the "level" of the features as defined by their neural mechanisms. For low-level simple features, prolonged exposure to visual patterns (e.g. tilt) and simple translational motion can alter the appearance of subsequent visible features (i.e. adaptation). For invisible high-level features, complex spiral motion cannot produce adaptation, nor can objects/words enhance subsequent processing of related stimuli (i.e. priming). Yet images of tools can activate the dorsal pathway. Second, processing of invisible features has functional significance. Although invisible central cues cannot orient attention, invisible erotic pictures in the periphery can nevertheless guide attention, likely through emotional arousal; reciprocally, the processing of invisible information can be modulated by attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhicheng Lin
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 E. River Rd., Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Gheorghiu E, Kingdom FAA. The spatial feature underlying the shape-frequency and shape-amplitude after-effects. Vision Res 2007; 47:834-44. [PMID: 17292437 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.11.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2006] [Revised: 11/06/2006] [Accepted: 11/07/2006] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The shape-frequency and shape-amplitude after-effects, or SFAE and SAAE, refer respectively to the shifts observed in the perceived shape-frequency and shape-amplitude of a sinusoidal test contour following adaptation to a similar-shaped contour. As with other shape after-effects the shifts are in a direction away from that of the adapting stimulus. Using a variety of procedures we tested whether the spatial feature that was adapted in the SFAE and SAAE was (a) local orientation, (b) average unsigned curvature, (c) periodicity/density, (d) shape-amplitude and (e) local curvature. Our results suggest that the last of these, local curvature, underlies both the SFAE and SAAE. The evidence in favour of local curvature was that the after-effect reached its maximum value when just half-a-cycle of the test contour, in +/-cosine phase, was present. We suggest that the SFAE and SAAE are mediated by intermediate-level mechanisms that encode the shapes of contour fragments with constant sign of curvature. Given the neurophysiological evidence that neurons in area V4 encode parts of shapes with constant sign of curvature, we suggest V4 is the likely neural substrate for both the SFAE and SAAE.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Gheorghiu
- McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, 687 Pine Avenue West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1A1.
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Spivey MJ, Spirn MJ. Selective visual attention modulates the direct tilt aftereffect. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 2000; 62:1525-33. [PMID: 11140176 DOI: 10.3758/bf03212153] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
One's being able to allocate attention to particular regions or properties of the visual field is fundamental to visual information processing. Visual attention determines what input is carefully analyzed and what input is more or less ignored. But at what stage of the visual system is this process evident? We describe three experiments that demonstrate an effect of voluntary spatial attention and voluntary object-based attention on an orientation illusion (the tilt aftereffect) that is believed to take place in primary visual cortex. This finding, in which selective visual attention influences adaptation to visual orientation information, contributes to mounting evidence for a view of visual perception in which mutual interaction takes place between high-level and low-level subsystems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M J Spivey
- Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
A key emergent property of the primary visual cortex (V1) is the orientation selectivity of its neurons. The extent to which adult visual cortical neurons can exhibit changes in orientation selectivity is unknown. Here we use single-unit recording and intrinsic signal imaging in V1 of adult cats to demonstrate systematic repulsive shifts in orientation preference following short-term exposure (adaptation) to one stimulus orientation. In contrast to the common view of adaptation as a passive process by which responses around the adapting orientation are reduced, we show that changes in orientation tuning also occur due to response increases at orientations away from the adapting stimulus. Adaptation-induced orientation plasticity is thus an active time-dependent process that involves network interactions and includes both response depression and enhancement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V Dragoi
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge 02139, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Spillmann L. From elements to perception: local and global processing in visual neurons. Perception 2000; 28:1461-92. [PMID: 10793882 DOI: 10.1068/p2763] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Gestalt psychologists in the early part of the century challenged psychophysical notions that perceptual phenomena can be understood from a punctate (atomistic) analysis of the elements present in the stimulus. Their ideas slowed later attempts to explain vision in terms of single-cell recordings from individual neurons. A rapprochement between Gestalt phenomenology and neurophysiology seemed unlikely when the first ECVP was held in Marburg, Germany, in 1978. Since that time, response properties of neurons have been discovered that invite an interpretation of visual phenomena (including illusions) in terms of neuronal processing by long-range interactions, as first proposed by Mach and Hering in the last century. This article traces a personal journey into the early days of neurophysiological vision research to illustrate the progress that has taken place from the first attempts to correlate single-cell responses with visual perceptions. Whereas initially the receptive-field properties of individual classes of cells--e.g., contrast, wavelength, orientation, motion, disparity, and spatial-frequency detectors--were used to account for relatively simple visual phenomena, nowadays complex perceptions are interpreted in terms of long-range interactions, involving many neurons. This change in paradigm from local to global processing was made possible by recent findings, in the cortex, on horizontal interactions and backward propagation (feedback loops) in addition to classical feedforward processing. These mechanisms are exemplified by studies of the tilt effect and tilt aftereffect, direction-specific motion adaptation, illusory contours, filling-in and fading, figure--ground segregation by orientation and motion contrast, and pop-out in dynamic visual-noise patterns. Major questions for future research and a discussion of their epistemological implications conclude the article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Spillmann
- Institute of Biophysics and Radiation Biology, University of Freiburg, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
We investigated whether the size aftereffect (apparent spatial-frequency shift after adaptation to slightly different frequencies) is direction selective; i.e., whether it is stronger for test stimuli moving in the adapting direction than the opposite direction. We used drifting sinusoidal gratings of various spatiotemporal frequencies for both adaptation and test stimuli, and the perceived test frequency was estimated by means of a matching technique with a staircase method. For the purpose of comparison, the post-adaptation threshold elevation was measured in addition to the size aftereffect. The results revealed that the direction of stimuli had no influence on the magnitude of the size aftereffect for a wide range of spatiotemporal frequencies, whereas the post-adaptation threshold elevation showed clear direction selectivity. Although there was a significant direction selectivity for the size aftereffect at low spatial and high temporal frequencies, the selectivity was much weaker than that seen in the threshold elevation data. These findings are discussed in relation to the validity of a unified account of selective adaptation at and above threshold contrast and the notion of the separate processing of pattern and motion information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Nishida
- Human and Information Science Research Laboratory, NTT Communication Science Laboratories, Kanagawa, Japan.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Lieb K, Brucker S, Bach M, Els T, Lücking CH, Greenlee MW. Impairment in preattentive visual processing in patients with Parkinson's disease. Brain 1999; 122 ( Pt 2):303-13. [PMID: 10071058 DOI: 10.1093/brain/122.2.303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
We explored the possibility of whether preattentive visual processing is impaired in Parkinson's disease. With this aim, visual discrimination thresholds for orientation texture stimuli were determined in two separate measurement sessions in 16 patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. The results were compared with those of 16 control subjects age-matched and 16 young healthy volunteers. Discrimination thresholds were measured in a four-alternative spatial forced-choice paradigm, in which subjects judged the location of a target embedded in a background of distractors. Four different stimulus configurations were employed: (i) a group of vertical targets among horizontal distractors ('vertical line targets'); (ii) targets with varying levels of orientation difference on a background of spatially filtered vertically oriented noise ('Gaussian filtered noise'); (iii) one 'L' among 43 '+' signs ('texton'), all of which assess preattentive visual processing; and (iv) control condition, of one 'L' among 43 'T' distractors ('non-texton' search target), which reflects attentive visual processing. In two of the preattentive tasks (filtered noise and texton), patients with Parkinson's disease required significantly greater orientation differences and longer stimulus durations, respectively. In contrast, their performance in the vertical line target and non-texton search target was comparable to that of the matched control subjects. These differences were more pronounced in the first compared with the second session. Duration of illness and age within the patient group correlated significantly with test performance. In all conditions tested, the young control subjects performed significantly better than the more elderly control group, further indicating an effect of age on this form of visual processing. The results suggest that, in addition to the well documented impairment in retinal processing, idiopathic Parkinson's disease is associated with a deficit in preattentive cortical visual processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Lieb
- Neurologische Universitätsklinik, Universität Freiburg, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
A new method to study the tuning of orientation detectors in the human visual system is proposed. The stimulus consists of a sequence of flashed sinusoidal gratings of random orientations and spatial phases shown at a fast presentation rate. The subject's task is to report, as fast as possible, when the presence of a particular orientation (horizontal, vertical, or oblique) is seen in the stimulus sequence by pressing a button. The data are analyzed by calculating the empirical distribution of orientations present in the stimulus sequence within an optimal time-window before the button was pressed. The resulting orientation distributions show a "Mexican hat" shape, which resembles the distributions obtained in some single neurons of monkey primary visual cortex using a similar method (Ringach et al., 1997). The findings are consistent with the idea of "lateral inhibition" between neighboring detectors in the orientation domain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D L Ringach
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, NY 10003, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Zenger B, Sagi D. Isolating excitatory and inhibitory nonlinear spatial interactions involved in contrast detection. Vision Res 1996; 36:2497-513. [PMID: 8917811 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00303-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
Interactions between filters tuned to different orientations and spatial locations were investigated with a masking paradigm. Targets were masked by pairs of Gabor signals presented either at a different orientation (+/- delta theta) or at a different spatial location (+/- delta y). The two mask components were either of equal phase or of opposite phase to each other. Detection thresholds of the target were measured as a function of mask contrast. Typically, the curves obtained showed the following behavior: for increasing mask contrast the threshold first decreased, then reached a minimum and then increased linearly on a log-log scale reflecting a power-law behavior. Mask pairs of equal phase as well as pairs of opposite phase were shown to facilitate detection. Facilitation by mask pairs of equal phase was larger (up to 0.4 log units) and decreased for increasing delta theta and delta y. The facilitation for mask pairs of opposite phase (approximately 0.1 log units) was observed only for larger delta theta and delta y. Phase independent suppression was observed with higher mask contrasts at smaller delta theta and delta y. The strength of this suppression was shown to decrease with practice. We account for the observed facilitation with an accelerating transducer function applied on a second-stage filter. Suppression is modeled with an additional inhibitory second stage filter that divides the output of this transducer. Selective reduction of the inhibitory gain accounts for the practice effects.
Collapse
|
18
|
Affiliation(s)
- R Rosness
- Vision Laboratory, Institute of Psychology, University of Oslo, Blindern, Norway
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Sloane ME, Ost JW, Etheriedge DB, Henderlite SE. Overprediction and blocking in the McCollough aftereffect. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1989; 45:110-20. [PMID: 2928072 DOI: 10.3758/bf03208045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
The Rescorla-Wagner theory (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972) of associative learning offers specific predictions about the associative strength of CS-US pairs when two or more CSs are conditioned to the same US separately and subsequently paired in a compound with the same US. The magnitude of orientation-contingent color aftereffects (AEs) was used as an index of associative strength in this study. The results of experiments using an "overprediction" (Rescorla, 1970) and a "blocking" (Kamin, 1969) paradigm conformed to the predictions of the Rescorla-Wagner theory. In Experiment 1, AEs were established simultaneously for horizontal-vertical and diagonal patterns. When observers subsequently viewed compound induction patterns, AE magnitude was found to be significantly decreased, relative to a condition in which observers did not view such an induction pattern. In Experiment 2, AE magnitude for a given test pattern following inspection of compound induction stimuli was significantly reduced by inspection of the other component prior to viewing the compound induction stimuli. The applicability of associative learning and feature-adaptation models of the McCollough effect is discussed.
Collapse
|
20
|
Greenlee MW, Magnussen S. Interactions among spatial frequency and orientation channels adapted concurrently. Vision Res 1988; 28:1303-10. [PMID: 3256147 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90061-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
Interactions between size and orientation-specific mechanisms in the human visual system were investigated using a sequential adaptation technique. Subjects adapted to a vertical, 4 c/deg high-contrast (0.7) sinewave grating that was interleaved at a rate of 0.5 Hz with another adapting grating differing either in (1) spatial frequency or (2) orientation. Before and after adaptation contrast thresholds were measured for a vertical 4 c/deg sinewave test grating. The resultant elevation in contrast threshold was plotted as a function of the (1) spatial frequency or (2) orientation differences between the first and second adapting gratings. Maximum threshold elevation was found when both adapting gratings shared the same spatial frequency and orientation. Minimum elevations were found when the second grating's spatial frequency or orientation differed by approx. 1.5 octaves or 45 deg, respectively. Beyond these values threshold elevations reapproached the baseline value measured in a control condition, where the 4.0 c/deg adapting grating was interleaved with a blank. The minimum threshold elevations were 0.2-0.3 log units below the baseline level. The results suggest the existence of inhibitory interactions between neural mechanisms tuned to the size and orientation of retinal images.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M W Greenlee
- Neurologische Universitätsklinik mit Abteilung für Neurophysiologie, Freiburg, F.R.G
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
Exposure to a vernier offset can cause a subsequently viewed straight contour to appear offset in the opposite direction. The size of this vernier aftereffect (VAE) varies systematically with the size of the adapting offset. The VAE may be a version of the tilt aftereffect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J M Wolfe
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
|
23
|
Jordan K, Uhlarik J. Length contrast in the Müller-Lyer figure: functional equivalence of temporal and spatial separation. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1986; 39:267-74. [PMID: 3737355 DOI: 10.3758/bf03204934] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
|
24
|
Abstract
Growth and decay characteristics of the tilt aftereffect were studied for aftereffects induced by normal or continuous adaptation routines, and for aftereffects induced by successive or spaced adaptation to the same or different orientations on an adapt-partial decay-readapt schedule. In the continuous adaptation condition, growth and decay of the aftereffect were logarithmic functions of time. There was no evidence for saturation after 30 min adaptation. Aftereffect decay following spaced adaptation progresses as by continuous adaptation, but an adapting stimulus introduced during recovery from previous adaptation is more effective on the time scale than when introduced to a fully recovered system, summing approximately linearly with the residual aftereffect and off-setting the recovery process to zero. A second adapting stimulus whose orientation is of opposite sign (ccw vs cw) induces a two-phased decay process consisting of an early cancellation and a later enhancement of the original aftereffect. A two-stage model of adaptation is proposed.
Collapse
|
25
|
Vaitkevicius H, Karalius M, Meskauskas A, Sinius J, Sokolov E. A model for the monocular line orientation analyzer. BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS 1983; 48:139-147. [PMID: 6639978 DOI: 10.1007/bf00318081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/21/2023]
Abstract
A model for monocular line perception by human Ss is based on three basic assumptions: (a) the line's inclination is coded by the maximally excited orientation detector's number; (b) the inclination of the perceived line is equivocally determined by the excitation vector in the subjective space; (c) the analyzer has a maximum differential sensitivity over the whole range of the line inclinations. This simple model for the line inclination analyzer, taking into account the optimization of its sensitivity, provides incomplex explanations for a wide range of psychophysical and neurophysiological data obtained from human and animal experiments.
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
The perceived orientation of a test grating is rotated from its veridical orientation if an annulus grating with a similar orientation is present. The magnitude of this misperception was measured and found to be greater when the two gratings moved in the same direction than when they moved in opposite directions. This demonstration of a direction-specific tilt illusion is similar to the previously demonstrated direction-specific tilt aftereffect--which is to be expected if similar mechanisms are responsible for both phenomena. If the tilt illusion is caused by lateral inhibition between orientation-selective units, then these findings indicate that such inhibition is principally between units with similar orientation and direction of motion selectivities.
Collapse
|
27
|
Abstract
In the tilt aftereffect a grating or bar is perceived as being slightly rotated from its veridical orientation if it is preceded by a similar adaptation stimulus with a slightly different orientation. It has been reported that the tilt aftereffect is not direction specific. That is, the magnitude of the misperception was not affected by whether the adaptation and test stimuli were moving in the same or the opposite directions. However, when we required subjects to fixate on a stationary spot during adaptation to a moving grating, the tilt aftereffect was strongest when both stimuli moved in the same direction. Moreover, the tilt aftereffect was not direction specific without such fixation. These results are consistent with the distribution shift model in which the perceived orientation reflects the distribution of orientation selective units, some of which are also direction selective.
Collapse
|
28
|
Skottun BC, Johnsen T, Magnussen S. Tilt aftereffect with small adapting angles. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1981; 30:199-200. [PMID: 7301521 DOI: 10.3758/bf03204479] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
|
29
|
Long NR, Scheirlinck JG. Spatial disinhibition of orientation analyzers. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1981; 29:212-6. [PMID: 7267272 DOI: 10.3758/bf03207287] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
|