1
|
Rossi C, Roemmich RT, Bastian AJ. Understanding mechanisms of generalization following locomotor adaptation. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2024; 9:48. [PMID: 39043679 PMCID: PMC11266392 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-024-00258-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2023] [Accepted: 06/26/2024] [Indexed: 07/25/2024]
Abstract
Our nervous system has the remarkable ability to adapt our gait to accommodate changes in our body or surroundings. However, our adapted walking patterns often generalize only partially (or not at all) between different contexts. Here, we sought to understand how the nervous system generalizes adapted gait patterns from one context to another. Through a series of split-belt treadmill walking experiments, we evaluated different mechanistic hypotheses to explain the partial generalization of adapted gait patterns from split-belt treadmill to overground walking. In support of the credit assignment hypothesis, our experiments revealed the central finding that adaptation involves recalibration of two distinct forward models. Recalibration of the first model generalizes to overground walking, suggesting that the model represents the general movement dynamics of our body. On the other hand, recalibration of the second model does not generalize to overground walking, suggesting the model represents dynamics specific to treadmill walking. These findings reveal that there is a predefined portion of forward model recalibration that generalizes across context, leading to overall partial generalization of walking adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristina Rossi
- Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| | - Ryan T Roemmich
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| | - Amy J Bastian
- Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA.
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Wang Y, Huynh AT, Bao S, Buchanan JJ, Wright DL, Lei Y. Memory consolidation of sequence learning and dynamic adaptation during wakefulness. Cereb Cortex 2024; 34:bhad507. [PMID: 38185987 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhad507] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2023] [Revised: 12/04/2023] [Accepted: 12/15/2023] [Indexed: 01/09/2024] Open
Abstract
Motor learning involves acquiring new movement sequences and adapting motor commands to novel conditions. Labile motor memories, acquired through sequence learning and dynamic adaptation, undergo a consolidation process during wakefulness after initial training. This process stabilizes the new memories, leading to long-term memory formation. However, it remains unclear if the consolidation processes underlying sequence learning and dynamic adaptation are independent and if distinct neural regions underpin memory consolidation associated with sequence learning and dynamic adaptation. Here, we first demonstrated that the initially labile memories formed during sequence learning and dynamic adaptation were stabilized against interference through time-dependent consolidation processes occurring during wakefulness. Furthermore, we found that sequence learning memory was not disrupted when immediately followed by dynamic adaptation and vice versa, indicating distinct mechanisms for sequence learning and dynamic adaptation consolidation. Finally, by applying patterned transcranial magnetic stimulation to selectively disrupt the activity in the primary motor (M1) or sensory (S1) cortices immediately after sequence learning or dynamic adaptation, we found that sequence learning consolidation depended on M1 but not S1, while dynamic adaptation consolidation relied on S1 but not M1. For the first time in a single experimental framework, this study revealed distinct neural underpinnings for sequence learning and dynamic adaptation consolidation during wakefulness, with significant implications for motor skill enhancement and rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yiyu Wang
- Program of Motor Neuroscience, Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, United States
| | - Angelina T Huynh
- Program of Motor Neuroscience, Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, United States
| | - Shancheng Bao
- Program of Motor Neuroscience, Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, United States
| | - John J Buchanan
- Program of Motor Neuroscience, Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, United States
| | - David L Wright
- Program of Motor Neuroscience, Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, United States
| | - Yuming Lei
- Program of Motor Neuroscience, Department of Kinesiology & Sport Management, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843, United States
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Wood JM, Morton SM, Kim HE. A reliable and efficient adaptive Bayesian method to assess static lower limb position sense. J Neurosci Methods 2023; 392:109875. [PMID: 37150304 PMCID: PMC10285506 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2023.109875] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2023] [Revised: 04/14/2023] [Accepted: 05/04/2023] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Lower limb proprioception is critical for maintaining stability during gait and may impact how individuals modify their movements in response to changes in the environment and body state, a process termed "sensorimotor adaptation". However, the connection between lower limb proprioception and sensorimotor adaptation during human gait has not been established. We suspect this gap is due in part to the lack of reliable, efficient methods to assess global lower limb proprioception in an ecologically valid context. NEW METHOD We assessed static lower limb proprioception using an alternative forced choice task, administered twice to determine test-retest reliability. Participants stood on a dual-belt treadmill which passively moved one limb to stimulus locations selected by a Bayesian adaptive algorithm. At the stimulus locations, participants judged relative foot positions and the algorithm estimated the point of subjective equality (PSE) and the uncertainty of lower limb proprioception. RESULTS Using the Bland-Altman method, combined with Bayesian statistics, we found that both the PSE and uncertainty estimates had good reliability. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHOD(S) Current methods assessing static lower limb proprioception do so within a single joint, in non-weight bearing positions, and rely heavily on memory. One exception assessed static lower limb proprioception in standing but did not measure reliability and contained confounds impacting participants' judgments, which we experimentally controlled here. CONCLUSIONS This efficient and reliable method assessing lower limb proprioception will aid future mechanistic understanding of locomotor adaptation and serve as a useful tool for basic and clinical researchers studying balance and falls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan M Wood
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States; Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States.
| | - Susanne M Morton
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States; Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
| | - Hyosub E Kim
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States; Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States; Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, United States; School of Kinesiology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Wood JM, Morton SM, Kim HE. A reliable and efficient adaptive Bayesian method to assess static lower limb position sense. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.01.23.525102. [PMID: 36747823 PMCID: PMC9900742 DOI: 10.1101/2023.01.23.525102] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Background Lower limb proprioception is critical for maintaining stability during gait and may impact how individuals modify their movements in response to changes in the environment and body state, a process termed "sensorimotor adaptation". However, the connection between lower limb proprioception and sensorimotor adaptation during human gait has not been established. We suspect this gap is due in part to the lack of reliable, efficient methods to assess global lower limb proprioception in an ecologically valid context. New Method We assessed static lower limb proprioception using an alternative forced choice task, administered twice to determine test-retest reliability. Participants stood on a dual-belt treadmill which passively moved one limb to stimulus locations selected by a Bayesian adaptive algorithm. At the stimulus locations, participants judged relative foot positions and the algorithm estimated the point of subjective equality (PSE) and the uncertainty of lower limb proprioception. Results Using the Bland-Altman method, combined with Bayesian statistics, we found that both the PSE and uncertainty estimates had good reliability. Comparison with Existing Methods Current methods assessing static lower limb proprioception do so within a single joint, in non-weight bearing positions, and rely heavily on memory. One exception assessed static lower limb proprioception in standing but did not measure reliability and contained confounds impacting participants' judgments, which we experimentally controlled here. Conclusions This efficient and reliable method assessing lower limb proprioception will aid future mechanistic understanding of locomotor adaptation and serve as a useful tool for basic and clinical researchers studying balance and falls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan M Wood
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
- Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
| | - Susanne M Morton
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
- Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
| | - Hyosub E Kim
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
- Biomechanics and Movement Sciences Program, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19711, United States
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, United States
- School of Kinesiology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Table Tennis, as a Method of Sensorimotor Training, Induces Haptic and Motor Gains in Children With a Probable Developmental Coordination Disorder. Motor Control 2023:1-18. [PMID: 36669507 DOI: 10.1123/mc.2021-0123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2021] [Revised: 11/01/2022] [Accepted: 11/12/2022] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
This study examined whether table tennis as a method of sensorimotor training improves haptic and motor function and to what extent haptic function gain correlates with changes in motor ability in children with probable developmental coordination disorder (pDCD). Children with pDCD were randomly assigned to the table tennis and nontraining control groups. The children in the table tennis group received 36 sessions of table tennis training, including ball balancing, hitting the ball against the wall, strokes, and serving. Haptic sensitivity, acuity, and motor function domains were measured. The results showed a 41.5% improvement in haptic sensitivity in children exposed to table tennis training compared with 2.8% in those without training. This improved haptic sensitivity significantly correlated with motor function gain, suggesting that somatosensory gains occur simultaneously with changes in motor function in children with pDCD. This novel upper limb motor training approach may be an interesting method of sensorimotor training in neurological rehabilitation in children with pDCD.
Collapse
|
6
|
Tsay JS, Kim H, Haith AM, Ivry RB. Understanding implicit sensorimotor adaptation as a process of proprioceptive re-alignment. eLife 2022; 11:e76639. [PMID: 35969491 PMCID: PMC9377801 DOI: 10.7554/elife.76639] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2021] [Accepted: 07/13/2022] [Indexed: 01/11/2023] Open
Abstract
Multiple learning processes contribute to successful goal-directed actions in the face of changing physiological states, biomechanical constraints, and environmental contexts. Amongst these processes, implicit sensorimotor adaptation is of primary importance, ensuring that movements remain well-calibrated and accurate. A large body of work on reaching movements has emphasized how adaptation centers on an iterative process designed to minimize visual errors. The role of proprioception has been largely neglected, thought to play a passive role in which proprioception is affected by the visual error but does not directly contribute to adaptation. Here, we present an alternative to this visuo-centric framework, outlining a model in which implicit adaptation acts to minimize a proprioceptive error, the distance between the perceived hand position and its intended goal. This proprioceptive re-alignment model (PReMo) is consistent with many phenomena that have previously been interpreted in terms of learning from visual errors, and offers a parsimonious account of numerous unexplained phenomena. Cognizant that the evidence for PReMo rests on correlational studies, we highlight core predictions to be tested in future experiments, as well as note potential challenges for a proprioceptive-based perspective on implicit adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan S Tsay
- Department of Psychology, University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
| | - Hyosub Kim
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of DelawareNewarkUnited States
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of DelawareNewarkUnited States
| | - Adrian M Haith
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins UniversityBaltimoreUnited States
| | - Richard B Ivry
- Department of Psychology, University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Ishikawa R, Ayabe-Kanamura S, Izawa J. The role of motor memory dynamics in structuring bodily self-consciousness. iScience 2021; 24:103511. [PMID: 34934929 PMCID: PMC8661550 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.103511] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2021] [Revised: 10/21/2021] [Accepted: 11/22/2021] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Bodily self-consciousness has been considered a sensorimotor root of self-consciousness. If this is the case, how does sensorimotor memory, which is important for the prediction of sensory consequences of volitional actions, influence awareness of bodily self-consciousness? This question is essential for understanding the effective acquisition and recovery of self-consciousness following its impairment, but it has remained unexamined. Here, we investigated how body ownership and agency recovered following body schema distortion in a virtual reality environment along with two kinds of motor memories: memories that were rapidly updated and memories that were gradually updated. We found that, although agency and body ownership recovered in parallel, the recovery of body ownership was predicted by fast memories and that of agency was predicted by slow memories. Thus, the bodily self was represented in multiple motor memories with different dynamics. This finding demystifies the controversy about the causal relationship between body ownership and agency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ryota Ishikawa
- Ph.D. Program in Humanics, University of Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8573, Japan
| | | | - Jun Izawa
- Faculty of Engineering, Information, and Systems, University of Tsukuba, 1-1-1 Tennodai, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8573, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Sedda G, Ostry DJ, Sanguineti V, Sabatini SP. Self-operated stimuli improve subsequent visual motion integration. J Vis 2021; 21:13. [PMID: 34529006 PMCID: PMC8447044 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.10.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Evidences of perceptual changes that accompany motor activity have been limited primarily to audition and somatosensation. Here we asked whether motor learning results in changes to visual motion perception. We designed a reaching task in which participants were trained to make movements along several directions, while the visual feedback was provided by an intrinsically ambiguous moving stimulus directly tied to hand motion. We find that training improves coherent motion perception and that changes in movement are correlated with perceptual changes. No perceptual changes are observed in passive training even when observers were provided with an explicit strategy to facilitate single motion perception. A Bayesian model suggests that movement training promotes the fine-tuning of the internal representation of stimulus geometry. These results emphasize the role of sensorimotor interaction in determining the persistent properties in space and time that define a percept.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giulia Sedda
- Department of Informatics, Bioengineering, Robotics and Systems Engineering, University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy.,
| | - David J Ostry
- Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada.,Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA.,
| | - Vittorio Sanguineti
- Department of Informatics, Bioengineering, Robotics and Systems Engineering, University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy.,
| | - Silvio P Sabatini
- Department of Informatics, Bioengineering, Robotics and Systems Engineering, University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy.,
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Flix-Díez L, Delicado-Miralles M, Gurdiel-Álvarez F, Velasco E, Galán-Calle M, Lerma Lara S. Reversed Polarity bi-tDCS over M1 during a Five Days Motor Task Training Did Not Influence Motor Learning. A Triple-Blind Clinical Trial. Brain Sci 2021; 11:brainsci11060691. [PMID: 34070256 PMCID: PMC8225177 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci11060691] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2021] [Revised: 05/18/2021] [Accepted: 05/22/2021] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been investigated as a way of improving motor learning. Our purpose was to explore the reversal bilateral tDCS effects on manual dexterity training, during five days, with the retention component measured after 5 days to determine whether somatosensory effects were produced. In this randomized, triple-blind clinical trial, 28 healthy subjects (14 women) were recruited and randomized into tDCS and placebo groups, although only 23 participants (13 women) finished the complete protocol. Participants received the real or placebo treatment during five consecutive days, while performing a motor dexterity training program of 20 min. The motor dexterity and the sensitivity of the hand were assessed pre- and post-day 1, post 5 days of training, and 5 days after training concluded. Training improved motor dexterity, but tDCS only produced a tendency to improve retention. The intervention did not produce changes in the somatosensory variables assessed. Thus, reversal bi-tDCS had no effects during motor learning on healthy subjects, but it could favor the retention of the motor skills acquired. These results do not support the cooperative inter-hemispheric model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Flix-Díez
- Department of Physical Therapy, University of Valencia (UV), 46003 Valencia, Spain;
| | - Miguel Delicado-Miralles
- Instituto de Neurociencias de Alicante (UMH-CSIC), 03550 Sant Joant d’Alacant, Spain; (M.D.-M.); (E.V.)
| | - Francisco Gurdiel-Álvarez
- Department of Physical Therapy, Occupational Therapy, Rehabilitation and Physical Medicine University of Rey Juan Carlos, 28922 Alcorcón, Spain;
| | - Enrique Velasco
- Instituto de Neurociencias de Alicante (UMH-CSIC), 03550 Sant Joant d’Alacant, Spain; (M.D.-M.); (E.V.)
| | - María Galán-Calle
- Health Sciences Faculty, Centro Superior de Estudios Universitarios La Salle, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, 28023 Madrid, Spain;
| | - Sergio Lerma Lara
- Health Sciences Faculty, Centro Superior de Estudios Universitarios La Salle, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, 28023 Madrid, Spain;
- Motion in Brains Research Group, Centro Superior de Estudios Universitarios La Salle, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, 28023 Madrid, Spain
- Correspondence: ; Tel.: +34-91-5035900 (ext. 255)
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
|
11
|
Kitchen NM, Miall RC. Adaptation of reach action to a novel force-field is not predicted by acuity of dynamic proprioception in either older or younger adults. Exp Brain Res 2020; 239:557-574. [PMID: 33315127 PMCID: PMC7936968 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05997-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2020] [Accepted: 11/24/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Healthy ageing involves degeneration of the neuromuscular system which impacts movement control and proprioception. Yet the relationship between these sensory and motor deficits in upper limb reaching has not been examined in detail. Recently, we reported that age-related proprioceptive deficits were unrelated to accuracy in rapid arm movements, but whether this applied in motor tasks more heavily dependent on proprioceptive feedback was not clear. To address this, we have tested groups of younger and older adults on a force-field adaptation task under either full or limited visual feedback conditions and examined how performance was related to dynamic proprioceptive acuity. Adaptive performance was similar between the age groups, regardless of visual feedback condition, although older adults showed increased after-effects. Physically inactive individuals made larger systematic (but not variable) proprioceptive errors, irrespective of age. However, dynamic proprioceptive acuity was unrelated to adaptation and there was no consistent evidence of proprioceptive recalibration with adaptation to the force-field for any group. Finally, in spite of clear age-dependent loss of spatial working memory capacity, we found no relationship between memory capacity and adaptive performance or proprioceptive acuity. Thus, non-clinical levels of deficit in dynamic proprioception, due to age or physical inactivity, do not affect force-field adaptation, even under conditions of limited visual feedback that might require greater proprioceptive control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nick M Kitchen
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK.
- Department of Speech and Hearing Science, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA.
| | - R Chris Miall
- School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Opposing force fields induce direction-specific sensorimotor adaptation but a non-specific perceptual shift consistent with a contraction of peripersonal space representation. Exp Brain Res 2020; 239:31-46. [PMID: 33097985 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05945-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2020] [Accepted: 10/06/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Most of our daily interactions with objects occur in the space immediately surrounding the body, i.e. the peripersonal space. The peripersonal space is characterized by multisensory processing of objects which are coded in terms of potential actions, specifying for instance whether objects are within reach or not. Our recent work suggested a link between exposure to a new force field, which changed the effector dynamics, and the representation of peripersonal space. To better understand the interplay between the plasticity of the motor system and peripersonal space representation, the present study examined whether changing the direction of the force field specifically modified the perception of action boundaries. Participants seated at the centre of an experimental platform estimated visual targets' reachability before and after adapting upper-limb reaching movements to the Coriolis force generated by either clockwise or counter clockwise rotation of the platform (120°/s). Opposite spatial after-effects were observed, showing that force-field adaptation depends on the direction of the rotation. In contrast, perceived action boundaries shifted leftward following exposure to the new force field, regardless of the direction of the rotation. Overall, these findings support the idea that abrupt exposure to a new force field results in a direction-specific updating of the central sensorimotor representations underlying the control of arm movements. Abrupt exposure to a new force field also results in a nonspecific shift in the perception of action boundaries, which is consistent with a contraction of the peripersonal space. Such effect, which does not appear to be related to state anxiety, could be related to the protective role of the peripersonal space in response to the uncertainty of the sensorimotor system induced by the abrupt modification of the environment.
Collapse
|
13
|
Avraham G, Sulimani E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Nisky I. Effects of visuomotor delays on the control of movement and on perceptual localization in the presence and absence of visual targets. J Neurophysiol 2019; 122:2259-2271. [PMID: 31577532 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00017.2019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The sensory system constantly deals with delayed feedback. Recent studies showed that playing a virtual game of pong with delayed feedback caused hypermetric reaching movements. We investigated whether this effect is associated with a perceptual bias. In addition, we examined the importance of the target in causing hypermetric movements. In a first experiment, participants played a delayed pong game and blindly reached to presented targets. Following each reaching movement, they assessed the position of the invisible cursor. We found that participants performed hypermetric movements but reported that the invisible cursor reached the target, suggesting that they were unaware of the hypermetria and that their perception was biased toward the target rather than toward their hand position. In a second experiment, we removed the visual target, and strikingly, the hypermetria vanished. Moreover, participants reported that the invisible cursor was located with their hand. Taking these results together, we conclude that the adaptation to the visuomotor delay during the pong game selectively affected the execution of goal directed movements, resulting in hypermetria and perceptual bias when movements are directed toward visual targets but not when such targets are absent.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Recent studies showed that adaptation to visuomotor delays causes hypermetric movements in the absence of visual feedback, suggesting that visuomotor delay is represented using current state information. We report that this adaptation also affects perception. Importantly, both the motor and perceptual effects are selective to the representations that are used in the execution of goal-directed movements toward visual targets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guy Avraham
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel.,Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California.,Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California
| | - Erez Sulimani
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel
| | - Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi
- Shirley Ryan AbilityLab, Chicago, Illinois.,Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
| | - Ilana Nisky
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Sombric C, Gonzalez-Rubio M, Torres-Oviedo G. Split-Belt walking induces changes in active, but not passive, perception of step length. Sci Rep 2019; 9:16442. [PMID: 31712598 PMCID: PMC6848101 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-52860-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2019] [Accepted: 10/23/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Successful motor control requires accurate estimation of our body in space for planning, executing, and evaluating the outcome of our actions. It has been shown that the estimation of limb position is susceptible to motor adaptation. However, a similar effect has not been found in locomotion, possibly due to how it was tested. We hypothesized that split-belt walking with the legs moving at different speeds changes the estimation of the legs' position when taking a step. Thus, we assessed young subjects' perception of step length (i.e., inter-feet distance at foot landing) when they moved their legs (active perception) or when the legs were moved by the experimenter (passive perception). We found that the active perception of step length was substantially altered following split-belt walking, whereas passive perception exhibited minor changes. This suggests that split-belt walking induced the adaptation of efferent signals, without altering sensory signals. We also found that active perceptual shifts were sensitive to how they were tested: they were most salient in the trailing leg and at short step lengths. Our results suggest that split-belt walking could modulate the deficient perception of step length post-stroke, which may contribute to gait asymmetries impairing patients' mobility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carly Sombric
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | | | - Gelsy Torres-Oviedo
- Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Avraham C, Dominitz M, Khait H, Avraham G, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Nisky I. Adaptation to Laterally Asymmetrical Visuomotor Delay Has an Effect on Action But Not on Perception. Front Hum Neurosci 2019; 13:312. [PMID: 31551739 PMCID: PMC6743346 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00312] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2018] [Accepted: 08/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
When interacting with the environment, the sensorimotor system faces temporal and spatial discrepancies between sensory inputs, such as delay in sensory information transmission, and asymmetrical visual inputs across space. These discrepancies can affect motor control and the representation of space. We recently showed that adaptation to a laterally asymmetric delay in the visual feedback induces neglect-like effects in blind drawing movements, expressed by asymmetrical elongation of circles that are drawn in different workspaces and directions; this establishes a possible connection between delayed feedback and asymmetrical spatial processing in the control of action. In the current study, we investigate whether such adaptation also influences visual perception. In addition, we examined transfer to another motor task – a line bisection task that is commonly used to detect spatial disorders, and extend these results to examine the mapping of these neglect-like effects. We performed two sets of experiments in which participants executed lateral reaching movements, and were exposed to visual feedback delay only in the left workspace. We examined transfer of adaptation to a perceptual line bisection task – answers about the perceived midline of lines that were presented in different directions and workspaces, and to a blind motor line bisection task – reaching movements toward the centers of similar lines. We found that the adaptation to the asymmetrical delay transferred to the control of lateral movements, but did not affect the perceived location of the midlines. Our results clarify the effect of asymmetrical delayed visual feedback on perception and action, and provide potential insights on the link between visuomotor delay and neurological disorders such as the hemispatial neglect syndrome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chen Avraham
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Mor Dominitz
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Hana Khait
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Guy Avraham
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States.,Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States
| | - Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi
- Department of Physiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States.,Department of Biomedical Engineering, McCormick School of Engineering and Applied Science, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States.,Shirley Ryan AbilityLab, Chicago, IL, United States
| | - Ilana Nisky
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Ohashi H, Gribble PL, Ostry DJ. Somatosensory cortical excitability changes precede those in motor cortex during human motor learning. J Neurophysiol 2019; 122:1397-1405. [PMID: 31390294 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00383.2019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor learning is associated with plasticity in both motor and somatosensory cortex. It is known from animal studies that tetanic stimulation to each of these areas individually induces long-term potentiation in its counterpart. In this context it is possible that changes in motor cortex contribute to somatosensory change and that changes in somatosensory cortex are involved in changes in motor areas of the brain. It is also possible that learning-related plasticity occurs in these areas independently. To better understand the relative contribution to human motor learning of motor cortical and somatosensory plasticity, we assessed the time course of changes in primary somatosensory and motor cortex excitability during motor skill learning. Learning was assessed using a force production task in which a target force profile varied from one trial to the next. The excitability of primary somatosensory cortex was measured using somatosensory evoked potentials in response to median nerve stimulation. The excitability of primary motor cortex was measured using motor evoked potentials elicited by single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation. These two measures were interleaved with blocks of motor learning trials. We found that the earliest changes in cortical excitability during learning occurred in somatosensory cortical responses, and these changes preceded changes in motor cortical excitability. Changes in somatosensory evoked potentials were correlated with behavioral measures of learning. Changes in motor evoked potentials were not. These findings indicate that plasticity in somatosensory cortex occurs as a part of the earliest stages of motor learning, before changes in motor cortex are observed.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We tracked somatosensory and motor cortical excitability during motor skill acquisition. Changes in both motor cortical and somatosensory excitability were observed during learning; however, the earliest changes were in somatosensory cortex, not motor cortex. Moreover, the earliest changes in somatosensory cortical excitability predict the extent of subsequent learning; those in motor cortex do not. This is consistent with the idea that plasticity in somatosensory cortex coincides with the earliest stages of human motor learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hiroki Ohashi
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut.,Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Paul L Gribble
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut.,The Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada.,Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada.,Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
| | - David J Ostry
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut.,Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Sexton BM, Liu Y, Block HJ. Increase in weighting of vision vs. proprioception associated with force field adaptation. Sci Rep 2019; 9:10167. [PMID: 31308399 PMCID: PMC6629615 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-46625-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2019] [Accepted: 07/02/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Hand position can be estimated by vision and proprioception (position sense). The brain is thought to weight and integrate these percepts to form a multisensory estimate of hand position with which to guide movement. Force field adaptation, a type of cerebellum-dependent motor learning, is associated with both motor and proprioceptive changes. The cerebellum has connections with multisensory parietal regions; however, it is unknown if force adaptation is associated with changes in multisensory perception. If force adaptation affects all relevant sensory modalities similarly, the brain’s weighting of vision vs. proprioception should be maintained. Alternatively, if force perturbation is interpreted as somatosensory unreliability, vision may be up-weighted relative to proprioception. We assessed visuo-proprioceptive weighting with a perceptual estimation task before and after subjects performed straight-ahead reaches grasping a robotic manipulandum. Each subject performed one session with a clockwise or counter-clockwise velocity-dependent force field, and one session in a null field. Subjects increased their weight of vision vs. proprioception in the force field session relative to the null session, regardless of force field direction, in the straight-ahead dimension (F1,44 = 5.13, p = 0.029). This suggests that force field adaptation is associated with an increase in the brain’s weighting of vision vs. proprioception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brandon M Sexton
- Department of Kinesiology & Program in Neuroscience, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, USA
| | - Yang Liu
- Department of Kinesiology & Program in Neuroscience, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, USA
| | - Hannah J Block
- Department of Kinesiology & Program in Neuroscience, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
The capacity to learn new motor and perceptual calibrations develops concurrently in childhood. Sci Rep 2019; 9:9322. [PMID: 31249379 PMCID: PMC6597729 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-45074-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2019] [Accepted: 05/29/2019] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Learning new movements through an error-based process called motor adaptation is thought to involve multiple mechanisms which are still largely not understood. Previous studies have shown that young children adapt movement more slowly than adults, perhaps supporting the involvement of distinct neural circuits that come online at different stages of development. Recent studies in adults have shown that in addition to recalibrating a movement, motor adaptation also leads to changes in the perception of that movement. However, we do not yet understand the relationship between the processes that underlie motor and perceptual recalibration. Here we studied motor and perceptual recalibration with split-belt walking adaptation in adults and children aged 6-8 years. Consistent with previous work, we found that this group of children adapted their walking patterns more slowly than adults, though individual children ranged from slow to adult-like in their adaptation rates. Perceptual recalibration was also reduced in the same group of children compared to adults, with individual children ranging from having no recalibration to having adult-like recalibration. In sum, faster motor adaptation and the ability to recalibrate movement perception both come online within a similar age-range, raising the possibility that the same sensorimotor mechanisms underlie these processes.
Collapse
|
19
|
Ohashi H, Valle-Mena R, Gribble PL, Ostry DJ. Movements following force-field adaptation are aligned with altered sense of limb position. Exp Brain Res 2019; 237:1303-1313. [PMID: 30863880 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-019-05509-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2018] [Accepted: 03/04/2019] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Abstract
Previous work has shown that motor learning is associated with changes to both movements and to the somatosensory perception of limb position. In an earlier study that motivates the current work, it appeared that following washout trials, movements did not return to baseline but rather were aligned with associated changes to sensed limb position. Here, we provide a systematic test of this relationship, examining the idea that adaptation-related changes to sensed limb position and to the path of the limb are linked, not only after washout trials but at all stages of the adaptation process. We used a force-field adaptation paradigm followed by washout trials in which subjects performed movements without visual feedback of the limb. Tests of sensed limb position were conducted at each phase of adaptation, specifically before and after baseline movements in a null field, after force-field adaptation, and following washout trials in a null field. As in previous work, sensed limb position changed in association with force-field adaptation. At each stage of adaptation, we observed a correlation between the sensed limb position and associated path of the limb. At a group level, there were differences between the clockwise and counter-clockwise conditions. However, whenever there were changes in sensed limb position, movements following washout did not return to baseline. This suggests that adaptation in sensory and motor systems is not independent processes but rather sensorimotor adaptation is linked to sensory change. Sensory change and limb movement remain in alignment throughout adaptation such that the path of the limb is aligned with the altered sense of limb position.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Paul L Gribble
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA.,Western University, London, ON, Canada
| | - David J Ostry
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA. .,Department of Psychology, McGill University, 2001 McGill College, Montreal, QC, H3A 1G1, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Making Sense of Cerebellar Contributions to Perceptual and Motor Adaptation. THE CEREBELLUM 2019; 17:111-121. [PMID: 28840476 DOI: 10.1007/s12311-017-0879-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
The cerebellum is thought to adapt movements to changes in the environment in order to update an implicit understanding of the association between our motor commands and their sensory consequences. This trial-by-trial motor recalibration in response to external perturbations is frequently impaired in people with cerebellar damage. In healthy people, adaptation to motor perturbations is also known to induce a form of sensory perceptual recalibration. For instance, hand-reaching adaptation tasks produce transient changes in the sense of hand position, and walking adaptation tasks can lead to changes in perceived leg speed. Though such motor adaptation tasks are heavily dependent on the cerebellum, it is not yet understood how the cerebellum is associated with these accompanying sensory recalibration processes. Here we asked if the cerebellum is required for the recalibration of leg-speed perception that normally occurs alongside locomotor adaptation, as well as how ataxia severity is related to sensorimotor recalibration deficits in patients with cerebellar damage. Cerebellar patients performed a speed-matching task to assess perception of leg speed before and after walking on a split-belt treadmill, which has two belts driving each leg at a different speed. Healthy participants update their perception of leg speed following split-belt walking such that the "fast" leg during adaptation feels slower afterwards, whereas cerebellar patients have significant deficits in this sensory perceptual recalibration. Furthermore, our analysis demonstrates that ataxia severity is a crucial factor for both the sensory and motor adaptation impairments that affect patients with cerebellar damage.
Collapse
|
21
|
Reuter EM, Mattingley JB, Cunnington R, Riek S, Carroll TJ. Pushing attention to one side: Force field adaptation alters neural correlates of orienting and disengagement of spatial attention. Eur J Neurosci 2018; 49:120-136. [PMID: 30408253 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14266] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2018] [Revised: 10/24/2018] [Accepted: 10/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Sensorimotor adaptation to wedge prisms can alter the balance of attention between left and right space in healthy adults, and improve symptoms of spatial neglect after stroke. Here we asked whether the orienting of spatial attention to visual stimuli is affected by a different form of sensorimotor adaptation that involves physical perturbations of arm movement, rather than distortion of visual feedback. Healthy participants performed a cued discrimination task before and after they made reaching movements to a central target. A velocity-dependent force field pushed the hand aside during each reach, and required participants to apply compensatory forces toward the opposite side. We used event-related potentials (ERPs) to determine whether electroencephalography (EEG) responses reflecting orienting (cue-locked N1) and disengagement (target-locked P1) of spatial attention are affected by adaptation to force fields. After adaptation, the cue-locked N1 was relatively larger for stimuli presented in the hemispace corresponding to the direction of compensatory hand force. P1 amplitudes evoked by invalidly cued targets presented on the opposite side were reduced. This suggests that force field adaptation boosted attentional orienting responses toward the side of hand forces, and impeded attentional disengagement from that side, mimicking previously reported effects of prism adaptation. Thus, remapping between motor commands and intended movement direction is sufficient to bias ERPs, reflecting changes in the orienting of spatial attention in the absence of visuo-spatial distortion or visuo-proprioceptive mismatch. Findings are relevant to theories of how sensorimotor adaptation can modulate attention, and may open new avenues for treatment of spatial neglect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva-Maria Reuter
- Centre for Sensorimotor Performance, School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia.,Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Jason B Mattingley
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia.,School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Ross Cunnington
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia.,School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Stephan Riek
- Centre for Sensorimotor Performance, School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Timothy J Carroll
- Centre for Sensorimotor Performance, School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Klingner CM, Brodoehl S, Witte OW, Guntinas-Lichius O, Volk GF. The impact of motor impairment on the processing of sensory information. Behav Brain Res 2018; 359:701-708. [PMID: 30248364 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.09.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2018] [Revised: 08/19/2018] [Accepted: 09/19/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Sensorimotor adaptation is driven by mismatch errors between desired movements and actual movement outcomes. A mismatch error can be minimized by adjusting movements or by altering the interpretation of sensory information. While the effect of mismatch errors on the motor system has received much attention, the contribution of somatosensory feedback, particularly the sensory-motor interplay in the process of adaptation, remains poorly understood. Our study analyzes the impact of peripheral deefferentation on the plasticity of the brain networks responsible for sensory-motor adaptation, focusing particularly on changes in the processing of somatosensory information. For this aim, task-based and resting-state functional MRI was performed on 24 patients in the acute state of a left-sided idiopathic peripheral facial nerve palsy. The functional connectivity of cortical and subcortical networks was analyzed and compared to a healthy control group. We found a strong involvement of the somatosensory system and the thalamus in the adaptation process following an acute peripheral deefferentation. The investigated network shows the principal pattern of a reduced connectivity between cortical areas, while the connectivity to subcortical areas (the basal ganglia and the thalamus) is increased. We suggest that the increased connectivity between the subcortical and cortical structures indicates an active sensory-motor adaptation process. We further hypothesize that the decreased functional connectivity at the cortical level reflects an unsuccessful sensorimotor adaptation process due to the inability to solve the somatosensory-motor mismatch. These results extend our understanding of the somatosensory-motor interaction in response to a mismatch signal and highlight the importance of the thalamus in this process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carsten M Klingner
- Hans Berger Department of Neurology, Jena University Hospital, Germany; Biomagnetic Center, University Hospital, Germany; Facial-Nerve-Center, Jena University Hospital, Germany.
| | - Stefan Brodoehl
- Hans Berger Department of Neurology, Jena University Hospital, Germany; Biomagnetic Center, University Hospital, Germany
| | - Otto W Witte
- Hans Berger Department of Neurology, Jena University Hospital, Germany
| | - Orlando Guntinas-Lichius
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Jena University Hospital, Germany; Facial-Nerve-Center, Jena University Hospital, Germany
| | - Gerd F Volk
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Jena University Hospital, Germany; Facial-Nerve-Center, Jena University Hospital, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Leech KA, Day KA, Roemmich RT, Bastian AJ. Movement and perception recalibrate differently across multiple days of locomotor learning. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:2130-2137. [PMID: 30183471 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00355.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Learning a new movement through error-based adaptation leads to recalibration of movement and altered perception of that movement. Although presumed to be closely related, the relationship between adaptation-based motor and perceptual changes is not well understood. Here we investigated the changes in motor behavior and leg speed perception over 5 days of split-belt treadmill adaptation. We specifically wanted to know if changes in the perceptual domain would demonstrate savings-like behavior (i.e., less recalibration with more practice) and if these changes would parallel the savings observed in the motor domain. We found that the recalibration of leg speed perception decreased across days of training, indicating savings-like behavior in this domain. However, we observed that the magnitude of savings across days was different between motor and perceptual domains. These findings suggest a degree of independence between the motor and perceptual processes that occur with locomotor adaptation. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Error-based adaptation learning drives changes in movement and perception of movement. Are these changes across domains linked or simply coincidental? Here, we studied changes in movement and perception across 5 days of repeated locomotor adaptation. Savings-like behavior in the motor and perceptual domains developed with different magnitudes and over different timescales, leading us to conclude that motor and perceptual processes operate at least somewhat independently during locomotor adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristan A Leech
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute , Baltimore, Maryland.,Department of Neuroscience, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine , Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Kevin A Day
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute , Baltimore, Maryland.,Department of Biomedical Engineering, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine , Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Ryan T Roemmich
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute , Baltimore, Maryland.,Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine , Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Amy J Bastian
- Center for Movement Studies, Kennedy Krieger Institute , Baltimore, Maryland.,Department of Neuroscience, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine , Baltimore, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Ruttle JE, 't Hart BM, Henriques DYP. The fast contribution of visual-proprioceptive discrepancy to reach aftereffects and proprioceptive recalibration. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0200621. [PMID: 30016356 PMCID: PMC6049908 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200621] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2017] [Accepted: 06/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Adapting reaches to altered visual feedback not only leads to motor changes, but also to shifts in perceived hand location; "proprioceptive recalibration". These changes are robust to many task variations and can occur quite rapidly. For instance, our previous study found both motor and sensory shifts arise in as few as 6 rotated-cursor training trials. The aim of this study is to investigate one of the training signals that contribute to these rapid sensory and motor changes. We do this by removing the visuomotor error signals associated with classic visuomotor rotation training; and provide only experience with a visual-proprioceptive discrepancy for training. While a force channel constrains reach direction 30o away from the target, the cursor representing the hand unerringly moves straight to the target. The resulting visual-proprioceptive discrepancy drives significant and rapid changes in no-cursor reaches and felt hand position, again within only 6 training trials. The extent of the sensory change is unexpectedly larger following the visual-proprioceptive discrepancy training. Not surprisingly the size of the reach aftereffects is substantially smaller than following classic visuomotor rotation training. However, the time course by which both changes emerge is similar in the two training types. These results suggest that even the mere exposure to a discrepancy between felt and seen hand location is a sufficient training signal to drive robust motor and sensory plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer E Ruttle
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Canada.,Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada
| | - Bernard Marius 't Hart
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Canada.,School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Canada
| | - Denise Y P Henriques
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Canada.,Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada.,School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Cuppone AV, Semprini M, Konczak J. Consolidation of human somatosensory memory during motor learning. Behav Brain Res 2018; 347:184-192. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2017] [Revised: 02/09/2018] [Accepted: 03/07/2018] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
|
26
|
Maksimovic S, Cressman EK. Long-term retention of proprioceptive recalibration. Neuropsychologia 2018; 114:65-76. [PMID: 29654883 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2017] [Revised: 03/19/2018] [Accepted: 04/08/2018] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Sensorimotor changes are well documented following reaches with altered visual feedback of the hand. Specifically, reaches are adapted and proprioceptive estimates of felt hand position shifted in the direction of the visual feedback experienced. While research has examined one's ability to retain reach adaptation, limited attention has been given to the retention of proprioceptive recalibration. This experiment examined retention of proprioceptive recalibration in the form of recall and savings (i.e., faster proprioceptive recalibration on subsequent testing days) over an extended period of time (i.e., four days). As well, we looked to determine the benefits of additional training on short-term retention (i.e., one day) of proprioceptive recalibration. Twenty-four participants trained to reach to a visual target while seeing a cursor that was rotated 30° clockwise relative to their hand on an initial day of testing. Half of the participants then completed additional reach training trials on 4 subsequent testing days (Training group), whereas the second half of participants did not complete additional training until Day 5 (Non-Training group). Participants provided estimates of their felt hand position on all 5 testing days to establish retention of proprioceptive recalibration. Results revealed that proprioceptive recalibration was recalled 24 h after initial training across all participants. Recall of proprioceptive recalibration was not observed on subsequent testing days for the Non-Training group, while recall of proprioceptive recalibration was retained at a similar level across all subsequent testing days for the Training group. Retention of proprioceptive recalibration in the form of savings was observed on Day 5 in the Non-Training group. These results reveal that short-term recall of proprioceptive recalibration does not benefit from additional training. Moreover, the different time scales (i.e., retention in the form of recall seen only at 24 h after initial training versus savings observed 4 days after initial training in the Non-Training group), suggest that distinct processes may underlie recall and savings of proprioceptive recalibration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefan Maksimovic
- School of Human Kinetics, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1N 6N5
| | - Erin K Cressman
- School of Human Kinetics, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1N 6N5.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Munoz-Rubke F, Mirdamadi JL, Lynch AK, Block HJ. Modality-specific Changes in Motor Cortex Excitability After Visuo-proprioceptive Realignment. J Cogn Neurosci 2017; 29:2054-2067. [PMID: 28777059 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01171] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Spatial realignment of visual and proprioceptive estimates of hand position is necessary both to keep the estimates in register over time and to compensate for sensory perturbations. Such realignment affects perceived hand position, which the brain must use to plan hand movements. We would therefore expect visuo-proprioceptive realignment to affect the motor system at some level, but the physiological basis of this interaction is unknown. Here, we asked whether activity in primary motor cortex (M1), a well-known substrate of motor control, shows evidence of change after visuo-proprioceptive realignment. In two sessions each, 32 healthy adults experienced spatially misaligned or veridical visual and proprioceptive information about their static left index finger. Participants indicated perceived finger position with no performance feedback or knowledge of results. Using TMS over the M1 representation of the misaligned finger, we found no average difference between sessions. However, regression analysis indicated that, in the misaligned session only, proprioceptive realignment was linked with a decrease in M1 activity and visual realignment was linked with an increase in M1 activity. Proprioceptive and visual realignment were inversely related to each other. These results suggest that visuo-proprioceptive realignment does indeed have a physiological impact on the motor system. The lack of a between-session mean difference in M1 activity suggests that the basis of the effect is not the multisensory realignment computation itself, independent of modality. Rather, the changes in M1 are consistent with a modality-specific neural mechanism, such as modulation of somatosensory cortex or dorsal stream visual areas that impact M1.
Collapse
|
28
|
Alvarez TL, Kim EH, Yaramothu C, Granger-Donetti B. The influence of age on adaptation of disparity vergence and phoria. Vision Res 2017; 133:1-11. [PMID: 28192091 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2016] [Revised: 12/13/2016] [Accepted: 01/05/2017] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
A paucity of research exists to investigate whether the normal aging process influences the ability to adapt disparity vergence and phoria. Vergence eye movements and dissociated phoria were recorded from 49 healthy subjects (ages 20-70years) using an objective eye movement tracking system. Four-degree vergence responses were modified using a double-step protocol. Dynamics of vergence were quantified via peak velocity. The phoria adaptation experiment measured the magnitude (net change in phoria level) and rate (magnitude divided by the time constant) of phoria adaption during 5min of sustained fixation on a binocular target (40cm/8.44° from midline). The magnitude of phoria adaptation decreased as a function of age (r=-0.33; p=0.04). The ability to adapt vergence peak velocity and the rate of phoria adaptation showed no significant age-related influence (p>0.05). The data suggest that the ability to modify the disparity vergence system and the rate of phoria adaptation are not dependent on age; whereas, the magnitude of phoria adaptation decreases as part of the normal adult aging process. These results have clinical and basic science implications because one should consider age when assessing the changes in the magnitude of phoria adaptation which can be abnormal in those with oculomotor dysfunctions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tara L Alvarez
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ 07102, USA.
| | - Eun H Kim
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
| | - Chang Yaramothu
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ruttle JE, Cressman EK, ’t Hart BM, Henriques DYP. Time Course of Reach Adaptation and Proprioceptive Recalibration during Visuomotor Learning. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0163695. [PMID: 27732595 PMCID: PMC5061360 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163695] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2016] [Accepted: 09/13/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Training to reach with rotated visual feedback results in adaptation of hand movements, which persist when the perturbation is removed (reach aftereffects). Training also leads to changes in felt hand position, which we refer to as proprioceptive recalibration. The rate at which motor and proprioceptive changes develop throughout training is unknown. Here, we aim to determine the timescale of these changes in order to gain insight into the processes that may be involved in motor learning. Following six rotated reach training trials (30° rotation), at three radially located targets, we measured reach aftereffects and perceived hand position (proprioceptive guided reaches). Participants trained with opposing rotations one week apart to determine if the original training led to any retention or interference. Results suggest that both motor and proprioceptive recalibration occurred in as few as six rotated-cursor training trials (7.57° & 3.88° respectively), with no retention or interference present one week after training. Despite the rapid speed of both motor and sensory changes, these shifts do not saturate to the same degree. Thus, different processes may drive these changes and they may not constitute a single implicit process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer E. Ruttle
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada
- * E-mail:
| | | | | | - Denise Y. P. Henriques
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Canada
- Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Cuppone AV, Squeri V, Semprini M, Masia L, Konczak J. Robot-Assisted Proprioceptive Training with Added Vibro-Tactile Feedback Enhances Somatosensory and Motor Performance. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0164511. [PMID: 27727321 PMCID: PMC5058482 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164511] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2016] [Accepted: 09/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
This study examined the trainability of the proprioceptive sense and explored the relationship between proprioception and motor learning. With vision blocked, human learners had to perform goal-directed wrist movements relying solely on proprioceptive/haptic cues to reach several haptically specified targets. One group received additional somatosensory movement error feedback in form of vibro-tactile cues applied to the skin of the forearm. We used a haptic robotic device for the wrist and implemented a 3-day training regimen that required learners to make spatially precise goal-directed wrist reaching movements without vision. We assessed whether training improved the acuity of the wrist joint position sense. In addition, we checked if sensory learning generalized to the motor domain and improved spatial precision of wrist tracking movements that were not trained. The main findings of the study are: First, proprioceptive acuity of the wrist joint position sense improved after training for the group that received the combined proprioceptive/haptic and vibro-tactile feedback (VTF). Second, training had no impact on the spatial accuracy of the untrained tracking task. However, learners who had received VTF significantly reduced their reliance on haptic guidance feedback when performing the untrained motor task. That is, concurrent VTF was highly salient movement feedback and obviated the need for haptic feedback. Third, VTF can be also provided by the limb not involved in the task. Learners who received VTF to the contralateral limb equally benefitted. In conclusion, somatosensory training can significantly enhance proprioceptive acuity within days when learning is coupled with vibro-tactile sensory cues that provide feedback about movement errors. The observable sensory improvements in proprioception facilitates motor learning and such learning may generalize to the sensorimotor control of the untrained motor tasks. The implications of these findings for neurorehabilitation are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna Vera Cuppone
- Motor Learning and Robotic Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
| | - Valentina Squeri
- Motor Learning and Robotic Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
| | - Marianna Semprini
- Neural Computation Laboratory, Center for Neuroscience and Cognitive Systems, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Lorenzo Masia
- School of Mechanical & Aerospace Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Jürgen Konczak
- Human Sensorimotor Control Laboratory, School of Kinesiology and Center for Clinical Movement Science, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Choi JT, Jensen P, Nielsen JB, Bouyer LJ. Error signals driving locomotor adaptation: cutaneous feedback from the foot is used to adapt movement during perturbed walking. J Physiol 2016; 594:5673-84. [PMID: 27218896 DOI: 10.1113/jp271996] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2015] [Accepted: 05/02/2016] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
KEY POINTS Sensory input from peripheral receptors are important for the regulation of walking patterns. Cutaneous input mediates muscle responses to deal with immediate external perturbations. In this study we focused on the role of cutaneous feedback in locomotor adaptation that takes place over minutes of training. We show that interfering with cutaneous feedback reduced adaptation to ankle perturbations during walking. These results help us understand the neural mechanisms underlying walking adaptation, and have clinical implications for treating walking impairments after neurological injuries. ABSTRACT Locomotor patterns must be adapted to external forces encountered during daily activities. The contribution of different sensory inputs to detecting perturbations and adapting movements during walking is unclear. In the present study, we examined the role of cutaneous feedback in adapting walking patterns to force perturbations. Forces were applied to the ankle joint during the early swing phase using an electrohydraulic ankle-foot orthosis. Repetitive 80 Hz electrical stimulation was applied to disrupt cutaneous feedback from the superficial peroneal nerve (foot dorsum) and medial plantar nerve (foot sole) during walking (Choi et al. 2013). Sensory tests were performed to measure the cutaneous touch threshold and perceptual threshold of force perturbations. Ankle movement were measured when the subjects walked on the treadmill over three periods: baseline (1 min), adaptation (1 min) and post-adaptation (3 min). Subjects (n = 10) showed increased touch thresholds measured with Von Frey monofilaments and increased force perception thresholds with stimulation. Stimulation reduced the magnitude of walking adaptation to force perturbation. In addition, we compared the effects of interrupting cutaneous feedback using anaesthesia (n = 5) instead of repetitive nerve stimulation. Foot anaesthesia reduced ankle adaptation to external force perturbations during walking. The results of the present study suggest that cutaneous input plays a role in force perception, and may contribute to the 'error' signal involved in driving walking adaptation when there is a mismatch between expected and actual force.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia T Choi
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA. .,Neural Control of Movement Research Group, Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology & Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sport, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark.
| | - Peter Jensen
- Neural Control of Movement Research Group, Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology & Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sport, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
| | - Jens Bo Nielsen
- Neural Control of Movement Research Group, Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology & Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sport, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
| | - Laurent J Bouyer
- Department of Rehabilitation, Université Laval & Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration, Québec City, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Zbib B, Henriques DYP, Cressman EK. Proprioceptive recalibration arises slowly compared to reach adaptation. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:2201-13. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4624-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2015] [Accepted: 03/10/2016] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
|
33
|
Ostry DJ, Gribble PL. Sensory Plasticity in Human Motor Learning. Trends Neurosci 2016; 39:114-123. [PMID: 26774345 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 128] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2015] [Revised: 12/07/2015] [Accepted: 12/10/2015] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
There is accumulating evidence from behavioral, neurophysiological, and neuroimaging studies that the acquisition of motor skills involves both perceptual and motor learning. Perceptual learning alters movements, motor learning, and motor networks of the brain. Motor learning changes perceptual function and the sensory circuits of the brain. Here, we review studies of both human limb movement and speech that indicate that plasticity in sensory and motor systems is reciprocally linked. Taken together, this points to an approach to motor learning in which perceptual learning and sensory plasticity have a fundamental role.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David J Ostry
- McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada; Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA.
| | - Paul L Gribble
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Vazquez A, Statton MA, Busgang SA, Bastian AJ. Split-belt walking adaptation recalibrates sensorimotor estimates of leg speed but not position or force. J Neurophysiol 2015; 114:3255-67. [PMID: 26424576 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00302.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2015] [Accepted: 09/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor learning during reaching not only recalibrates movement but can also lead to small but consistent changes in the sense of arm position. Studies have suggested that this sensory effect may be the result of recalibration of a forward model that associates motor commands with their sensory consequences. Here we investigated whether similar perceptual changes occur in the lower limbs after learning a new walking pattern on a split-belt treadmill--a task that critically involves proprioception. Specifically, we studied how this motor learning task affects perception of leg speed during walking, perception of leg position during standing or walking, and perception of contact force during stepping. Our results show that split-belt adaptation leads to robust motor aftereffects and alters the perception of leg speed during walking. This is specific to the direction of walking that was trained during adaptation (i.e., backward or forward). The change in leg speed perception accounts for roughly half of the observed motor aftereffect. In contrast, split-belt adaptation does not alter the perception of leg position during standing or walking and does not change the perception of stepping force. Our results demonstrate that there is a recalibration of a sensory percept specific to the domain of the perturbation that was applied during walking (i.e., speed but not position or force). Furthermore, the motor and sensory consequences of locomotor adaptation may be linked, suggesting overlapping mechanisms driving changes in the motor and sensory domains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alejandro Vazquez
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland; Motion Analysis Laboratory, The Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Matthew A Statton
- Motion Analysis Laboratory, The Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, Maryland
| | - Stefanie A Busgang
- Department of Neuroscience, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland; and
| | - Amy J Bastian
- Department of Neuroscience, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland; and Motion Analysis Laboratory, The Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Cressman EK, Henriques DYP. Generalization patterns for reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration differ after visuomotor learning. J Neurophysiol 2015; 114:354-65. [PMID: 25972587 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00415.2014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2014] [Accepted: 05/11/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Visuomotor learning results in changes in both motor and sensory systems (Cressman EK, Henriques DY. J Neurophysiol 102: 3505-3518, 2009), such that reaches are adapted and sense of felt hand position recalibrated after reaching with altered visual feedback of the hand. Moreover, visuomotor learning has been shown to generalize such that reach adaptation achieved at a trained target location can influence reaches to novel target directions (Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C. J Neurosci 20: 8916-8924, 2000). We looked to determine whether proprioceptive recalibration also generalizes to novel locations. Moreover, we looked to establish the relationship between reach adaptation and changes in sense of felt hand position by determining whether proprioceptive recalibration generalizes to novel targets in a similar manner as reach adaptation. On training trials, subjects reached to a single target with aligned or misaligned cursor-hand feedback, in which the cursor was either rotated or scaled in extent relative to hand movement. After reach training, subjects reached to the training target and novel targets (including targets from a second start position) without visual feedback to assess generalization of reach adaptation. Subjects then performed a proprioceptive estimation task, in which they indicated the position of their hand relative to visual reference markers placed at similar locations as the trained and novel reach targets. Results indicated that shifts in hand position generalized across novel locations, independent of reach adaptation. Thus these distinct sensory and motor generalization patterns suggest that reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration arise from independent error signals and that changes in one system cannot guide adjustments in the other.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin K Cressman
- School of Human Kinetics, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
| | - Denise Y P Henriques
- Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Mostafa AA, Kamran-Disfani R, Bahari-Kashani G, Cressman EK, Henriques DYP. Generalization of reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration at different distances in the workspace. Exp Brain Res 2014; 233:817-27. [PMID: 25479737 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-4157-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2014] [Accepted: 11/20/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Studies have shown that adapting one's reaches in one location in the workspace can generalize to other novel locations. Generalization of this visuomotor adaptation is influenced by the location of novel targets relative to the trained location such that reaches made to novel targets that are located far from the trained target direction (i.e., ~22.5°; Krakauer et al. in J Neurosci 20:8916-8924, 2000) show very little generalization compared to those that are closer to the trained direction. However, generalization is much broader when reaching to novel targets in the same direction but at different distances from the trained target. In this study, we investigated whether changes in hand proprioception (proprioceptive recalibration), like reach adaptation, generalize to different distances of the workspace. Subjects adapted their reaches with a rotated cursor to two target locations at a distance of 13 cm from the home position. We then compared changes in open-loop reaches and felt hand position at these trained locations to novel targets located in the same direction as the trained targets but either at a closer (10 cm) or at a farther distance (15 cm) from the home position. We found reach adaptation generalized to novel closer and farther targets to the same extent as observed at the trained target distance. In contrast, while changes in felt hand position were significant across the two novel distances, this recalibration was smaller for the novel-far locations compared to the trained location. Given that reach adaptation completely generalized across the novel distances but proprioceptive recalibration generalized to a lesser extent for farther distances, we suggest that proprioceptive recalibration may arise independently of motor adaptation and vice versa.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ahmed A Mostafa
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Ontario, Canada,
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Abstract
Recent studies of human speech motor learning suggest that learning is accompanied by changes in auditory perception. But what drives the perceptual change? Is it a consequence of changes in the motor system? Or is it a result of sensory inflow during learning? Here, subjects participated in a speech motor-learning task involving adaptation to altered auditory feedback and they were subsequently tested for perceptual change. In two separate experiments, involving two different auditory perceptual continua, we show that changes in the speech motor system that accompany learning drive changes in auditory speech perception. Specifically, we obtained changes in speech perception when adaptation to altered auditory feedback led to speech production that fell into the phonetic range of the speech perceptual tests. However, a similar change in perception was not observed when the auditory feedback that subjects' received during learning fell into the phonetic range of the perceptual tests. This indicates that the central motor outflow associated with vocal sensorimotor adaptation drives changes to the perceptual classification of speech sounds.
Collapse
|
38
|
Barkley V, Salomonczyk D, Cressman EK, Henriques DYP. Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following terminal visual feedback of the hand. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8:705. [PMID: 25249969 PMCID: PMC4157547 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00705] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2014] [Accepted: 08/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We have shown that when subjects reach with continuous, misaligned visual feedback of their hand, their reaches are adapted and proprioceptive sense of hand position is recalibrated to partially match the visual feedback (Salomonczyk et al., 2011). It is unclear if similar changes arise after reaching with visual feedback that is provided only at the end of the reach (i.e., terminal feedback), when there are shorter temporal intervals for subjects to experience concurrent visual and proprioceptive feedback. Subjects reached to targets with an aligned hand-cursor that provided visual feedback at the end of each reach movement across a 99-trial training block, and with a rotated cursor over three successive blocks of 99 trials each. After each block, no cursor reaches, to measure aftereffects, and felt hand positions were measured. Felt hand position was determined by having subjects indicate the position of their unseen hand relative to a reference marker. We found that subjects adapted their reaches following training with rotated terminal visual feedback, yet slightly less (i.e., reach aftereffects were smaller), than subjects from a previous study who experienced continuous visual feedback. Nonetheless, current subjects recalibrated their sense of felt hand position in the direction of the altered visual feedback, but this proprioceptive change increased incrementally over the three rotated training blocks. Final proprioceptive recalibration levels were comparable to our previous studies in which subjects performed the same task with continuous visual feedback. Thus, compared to reach training with continuous, but altered visual feedback, subjects who received terminal altered visual feedback of the hand produced significant but smaller reach aftereffects and similar changes in hand proprioception when given extra training. Taken together, results suggest that terminal feedback of the hand is sufficient to drive motor adaptation, and also proprioceptive recalibration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria Barkley
- Sensorimotor Control Lab, Centre for Vision Research, Department of Psychology, York University Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Danielle Salomonczyk
- Sensorimotor Control Lab, Centre for Vision Research, Department of Psychology, York University Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Erin K Cressman
- Sensorimotor Control Lab, School of Human Kinetics, University of Ottawa Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | - Denise Y P Henriques
- Sensorimotor Control Lab, Centre for Vision Research, Department of Psychology, York University Toronto, ON, Canada ; School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Langan J. Older adults demonstrate greater accuracy in joint position matching using self-guided movements. Hum Mov Sci 2014; 36:97-106. [DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.05.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2013] [Revised: 05/13/2014] [Accepted: 05/31/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
40
|
Wong JD, Wilson ET, Kistemaker DA, Gribble PL. Bimanual proprioception: are two hands better than one? J Neurophysiol 2013; 111:1362-8. [PMID: 24381030 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00537.2013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Information about the position of an object that is held in both hands, such as a golf club or a tennis racquet, is transmitted to the human central nervous system from peripheral sensors in both left and right arms. How does the brain combine these two sources of information? Using a robot to move participant's passive limbs, we performed psychophysical estimates of proprioceptive function for each limb independently and again when subjects grasped the robot handle with both arms. We compared empirical estimates of bimanual proprioception to several models from the sensory integration literature: some that propose a combination of signals from the left and right arms (such as a Bayesian maximum-likelihood estimate), and some that propose using unimanual signals alone. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that the nervous system both has knowledge of and uses the limb with the best proprioceptive acuity for bimanual proprioception. Surprisingly, a Bayesian model that postulates optimal combination of sensory signals could not predict empirically observed bimanual acuity. These findings suggest that while the central nervous system seems to have information about the relative sensory acuity of each limb, it uses this information in a rather rudimentary fashion, essentially ignoring information from the less reliable limb.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeremy D Wong
- Brain and Mind Institute, Department Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Abstract
Motor learning often involves situations in which the somatosensory targets of movement are, at least initially, poorly defined, as for example, in learning to speak or learning the feel of a proper tennis serve. Under these conditions, motor skill acquisition presumably requires perceptual as well as motor learning. That is, it engages both the progressive shaping of sensory targets and associated changes in motor performance. In the present study, we test the idea that perceptual learning alters somatosensory function and in so doing produces changes to human motor performance and sensorimotor adaptation. Subjects in these experiments undergo perceptual training in which a robotic device passively moves the subject's arm on one of a set of fan-shaped trajectories. Subjects are required to indicate whether the robot moved the limb to the right or the left and feedback is provided. Over the course of training both the perceptual boundary and acuity are altered. The perceptual learning is observed to improve both the rate and extent of learning in a subsequent sensorimotor adaptation task and the benefits persist for at least 24 h. The improvement in the present studies varies systematically with changes in perceptual acuity and is obtained regardless of whether the perceptual boundary shift serves to systematically increase or decrease error on subsequent movements. The beneficial effects of perceptual training are found to be substantially dependent on reinforced decision-making in the sensory domain. Passive-movement training on its own is less able to alter subsequent learning in the motor system. Overall, this study suggests perceptual learning plays an integral role in motor learning.
Collapse
|