1
|
Chen YY, Lambert KJM, Madan CR, Singhal A. Motor-related oscillations reveal the involvement of sensorimotor processes during recognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2024; 216:108003. [PMID: 39481525 DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2024.108003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2024] [Revised: 09/12/2024] [Accepted: 10/27/2024] [Indexed: 11/02/2024]
Abstract
Certain object properties may render an item as more memorable than others. One such property is manipulability, or the extent to which an object can be interacted with using our hands. This study sought to determine if the manipulability of an item modulates memory task performance on both a behavioural and neural level. We recorded electroencephalography (EEG) from a large sample of right-handed individuals (N = 53) during a visual item recognition memory task. The task contained stimuli of both high and low manipulability. Analysis focused on activity in the theta rhythm (3.5-7 Hz), which has been implicated in sensorimotor integration, and the mu rhythm (8-14 Hz), the primary oscillation associated with sensorimotor related behaviours. At both encoding and retrieval, theta oscillations were greater over the left motor region for high manipulability stimuli, suggesting that an item's sensorimotor properties are assessed immediately upon presentation. Manipulability did not affect activity in the mu rhythm. However, mu oscillations over the left motor region were lower during the retrieval of old versus new items and response time was faster for old items, aligning with the cortical reinstatement hypothesis. These results collectively reveal an association between motor oscillations and memory processes, highlight the involvement of sensorimotor processing at both encoding and retrieval.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yvonne Y Chen
- Department of Neurosurgery, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA.
| | - Kathryn J M Lambert
- Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| | | | - Anthony Singhal
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada; Neuroscience and Mental Health Institute, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Culham JC, Buckingham G, Harvey M, Sperandio I, Johnsrude IS. Introduction to the special issue on visual cognition and visuomotor control: A tribute to Mel Goodale. Neuropsychologia 2024:109015. [PMID: 39419471 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.109015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2024]
Affiliation(s)
- Jody C Culham
- Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada.
| | - Gavin Buckingham
- Department of Public Health and Sport Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter, United Kingdom
| | - Monika Harvey
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Glasgow, United Kingdom
| | - Irene Sperandio
- Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Ingrid S Johnsrude
- Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Mazzarella J, Richie D, Chaudhari AMW, Pan X, Tudella E, Spees CK, Heathcock JC. Object Weight and Hand Dominance Impact Kinematics in a Functional Reach-to-Drink Task in School-Aged Children. SENSORS (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2024; 24:5421. [PMID: 39205115 PMCID: PMC11360325 DOI: 10.3390/s24165421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2024] [Revised: 08/06/2024] [Accepted: 08/19/2024] [Indexed: 09/04/2024]
Abstract
This study evaluates the effects of object weight and hand dominance on the end-point kinematics of the hand-to-mouth (withdrawal) movement in a functional reach-to-drink task for typically developing school-aged children. Using 3D motion capture, speed (average velocity and peak velocity), straightness (ratio), and smoothness (number of velocity peaks and log dimensionless jerk) of hand movements were calculated for the withdrawal motion with three different bottle weights (empty, half-filled, and full). Average velocity (550.4 ± 142.0 versus 512.1 ± 145.6 mm/s) and peak velocity (916.3 ± 234 versus 842.7 ± 198.4 mm/s) were significantly higher with the empty versus half-filled bottle and with the non-dominant (average: 543.5 ± 145.2 mm/s; peak: 896.5 ± 207 mm/s) versus dominant (average: 525.2 ± 40.7 mm/s; peak: 864.2 ± 209.2 mm/s) hand. There were no differences in straightness or smoothness. These findings indicate that increasing weight in reach-to-drink task puts greater constraints on the task. The slower movements with the dominant hand might denote better precision control than the non-dominant hand. The quantitative motion capture results show average values for the kinematic variables for a functional reach-to-drink task in a typically developing population of school-aged children with changing weights of the bottles that are relevant to a real-life scenario. These results could inform the design of individualized therapeutic interventions to improve functional upper-extremity use in children with neurodevelopmental motor disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Mazzarella
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| | - Daniel Richie
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Engineering, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| | - Ajit M. W. Chaudhari
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Engineering, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| | - Xueliang Pan
- Center for Biostatistics, Department of Biomedical Informatics, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| | - Eloisa Tudella
- Departamento de Fisioterapia, Universidade Federal de São Carlos, São Carlos 13565-905, SP, Brazil
| | - Colleen K. Spees
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| | - Jill C. Heathcock
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kim T, Zhou R, Gassass S, Soberano T, Liu L, Philip BA. Healthy adults favor stable left/right hand choices over performance at an unconstrained reach-to-grasp task. Exp Brain Res 2024; 242:1349-1359. [PMID: 38563977 PMCID: PMC11506212 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-024-06828-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/10/2023] [Accepted: 03/25/2024] [Indexed: 04/04/2024]
Abstract
Reach-to-grasp actions are fundamental to the daily activities of human life, but few methods exist to assess individuals' reaching and grasping actions in unconstrained environments. The Block Building Task (BBT) provides an opportunity to directly observe and quantify these actions, including left/right hand choices. Here we sought to investigate the motor and non-motor causes of left/right hand choices, and optimize the design of the BBT, by manipulating motor and non-motor difficulty in the BBT's unconstrained reach-to-grasp task. We hypothesized that greater motor and non-motor (e.g. cognitive/perceptual) difficulty would drive increased usage of the dominant hand. To test this hypothesis, we modulated block size (large vs. small) to influence motor difficulty, and model complexity (10 vs. 5 blocks per model) to influence non-motor difficulty, in healthy adults (n = 57). Our data revealed that increased motor and non-motor difficulty led to lower task performance (slower task speed), but participants only increased use of their dominant hand only under the most difficult combination of conditions: in other words, participants allowed their performance to degrade before changing hand choices, even though participants were instructed only to optimize performance. These results demonstrate that hand choices during reach-to grasp actions are more stable than motor performance in healthy right-handed adults, but tasks with multifaceted difficulties can drive individuals to rely more on their dominant hand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taewon Kim
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
- Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Penn State College of Medicine, Hershey, PA, USA
| | - Ruiwen Zhou
- Department of Biostatistics, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Samah Gassass
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Téa Soberano
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Lei Liu
- Department of Biostatistics, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Benjamin A Philip
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Ganel T, Goodale MA. Revisiting the effect of visual illusions on grasping in left and right handers. Neuropsychologia 2024; 195:108806. [PMID: 38280669 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108806] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2023] [Revised: 11/29/2023] [Accepted: 01/24/2024] [Indexed: 01/29/2024]
Abstract
Visual illusions have provided compelling evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. For example, when two different-sized objects are placed on opposite ends of the Ponzo illusion, people erroneously perceive the physically smaller object to be bigger than the physically larger one, but when they pick up the objects, their grip aperture reflects the real difference in size between the objects. This and similar findings have been demonstrated almost entirely for the right hand in right handers. The scarce research that has examined right and left-handed subjects in this context, has typically used only small samples. Here, we extended this research with a larger sample size (more than 50 in each group) in a version of the Ponzo illusion that allowed us to disentangle the effects of real and illusory size on action and perception in much more powerful way. We also collected a wide range of kinematic measures to assess possible differences in visuomotor control in left and right handers. The results showed that the dissociation between perception and action persisted for both hands in right handers, but only for the right hand in left handers. The left hand of left handers was sensitive to the illusion. Left handers also showed more variable and slower movements, as well as larger safety margins in both hands. These findings suggest that grasping in left handers may require more cognitive supervision, which could lead to greater sensitivity to visual context , particularly with their dominant left hand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tzvi Ganel
- Psychology Department, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, 8410500, Israel.
| | - Melvyn A Goodale
- The Western Institute for Neuroscience and the Department of Psychology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, N6A 5B7, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Dexheimer B, Sainburg R, Sharp S, Philip BA. Roles of Handedness and Hemispheric Lateralization: Implications for Rehabilitation of the Central and Peripheral Nervous Systems: A Rapid Review. Am J Occup Ther 2024; 78:7802180120. [PMID: 38305818 PMCID: PMC11017742 DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2024.050398] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2024] Open
Abstract
IMPORTANCE Handedness and motor asymmetry are important features of occupational performance. With an increased understanding of the basic neural mechanisms surrounding handedness, clinicians will be better able to implement targeted, evidence-based neurorehabilitation interventions to promote functional independence. OBJECTIVE To review the basic neural mechanisms behind handedness and their implications for central and peripheral nervous system injury. DATA SOURCES Relevant published literature obtained via MEDLINE. FINDINGS Handedness, along with performance asymmetries observed between the dominant and nondominant hands, may be due to hemispheric specializations for motor control. These specializations contribute to predictable motor control deficits that are dependent on which hemisphere or limb has been affected. Clinical practice recommendations for occupational therapists and other rehabilitation specialists are presented. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE It is vital that occupational therapists and other rehabilitation specialists consider handedness and hemispheric lateralization during evaluation and treatment. With an increased understanding of the basic neural mechanisms surrounding handedness, clinicians will be better able to implement targeted, evidence-based neurorehabilitation interventions to promote functional independence. Plain-Language Summary: The goal of this narrative review is to increase clinicians' understanding of the basic neural mechanisms related to handedness (the tendency to select one hand over the other for specific tasks) and their implications for central and peripheral nervous system injury and rehabilitation. An enhanced understanding of these mechanisms may allow clinicians to better tailor neurorehabilitation interventions to address motor deficits and promote functional independence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brooke Dexheimer
- Brooke Dexheimer, PhD, OTD, OTR/L, is Assistant Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond;
| | - Robert Sainburg
- Robert Sainburg, PhD, OTR, is Professor and Huck Institutes Distinguished Chair, Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, and Department of Neurology, Pennsylvania State College of Medicine, Hershey
| | - Sydney Sharp
- Sydney Sharp, is Occupational Therapy Doctoral Student, Department of Occupational Therapy, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond
| | - Benjamin A Philip
- Benjamin A. Philip, PhD, is Assistant Professor, Program in Occupational Therapy, Department of Neurology and Department of Surgery, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Kim T, Lohse KR, Mackinnon SE, Philip BA. Patient Outcomes After Peripheral Nerve Injury Depend on Bimanual Dexterity and Preserved Use of the Affected Hand. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2024; 38:134-147. [PMID: 38268466 PMCID: PMC10922924 DOI: 10.1177/15459683241227222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Little is known about how peripheral nerve injury affects human performance, behavior, and life. Hand use choices are important for rehabilitation after unilateral impairment, but rarely measured, and are not changed by the normal course of rehabilitation and daily life. OBJECTIVE To identify the relationship between hand use (L/R choices), motor performance, and patient-centered outcomes. METHODS Participants (n = 48) with unilateral peripheral nerve injury were assessed for hand use via Block Building Task, Motor Activity Log, and Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; dexterity (separately for each hand) via Nine-Hole Peg Test, Jebsen Taylor Hand Function Test, and a precision drawing task; patient-centered outcomes via surveys of disability, activity participation, and health-related quality of life; and injury-related factors including injury cause and affected nerve. Factor Analysis of Mixed Data was used to explore relationships between these variables. The data were analyzed under 2 approaches: comparing dominant hand (DH) versus non-dominant hand (NH), or affected versus unaffected hand. RESULTS The data were best explained by 5 dimensions. Good patient outcomes were associated with NH performance, DH performance (separately and secondarily to NH performance), and preserved function and use of the affected hand; whereas poor patient outcomes were associated with preserved but unused function of the affected hand. CONCLUSION After unilateral peripheral nerve injury, hand function, hand usage, and patient life arise from a complex interaction of many factors. To optimize rehabilitation after unilateral impairment, new rehabilitation methods are needed to promote performance and use with the NH, as well as the injured hand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taewon Kim
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Keith R Lohse
- Program in Physical Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Susan E. Mackinnon
- Division of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
| | - Benjamin A. Philip
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, United States
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Petrovic I, Amiridis IG, Kannas T, Tsampalaki Z, Holobar A, Sahinis C, Kellis E, Stankovic D, Enoka RM. Footedness but not dominance influences force steadiness during isometric dorsiflexion in young men. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 2023; 73:102828. [PMID: 37782992 DOI: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2023.102828] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2023] [Revised: 09/18/2023] [Accepted: 09/28/2023] [Indexed: 10/04/2023] Open
Abstract
The aim of the study was to assess the potential influence of footedness and dominance on maximal force, force fluctuations and neural drive during dorsiflexion. Fifteen left-footed (LF) and fifteen right-footed (RF) young adults performed 2 maximal voluntary contractions (MVC) and 3 steady submaximal isometric contractions at five target forces (5, 10, 20, 40 and 60% MVC) with the dorsiflexors of both legs. High-density electromyography (EMG) was used to record the discharge characteristics of motor units (MUs) of Tibialis Anterior. MVC force and EMG amplitude (root mean square) were similar between the two legs and groups (p > 0.05). Force fluctuations (Coefficient of Variation, CoV for force), mean discharge rate of MUs, discharge variability (CoV of interspike interval), and variability in neural drive (standard deviation of filtered cumulative spike train) were greater (p < 0.05) and the input-output gain of the MUs (ΔDR/ΔF) was lower (p < 0.05) for the LF relative to the RF group. The differences in force fluctuations during steady contractions with the dorsiflexors were associated with footedness but not with dominance. They reflect greater variability in motor neuron output, as suggested by coefficient of variation for interspike interval (independent input) and the standard deviation of the smoothed discharge times (common input).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ivana Petrovic
- Laboratory of Neuromechanics, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Serres, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece; Faculty of Sport and Physical Education, University of Niš, Serbia
| | - Ioannis G Amiridis
- Laboratory of Neuromechanics, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Serres, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
| | - Theodoros Kannas
- Laboratory of Neuromechanics, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Serres, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Zoi Tsampalaki
- Laboratory of Neuromechanics, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Serres, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Ales Holobar
- Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, University of Maribor, Slovenia
| | - Chrysostomos Sahinis
- Laboratory of Neuromechanics, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Serres, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Eleftherios Kellis
- Laboratory of Neuromechanics, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Serres, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Daniel Stankovic
- Faculty of Sport and Physical Education, University of Niš, Serbia
| | - Roger M Enoka
- Department of Integrative Physiology, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
McIntosh RD, Iveson MH, Similä SS, Buonocore A. Pictorial depth cues always influence reaching distance. Neuropsychologia 2023; 190:108701. [PMID: 37820755 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108701] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2023] [Revised: 09/30/2023] [Accepted: 10/08/2023] [Indexed: 10/13/2023]
Abstract
We report five experiments to test the influence of pictorial depth on reaching. Our core method is to project a wide-field background of linear perspective and/or texture gradient onto a tabletop, and to measure the amplitude of reaches made to targets within it. In 63 healthy participants performing immediate open-loop reaches across Experiments 1-4, we observed a clear effect of pictorial depth. This effect was driven specifically by the convergence of the background pattern at the target position: for each additional degree of pictorial convergence, reaching distance increased by half a millimetre. In the individual experiments, we applied manipulations that might be expected to modify the influence of pictorial depth. We found no evidence that the effect was modified with monocular viewing, or when participants responded with the left hand, or if a memory delay was inserted before the response. Nor did participants become less susceptible to pictorial depth when visual feedback of terminal reaching errors was provided, although visual feedback during the reach did mitigate the influence of pictorial depth. Finally, the visual form agnosic patient DF showed an entirely normal effect of pictorial depth cues, which leads us to question the idea that this effect emanates from visual analyses of size and shape in the ventral stream, rather than from the dorsal stream, or from earlier stages of visual processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert D McIntosh
- Human Cognitive Neuroscience, Psychology, University of Edinburgh, UK.
| | | | | | - Antimo Buonocore
- Department of Educational, Psychological and Communication Sciences, Suor Orsola Benincasa University, Naples, 80135, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Mazuz Y, Kessler Y, Ganel T. The BTPI: An online battery for measuring susceptibility to visual illusions. J Vis 2023; 23:2. [PMID: 37669069 PMCID: PMC10484025 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.10.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2023] [Accepted: 07/31/2023] [Indexed: 09/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Visual illusions provide a powerful tool for probing the mechanisms that underlie perception. While most previous studies of visual illusions focused on average group-level performance, less attention has been devoted to individual differences in susceptibility to illusions. Unlike in other perceptual domains, in which there are established, validated tools to measure individual differences, such tools are not yet available in the domain of visual illusions. Here, we describe the development and validation of the BTPI (Ben-Gurion University Test for Perceptual Illusions), a new online battery designed to measure susceptibility to the influence of three prominent size illusions: the Ebbinghaus, the Ponzo, and the height-width illusions. The BTPI also measures perceptual resolution, reflected by the just noticeable difference (JND), to detect size differences in the context of each illusion. In Experiment 1 (N = 143), we examined performance in typical self-paced tasks, whereas in Experiment 2 (N = 69), we employed a fixed presentation duration paradigm. High test-retest reliability scores were found for all illusions, with little evidence for intercorrelations between different illusions. In addition, lower perceptual resolution (larger JND) was associated with a larger susceptibility to the illusory effect. The computerized task battery and analysis codes are freely available online.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yarden Mazuz
- Department of Psychology and School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Yoav Kessler
- Department of Psychology and School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Tzvi Ganel
- Department of Psychology and School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Philip BA, Thompson MR, Baune NA, Hyde M, Mackinnon SE. Failure to Compensate: Patients With Nerve Injury Use Their Injured Dominant Hand, Even When Their Nondominant Is More Dexterous. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2022; 103:899-907. [PMID: 34728192 PMCID: PMC11034713 DOI: 10.1016/j.apmr.2021.10.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2021] [Revised: 09/02/2021] [Accepted: 10/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To identify how individuals respond to unilateral upper extremity peripheral nerve injury via compensation (increased use of the nondominant hand). We hypothesized that injury to the dominant hand would have a greater effect on hand use (left vs right choices). We also hypothesized that compensation would not depend on current (postinjury) nondominant hand performance because many patients undergo rehabilitation that is not designed to alter hand use. DESIGN Observational survey, single-arm. SETTINGS Academic research institution and referral center. PARTICIPANTS A total of 48 adults (N=48) with unilateral upper extremity peripheral nerve injury. Another 14 declined participation. Referred sample, including all eligible patients from 16 months at 1 nerve injury clinic and 1 hand therapy clinic. INTERVENTIONS Not applicable. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Hand use (% of actions with each hand) via Block Building Task. Dexterity via Jebsen-Taylor Hand Function. RESULTS Participants preferred their dominant hand regardless of whether it was injured: hand usage (dominant/nondominant) did not differ from typical adults, regardless of injured side (P>.07), even though most participants (77%) were more dexterous with their uninjured nondominant hand (mean asymmetry index, -0.16±0.25). The Block Building Task was sensitive to hand dominance (P=2 × 10-4) and moderately correlated with Motor Activity Log amount scores (r2=0.33, P<.0001). Compensation was associated only with dominant hand dexterity (P=3.9 × 10-3), not on nondominant hand dexterity, rehabilitation, or other patient and/or injury factors (P>.1). CONCLUSIONS Patients with peripheral nerve injury with dominant hand injury do not compensate with their unaffected nondominant hand, even if it is more dexterous. For the subset of patients unlikely to recover function with the injured hand, they could benefit from rehabilitation that encourages compensation with the nondominant hand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin A Philip
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri.
| | - Madeline R Thompson
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri; Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
| | - Nathan A Baune
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
| | - Maureen Hyde
- Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
| | - Susan E Mackinnon
- Division of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Kozuch B. Conscious vision guides motor action—rarely. PHILOSOPHICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2022.2044461] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin Kozuch
- Philosophy Department, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Unilateral resection of both cortical visual pathways in a pediatric patient alters action but not perception. Neuropsychologia 2022; 168:108182. [PMID: 35182580 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108182] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2021] [Revised: 12/21/2021] [Accepted: 02/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The human cortical visual system consists of two major pathways, a ventral pathway that subserves perception and a dorsal pathway that primarily subserves visuomotor control. Previous studies have found that children with cortical resections of the ventral visual pathway retain largely normal visuoperceptual abilities. Whether visually guided actions, supported by computations carried out by the dorsal pathway, follow a similar pattern of preservation remains unknown. To address this question, we examined visuoperceptual and visuomotor behaviors in a pediatric patient, TC, who underwent a cortical resection that included portions of the left ventral and dorsal pathways. We collected kinematic data when TC used her right and left hands to perceptually estimate the width of blocks that varied in width and length, and, separately, to grasp the same blocks. TC's perceptual estimation performance was comparable to that of controls, independent of the hand used. In contrast, relative to controls, she showed reduced visuomotor sensitivity to object shape and this was more evident when she grasped the objects with her contralesional right hand. These results provide novel evidence for a striking difference in the competence of the two visual pathways to cortical injuries acquired in childhood.
Collapse
|
14
|
Talboom JS, De Both MD, Naymik MA, Schmidt AM, Lewis CR, Jepsen WM, Håberg AK, Rundek T, Levin BE, Hoscheidt S, Bolla Y, Brinton RD, Schork NJ, Hay M, Barnes CA, Glisky E, Ryan L, Huentelman MJ. Two separate, large cohorts reveal potential modifiers of age-associated variation in visual reaction time performance. NPJ Aging Mech Dis 2021; 7:14. [PMID: 34210964 PMCID: PMC8249619 DOI: 10.1038/s41514-021-00067-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/21/2020] [Accepted: 04/21/2021] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
To identify potential factors influencing age-related cognitive decline and disease, we created MindCrowd. MindCrowd is a cross-sectional web-based assessment of simple visual (sv) reaction time (RT) and paired-associate learning (PAL). svRT and PAL results were combined with 22 survey questions. Analysis of svRT revealed education and stroke as potential modifiers of changes in processing speed and memory from younger to older ages (ntotal = 75,666, nwomen = 47,700, nmen = 27,966; ages 18-85 years old, mean (M)Age = 46.54, standard deviation (SD)Age = 18.40). To complement this work, we evaluated complex visual recognition reaction time (cvrRT) in the UK Biobank (ntotal = 158,249 nwomen = 89,333 nmen = 68,916; ages 40-70 years old, MAge = 55.81, SDAge = 7.72). Similarities between the UK Biobank and MindCrowd were assessed using a subset of MindCrowd (UKBb MindCrowd) selected to mirror the UK Biobank demographics (ntotal = 39,795, nwomen = 29,640, nmen = 10,155; ages 40-70 years old, MAge = 56.59, SDAge = 8.16). An identical linear model (LM) was used to assess both cohorts. Analyses revealed similarities between MindCrowd and the UK Biobank across most results. Divergent findings from the UK Biobank included (1) a first-degree family history of Alzheimer's disease (FHAD) was associated with longer cvrRT. (2) Men with the least education were associated with longer cvrRTs comparable to women across all educational attainment levels. Divergent findings from UKBb MindCrowd included more education being associated with shorter svRTs and a history of smoking with longer svRTs from younger to older ages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J. S. Talboom
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| | - M. D. De Both
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| | - M. A. Naymik
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| | - A. M. Schmidt
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| | - C. R. Lewis
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| | - W. M. Jepsen
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| | - A. K. Håberg
- grid.5947.f0000 0001 1516 2393Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| | - T. Rundek
- grid.26790.3a0000 0004 1936 8606University of Miami Miller School of Medicine and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Miami, FL USA
| | - B. E. Levin
- grid.26790.3a0000 0004 1936 8606University of Miami Miller School of Medicine and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Miami, FL USA
| | - S. Hoscheidt
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - Y. Bolla
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - R. D. Brinton
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - N. J. Schork
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.410425.60000 0004 0421 8357City of Hope National Medical Center, Duarte, CA USA
| | - M. Hay
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - C. A. Barnes
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - E. Glisky
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - L. Ryan
- Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA ,grid.134563.60000 0001 2168 186XUniversity of Arizona, Tucson, AZ USA
| | - M. J. Huentelman
- grid.250942.80000 0004 0507 3225The Translational Genomics Research Institute (TGen), Phoenix, AZ USA ,Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium, Phoenix, AZ USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Fournier LR, Richardson BP. Partial repetition between action plans delays responses to ideomotor compatible stimuli. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2021; 86:627-641. [PMID: 33740105 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-021-01491-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2020] [Accepted: 02/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Often one must depart from an intended course of events to react to sudden situational demands before resuming his or her original action retained in working memory. Retaining an action plan in working memory (WM) can delay or facilitate the execution of an intervening action when the action features of the two action plans partly overlap (partial repetition) compared to when they do not overlap. We investigated whether partial repetition costs (PRCs) or benefits (PRBs) occur when the intervening event is an ideomotor-compatible stimulus that is a biological representation of the response required by the participant. Participants viewed two visual events and retained an action plan to the first event (A) while executing a speeded response to the second, intervening event (B). In Experiment 1A, the two visual events were ideomotor compatible, non-ideomotor compatible (abstract), or one was ideomotor compatible, and the other abstract. Results showed PRCs for all event A-B stimulus combinations with reduced PRCs for intervening, ideomotor compatible events. In contrast to previous research, there was no evidence that ideomotor-compatible actions were automatic and bypassed the selection bottleneck. Experiment 1B confirmed PRCs for ideomotor compatible stimuli that more accurately mimicked the required response. Findings suggest that mechanisms for activating, selecting, and retaining action plans are similar between ideomotor compatible and abstract visual events. We conclude that PRCs occur in response to intervening events when action plans are generated offline and rely on WM, including those for ideomotor-compatible stimuli; but PRBs may be restricted to actions generated online. This conclusion is consistent with the perceptual-motor framework by Goodale and Milner (Trends in Neuroscience 15:22-25, 1992).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa R Fournier
- Department of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA, 99164-4820, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Ozana A, Ganel T. A double dissociation between action and perception in bimanual grasping: evidence from the Ponzo and the Wundt-Jastrow illusions. Sci Rep 2020; 10:14665. [PMID: 32887921 PMCID: PMC7473850 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-71734-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2020] [Accepted: 07/24/2020] [Indexed: 11/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Research on visuomotor control suggests that visually guided actions toward objects rely on functionally distinct computations with respect to perception. For example, a double dissociation between grasping and between perceptual estimates was reported in previous experiments that pit real against illusory object size differences in the context of the Ponzo illusion. While most previous research on the relation between action and perception focused on one-handed grasping, everyday visuomotor interactions also entail the simultaneous use of both hands to grasp objects that are larger in size. Here, we examined whether this double dissociation extends to bimanual movement control. In Experiment 1, participants were presented with different-sized objects embedded in the Ponzo Illusion. In Experiment 2, we tested whether the dissociation between perception and action extends to a different illusion, the Wundt-Jastrow illusion, which has not been previously used in grasping experiments. In both experiments, bimanual grasping trajectories reflected the differences in physical size between the objects; At the same time, perceptual estimates reflected the differences in illusory size between the objects. These results suggest that the double dissociation between action and perception generalizes to bimanual movement control. Unlike conscious perception, bimanual grasping movements are tuned to real-world metrics, and can potentially resist irrelevant information on relative size and depth.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aviad Ozana
- Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, 8410500, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Tzvi Ganel
- Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, 8410500, Beer-Sheva, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Howells H, Puglisi G, Leonetti A, Vigano L, Fornia L, Simone L, Forkel SJ, Rossi M, Riva M, Cerri G, Bello L. The role of left fronto-parietal tracts in hand selection: Evidence from neurosurgery. Cortex 2020; 128:297-311. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.03.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2019] [Revised: 02/07/2020] [Accepted: 03/12/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
18
|
Philip BA, Kaskutas V, Mackinnon SE. Impact of Handedness on Disability After Unilateral Upper-Extremity Peripheral Nerve Disorder. Hand (N Y) 2020; 15:327-334. [PMID: 30417700 PMCID: PMC7225876 DOI: 10.1177/1558944718810880] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Background: Impairment of the dominant hand should lead to greater disability than impairment of the nondominant hand, but few studies have tested this directly, especially in the domain of upper-extremity peripheral nerve disorder. The aim of this study was to identify the association between hand dominance and standardized measures of disability and health status after upper-extremity peripheral nerve disorder. Methods: An existing database was reanalyzed to identify the relationship between affected-side (dominant vs nondominant) on individuals with unilateral upper-extremity peripheral nerve disorder (N = 400). Primary measure of disability was the Disabilities of the Arm, Shoulder, and Hand (DASH) questionnaire. Results: We found no differences in standardized measures of disability or health status between patients with affected dominant hand and patients with an affected nondominant hand. However, a post hoc exploratory analysis revealed that patients with an affected dominant hand reported substantially reduced ability to perform 2 activities in the DASH questionnaire: "write" and "turn a key." Conclusions: Following unilateral upper-extremity peripheral nerve disorder, impairment of the dominant hand (compared with impairment of the nondominant hand) is associated with reduced ability to perform specific activities, but this reduced ability is not reflected in standardized measures of disability and health status. To adequately identify disability following unilateral impairment of the dominant hand with the DASH, individual items must be used instead of the total score. New or alternative measures are also recommended.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin A. Philip
- Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA,Benjamin A. Philip, Washington University School of Medicine, 4444 Forest Park Avenue, Campus Box 8505, St. Louis, MO 63108, USA.
| | - Vicki Kaskutas
- Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Thirugnanasambandam N, Contreras-Castro FG, Hallett M. Dual-hemispheric transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over primary motor cortex does not affect movement selection. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0226103. [PMID: 31830094 PMCID: PMC6907765 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2019] [Accepted: 11/19/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Volition and sense of agency are two primary components of a voluntary or internally generated movement. It has been shown that movement selection cannot be altered without interfering with the sense of volition using single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the primary motor cortex. In the current study, we aimed at examining whether modulating the cortical excitability of the final effector in the voluntary motor pathway—the primary motor cortex, using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) would alter movement selection. Our hypothesis was that anodal tDCS would increase motor cortical excitability and thereby decrease the threshold for movement execution, which could favor selection of the contralateral hand. We recruited 13 healthy adults to perform a movement selection task involving free-choice and externally-cued trials while applying real/sham tDCS in a C3-C4 dual-hemispheric electrode montage. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not observe any effect of tDCS on movement selection either at the individual or group level. However, our data confirms the strong preference of right-handed individuals for the dominant right hand. We also found higher reaction time for internally generated movement compared to externally triggered movement. We therefore conclude that movement selection cannot be influenced at the level of primary motor cortex and that brain areas upstream of the primary motor cortex in the voluntary motor pathway may be possible targets for influencing movement selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nivethida Thirugnanasambandam
- Human Motor Control Section, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States of America
| | - Felix G. Contreras-Castro
- Human Motor Control Section, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States of America
- Amherst College, Amherst, MA, United States of America
| | - Mark Hallett
- Human Motor Control Section, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
The human brain is often characterized in terms of a duality, with the left and right brains serving complementary functions, and even individuals are sometimes classified as either "left-brained" or "right-brained." Recent evidence from brain imaging shows that hemispheric asymmetry is multidimensional, comprised of independent lateralized circuits. Cerebral asymmetries, which include handedness, probably arise in phylogenesis through the fissioning of ancestral systems that divided and lateralized with increasing demand for specialization. They also vary between individuals, with some showing absent or reversed asymmetries. It is unlikely that this variation is controlled by a single gene, as sometimes assumed, but depends rather on complex interplay among several, perhaps many, genes. Hemispheric asymmetry has often been regarded as a unique mark of being human, but it has also become evident that behavioral and cerebral asymmetries are not confined to humans, and are widespread among animal species. They nevertheless exist against a fundamental background of bilateral symmetry, suggesting a tradeoff between the two. Individual differences in asymmetry, moreover, are themselves adaptive, contributing to the cognitive and behavioral specializations necessary for societies to operate efficiently.
Collapse
|
21
|
Króliczak G, Gonzalez CLR, Carey DP. Editorial: Manual Skills, Handedness, and the Organization of Language in the Brain. Front Psychol 2019; 10:930. [PMID: 31080429 PMCID: PMC6497748 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00930] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2019] [Accepted: 04/08/2019] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory Króliczak
- Action and Cognition Laboratory, Department of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznan, Poznan, Poland
| | - Claudia L R Gonzalez
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, Canada.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, Canada
| | - David P Carey
- Perception, Action and Memory Research Group, School of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Castiello U, Dadda M. A review and consideration on the kinematics of reach-to-grasp movements in macaque monkeys. J Neurophysiol 2018; 121:188-204. [PMID: 30427765 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00598.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The bases for understanding the neuronal mechanisms that underlie the control of reach-to-grasp movements among nonhuman primates, particularly macaques, has been widely studied. However, only a few kinematic descriptions of their prehensile actions are available. A thorough understanding of macaques' prehensile movements is manifestly critical, in light of their role in biomedical research as valuable models for studying neuromotor disorders and brain mechanisms, as well as for developing brain-machine interfaces to facilitate arm control. This article aims to review the current state of knowledge on the kinematics of grasping movements that macaques perform in naturalistic, seminaturalistic, and laboratory settings, to answer the following questions: Are kinematic signatures affected by the context within which the movement is performed? In what ways are kinematics of humans' and macaques' prehensile actions similar/dissimilar? Our analysis reflects the challenges involved in making comparisons across settings and species due to the heterogeneous picture in terms of the number of subjects, stimuli, conditions, and hands used. The kinematics of free-ranging macaques are characterized by distinctive features that are exhibited neither by macaques in laboratory setting nor by human subjects. The temporal incidence of key kinematic landmarks diverges significantly between species, indicating disparities in the overall organization of movement. Given such complexities, we attempt a synthesis of the extant body of evidence, intending to generate some significant implications for directions that future research might take to recognize the remaining gaps and pursue the insights and resolutions to generate an interpretation of movement kinematics that accounts for all settings and subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Umberto Castiello
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova , Padua , Italy
| | - Marco Dadda
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova , Padua , Italy
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Flindall JW, Gonzalez CLR. Wait wait, don't tell me: Handedness questionnaires do not predict hand preference for grasping. Laterality 2018; 24:176-196. [PMID: 29975108 DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2018.1494184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Handedness questionnaires are a common screening tool in psychology and neuroscience, used whenever a participant's performance on a given task may conceivably be affected by their laterality. Two widely-used examples of such questionnaires are the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory and the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire. Both instruments ask respondents to report their hand preference for performing a variety of common tasks (e.g., throwing a ball, or opening a drawer). Here we combined questions from the two instruments (E-WHQ; 22 questions total) and asked participants to report their preferred hand for each via a five-point scale. The purpose of this study was to determine whether responses on the E-WHQ are accurate, reliable, and/or predictive of hand-preference for a simple grasp-to-construct task. Regarding accuracy, handedness scores were 5% lower when participants used a scrambled response key versus a consistent one. Test-retest reliability of the questionnaire was weak, with any given inventory item eliciting a different response from 34% of respondents upon retesting. Neither was the E-WHQ predictively useful-although both left- and right-handers preferred their dominant hands, E-WHQ score did not correlate with overall percentage of dominant-hand grasps in either group. We conclude that the E-WHQ is unsuited for predicting hand preference for grasping.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason W Flindall
- a Department of Psychology , University of British Columbia , Vancouver , Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Tang R, Ren S, Enns JT, Whitwell RL. The left hand disrupts subsequent right hand grasping when their actions overlap. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2018; 188:131-138. [PMID: 29933175 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2017] [Revised: 03/21/2018] [Accepted: 04/26/2018] [Indexed: 10/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Adaptive motor control is premised on the principle of movement minimization, which in turn is premised on a form of sensorimotor memory. But what is the nature of this memory and under what conditions does it operate? Here, we test the limits of sensorimotor memory in an intermanual context by testing the effect that the action performed by the left hand has on subsequent right hand grasps. Target feature-overlap predicts that sensorimotor memory is engaged when task-relevant sensory features of the target are similar across actions; partial effector-overlap predicts that sensorimotor memory is engaged when there is similarity in the task-relevant effectors used to perform an action; and the action-goal conjunction hypotheses predicts that sensorimotor memories are engaged when the action goal and the action type overlap. In three experiments, participants used their left hand to reach out and pick up an object, manually estimate its size, pinch it, look at it, or merely rest the left hand before reaching out to pick up a second object with their right hand. The in-flight anticipatory grip aperture of right-hand grasps was only influenced when it was preceded by grasps performed by the left-hand. Overlap in the sizes of the objects, partial overlap in the effectors used, and in the availability of haptic feedback bore no influence on this metric. These results support the hypothesis that intermanual transfer of sensorimotor memory on grasp execution is dependent on a conjunction of action type and goal.
Collapse
|
25
|
Howells H, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Beyh A, Zappalà G, Leslie A, Simmons A, Murphy DG, Catani M. Frontoparietal Tracts Linked to Lateralized Hand Preference and Manual Specialization. Cereb Cortex 2018; 28:2482-2494. [PMID: 29688293 PMCID: PMC6005057 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhy040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2017] [Revised: 02/05/2018] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans show a preference for using the right hand over the left for tasks and activities of everyday life. While experimental work in non-human primates has identified the neural systems responsible for reaching and grasping, the neural basis of lateralized motor behavior in humans remains elusive. The advent of diffusion imaging tractography for studying connectional anatomy in the living human brain provides the possibility of understanding the relationship between hemispheric asymmetry, hand preference, and manual specialization. In this study, diffusion tractography was used to demonstrate an interaction between hand preference and the asymmetry of frontoparietal tracts, specifically the dorsal branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, responsible for visuospatial integration and motor planning. This is in contrast to the corticospinal tract and the superior cerebellar peduncle, for which asymmetry was not related to hand preference. Asymmetry of the dorsal frontoparietal tract was also highly correlated with the degree of lateralization in tasks requiring visuospatial integration and fine motor control. These results suggest a common anatomical substrate for hand preference and lateralized manual specialization in frontoparietal tracts important for visuomotor processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Henrietta Howells
- Natbrainlab, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Denmark Hill, London, UK
- Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Michel Thiebaut de Schotten
- Brain Connectivity and Behaviour Group, Sorbonne Universities, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, Paris, France
- Frontlab, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière (ICM), UPMC UMRS 1127, Inserm U 1127, CNRS UMR 7225, Paris, France
| | - Flavio Dell’Acqua
- Natbrainlab, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Denmark Hill, London, UK
- Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Ahmad Beyh
- Natbrainlab, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Denmark Hill, London, UK
- Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Giuseppe Zappalà
- Garibaldi Hospital, Piazza Santa Maria di Gesú, 5, Catania, Italy
| | - Anoushka Leslie
- Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Andrew Simmons
- Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Declan G Murphy
- Natbrainlab, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| | - Marco Catani
- Natbrainlab, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Denmark Hill, London, UK
- Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Leib R, Rubin I, Nisky I. Force feedback delay affects perception of stiffness but not action, and the effect depends on the hand used but not on the handedness. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:781-794. [PMID: 29766763 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00822.2017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Interaction with an object often requires the estimation of its mechanical properties. We examined whether the hand that is used to interact with the object and their handedness affected people's estimation of these properties using stiffness estimation as a test case. We recorded participants' responses on a stiffness discrimination of a virtual elastic force field and the grip force applied on the robotic device during the interaction. In half of the trials, the robotic device delayed the participants' force feedback. Consistent with previous studies, delayed force feedback biased the perceived stiffness of the force field. Interestingly, in both left-handed and right-handed participants, for the delayed force field, there was even less perceived stiffness when participants used their left hand than their right hand. This result supports the idea that haptic processing is affected by laterality in the brain, not by handedness. Consistent with previous studies, participants adjusted their applied grip force according to the correct size and timing of the load force regardless of the hand that was used, the handedness, or the delay. This suggests that in all of these conditions, participants were able to form an accurate internal representation of the anticipated trajectory of the load force (size and timing) and that this representation was used for accurate control of grip force independently of the perceptual bias. Thus these results provide additional evidence for the dissociation between action and perception in the processing of delayed information. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Introducing delay to force feedback during interaction with an elastic force field biases the perceived stiffness of the force field. We show that this bias depends on the hand that was used for probing but not on handedness. At the same time, both left-handed and right-handed participants adjusted their applied grip force while using either their left or right hands in anticipation of the correct magnitude and timing despite the delay in load force.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raz Leib
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Inbar Rubin
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Ilana Nisky
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.,Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Begliomini C, Sartori L, Di Bono MG, Budisavljević S, Castiello U. The Neural Correlates of Grasping in Left-Handers: When Handedness Does Not Matter. Front Neurosci 2018; 12:192. [PMID: 29666567 PMCID: PMC5891894 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00192] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2017] [Accepted: 03/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Neurophysiological studies showed that in macaques, grasp-related visuomotor transformations are supported by a circuit involving the anterior part of the intraparietal sulcus, the ventral and the dorsal region of the premotor area. In humans, a similar grasp-related circuit has been revealed by means of neuroimaging techniques. However, the majority of "human" studies considered movements performed by right-handers only, leaving open the question of whether the dynamics underlying motor control during grasping is simply reversed in left-handers with respect to right-handers or not. To address this question, a group of left-handed participants has been scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging while performing a precision grasping task with the left or the right hand. Dynamic causal modeling was used to assess how brain regions of the two hemispheres contribute to grasping execution and whether the intra- and inter-hemispheric connectivity is modulated by the choice of the performing hand. Results showed enhanced inter-hemispheric connectivity between anterior intraparietal and dorsal premotor cortices during grasping execution with the left dominant hand (LDH) (e.g., right hemisphere) compared to the right (e.g., left hemisphere). These findings suggest that that the left hand, although dominant and theoretically more skilled in left handers, might need additional resources in terms of the visuomotor control and on-line monitoring to accomplish a precision grasping movement. The results are discussed in light of theories on the modulation of parieto-frontal networks during the execution of prehensile movements, providing novel evidence supporting the hypothesis of a handedness-independent specialization of the left hemisphere in visuomotor control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiara Begliomini
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padua, Italy.,Padua Neuroscience Center, Padua, Italy
| | - Luisa Sartori
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padua, Italy.,Padua Neuroscience Center, Padua, Italy
| | - Maria G Di Bono
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padua, Italy
| | | | - Umberto Castiello
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padua, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Mundinano IC, Fox DM, Kwan WC, Vidaurre D, Teo L, Homman-Ludiye J, Goodale MA, Leopold DA, Bourne JA. Transient visual pathway critical for normal development of primate grasping behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2018; 115:1364-1369. [PMID: 29298912 PMCID: PMC5819431 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1717016115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
An evolutionary hallmark of anthropoid primates, including humans, is the use of vision to guide precise manual movements. These behaviors are reliant on a specialized visual input to the posterior parietal cortex. Here, we show that normal primate reaching-and-grasping behavior depends critically on a visual pathway through the thalamic pulvinar, which is thought to relay information to the middle temporal (MT) area during early life and then swiftly withdraws. Small MRI-guided lesions to a subdivision of the inferior pulvinar subnucleus (PIm) in the infant marmoset monkey led to permanent deficits in reaching-and-grasping behavior in the adult. This functional loss coincided with the abnormal anatomical development of multiple cortical areas responsible for the guidance of actions. Our study reveals that the transient retino-pulvinar-MT pathway underpins the development of visually guided manual behaviors in primates that are crucial for interacting with complex features in the environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Inaki-Carril Mundinano
- Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - Dylan M Fox
- Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - William C Kwan
- Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - Diego Vidaurre
- Oxford Centre for Human Brain Activity, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7JX, United Kingdom
| | - Leon Teo
- Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - Jihane Homman-Ludiye
- Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - Melvyn A Goodale
- The Brain and Mind Institute, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada N6A 5B7
| | - David A Leopold
- Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20892
| | - James A Bourne
- Australian Regenerative Medicine Institute, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia;
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Saneyoshi A. The Ebbinghaus illusion with small inducers appears larger on the right side. Exp Brain Res 2018; 236:933-944. [PMID: 29368080 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5168-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2016] [Accepted: 01/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The effects of left and right alignment on the Ebbinghaus illusion were investigated in three experiments. In Experiment 1, the Ebbinghaus illusion was presented on the left or right side, and the points of subjective equality (PSE) were measured. Only the central disk of the figure with small inducers was perceived larger when it was positioned on the right side rather than on the left. In Experiments 2 and 3, left, right, and central placement were used to determine if the results of Experiment 1 were caused by a decrease of the illusion on the left side or an increase of the illusion on the right side. There was no difference in the illusion effect between the left and the center; however, the illusion effect increased when the figure was presented on the right side. These results suggest that a hemispheric asymmetry for global and local spatial attention influences the laterality of the Ebbinghaus illusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ayako Saneyoshi
- Department of Psychology, Teikyo University, 359 Otsuka, Hachioji, Tokyo, 192-0395, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Hopkins WD. A review of performance asymmetries in hand skill in nonhuman primates with a special emphasis on chimpanzees. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2018; 238:57-89. [DOI: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
31
|
Nelson EL, Berthier NE, Konidaris GD. Handedness and Reach-to-Place Kinematics in Adults: Left-Handers Are Not Reversed Right-Handers. J Mot Behav 2017; 50:381-391. [PMID: 28876178 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2017.1363698] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The primary goal of this study was to examine the relations between limb control and handedness in adults. Participants were categorized as left or right handed for analyses using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. Three-dimensional recordings were made of each arm on two reach-to-place tasks: adults reached to a ball and placed it into the opening of a toy (fitting task), or reached to a Cheerio inside a cup, which they placed on a designated mark after each trial (cup task). We hypothesized that limb control and handedness were related, and we predicted that we would observe side differences favoring the dominant limb based on the dynamic dominance hypothesis of motor lateralization. Specifically, we predicted that the dominant limb would be straighter and smoother on both tasks compared with the nondominant limb (i.e., right arm in right-handers and left arm in left-handers). Our results only partially supported these predictions for right-handers, but not for left-handers. When differences between hands were observed, the right hand was favored regardless of handedness group. Our findings suggest that left-handers are not reversed right-handers when compared on interlimb kinematics for reach-to-place tasks, and reaffirm that task selection is critical when evaluating manual asymmetries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eliza L Nelson
- a Department of Psychology , Florida International University , Miami
| | - Neil E Berthier
- b Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences , University of Massachusetts Amherst
| | - George D Konidaris
- c Department of Computer Science , Brown University , Providence , Rhode Island
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Scocchia L, Paroli M, Stucchi NA, Sedda A. Spatial Alignment and Response Hand in Geometric and Motion Illusions. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1169. [PMID: 28769830 PMCID: PMC5509767 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2017] [Accepted: 06/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Perception of visual illusions is susceptible to manipulation of their spatial properties. Further, illusions can sometimes affect visually guided actions, especially the movement planning phase. Remarkably, visual properties of objects related to actions, such as affordances, can prime more accurate perceptual judgements. In spite of the amount of knowledge available on affordances and on the influence of illusions on actions (or lack of thereof), virtually nothing is known about the reverse: the influence of action-related parameters on the perception of visual illusions. Here, we tested a hypothesis that the response mode (that can be linked to action-relevant features) can affect perception of the Poggendorff (geometric) and of the Vanishing Point (motion) illusion. We explored the role of hand dominance (right dominant versus left non-dominant hand) and its interaction with stimulus spatial alignment (i.e., congruency between visual stimulus and the hand used for responses). Seventeen right-handed participants performed our tasks with their right and left hands, and the stimuli were presented in regular and mirror-reversed views. It turned out that the regular version of the Poggendorff display generates a stronger illusion compared to the mirror version, and that participants are less accurate and show more variability when they use their left hand in responding to the Vanishing Point. In summary, our results show that there is a marginal effect of hand precision in motion related illusions, which is absent for geometrical illusions. In the latter, attentional anisometry seems to play a greater role in generating the illusory effect. Taken together, our findings suggest that changes in the response mode (here: manual action-related parameters) do not necessarily affect illusion perception. Therefore, although intuitively speaking there should be at least unidirectional effects of perception on action, and possible interactions between the two systems, this simple study still suggests their relative independence, except for the case when the less skilled (non-dominant) hand and arguably more deliberate responses are used.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Scocchia
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-BicoccaMilan, Italy
| | - Michela Paroli
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Heriot-Watt UniversityEdinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Natale A Stucchi
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-BicoccaMilan, Italy
| | - Anna Sedda
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Heriot-Watt UniversityEdinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Gamble CM, Song JH. Dynamic modulation of illusory and physical target size on separate and coordinated eye and hand movements. J Vis 2017; 17:23. [PMID: 28362898 PMCID: PMC5381334 DOI: 10.1167/17.3.23] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
In everyday behavior, two of the most common visually guided actions-eye and hand movements-can be performed independently, but are often synergistically coupled. In this study, we examine whether the same visual representation is used for different stages of saccades and pointing, namely movement preparation and execution, and whether this usage is consistent between independent and naturalistic coordinated eye and hand movements. To address these questions, we used the Ponzo illusion to dissociate the perceived and physical sizes of visual targets and measured the effects on movement preparation and execution for independent and coordinated saccades and pointing. During independent movements, we demonstrated that both physically and perceptually larger targets produced faster preparation for both effectors. Furthermore, participants who showed a greater influence of the illusion on saccade preparation also showed a greater influence on pointing preparation, suggesting that a shared mechanism involved in preparation across effectors is influenced by illusions. However, only physical but not perceptual target sizes influenced saccade and pointing execution. When pointing was coordinated with saccades, we observed different dynamics: pointing no longer showed modulation from illusory size, while saccades showed illusion modulation for both preparation and execution. Interestingly, in independent and coordinated movements, the illusion modulated saccade preparation more than pointing preparation, with this effect more pronounced during coordination. These results suggest a shared mechanism, dominated by the eyes, may underlie visually guided action preparation across effectors. Furthermore, the influence of illusions on action may operate within such a mechanism, leading to dynamic interactions between action modalities based on task demands.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine M Gamble
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, & Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI,
| | - Joo-Hyun Song
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, & Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI, USABrown Institute for Brain Science, Brown University, Providence, RI, ://research.clps.brown.edu/songlab/
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Corballis MC. The Evolution of Lateralized Brain Circuits. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1021. [PMID: 28670296 PMCID: PMC5472687 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2017] [Accepted: 06/02/2017] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
In the vast clade of animals known as the bilateria, cerebral and behavioral asymmetries emerge against the backdrop of bilateral symmetry, with a functional trade-off between the two. Asymmetries can lead to more efficient processing and packaging of internal structures, but at the expense of efficient adaptation to a natural world without systematic left-right bias. Asymmetries may arise through the fissioning of ancestral structures that are largely symmetrical, creating new circuits. In humans these may include asymmetrical adaptations to language and manufacture, and as one or other hemisphere gains dominance for functions that were previously represented bilaterally. This is best illustrated in the evolution of such functions as language and tool manufacture in humans, which may derive from the mirror-neuron system in primates, but similar principles probably apply to the many other asymmetries now evident in a wide range of animals. Asymmetries arise in largely independent manner with multi-genetic sources, rather than as a single over-riding principle.
Collapse
|
35
|
Programming of left hand exploits task set but that of right hand depends on recent history. Exp Brain Res 2017; 235:2215-2224. [PMID: 28451736 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-017-4964-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2016] [Accepted: 04/20/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
There are many differences between the left hand and the right hand. But it is not clear if there is a difference in programming between left hand and right hand when the hands perform the same movement. In current study, we carried out two experiments to investigate whether the programming of two hands was equivalent or they exploited different strategies. In the first experiment, participants were required to use one hand to grasp an object with visual feedback or to point to the center of one object without visual feedback on alternate trials, or to grasp an object without visual feedback and to point the center of one object with visual feedback on alternating trials. They then performed the tasks with the other hand. The result was that previous pointing task affected current grasping when it was performed by the left hand, but not the right hand. In experiment 2, we studied if the programming of the left (or right) hand would be affected by the pointing task performed on the previous trial not only by the same hand, but also by the right (or left) hand. Participants pointed and grasped the objects alternately with two hands. The result was similar with Experiment 1, i.e., left-hand grasping was affected by right-hand pointing, whereas right-hand grasping was immune from the interference from left hand. Taken together, the results suggest that when open- and closed-loop trials are interleaved, motor programming of grasping with the right hand was affected by the nature of the online feedback on the previous trial only if it was a grasping trial, suggesting that the trial-to-trial transfer depends on sensorimotor memory and not on task set. In contrast, motor programming of grasping with the left hand can use information about the nature of the online feedback on the previous trial to specify the parameters of the movement, even when the type of movement that occurred was quite different (i.e., pointing) and was performed with the right hand. This suggests that trial-to-trial transfer with the left hand depends on some sort of carry-over of task set for dealing with the availability of visual feedback.
Collapse
|
36
|
Gomez MA, Snow JC. Action properties of object images facilitate visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2017; 43:1115-1124. [PMID: 28263627 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000390] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
There is mounting evidence that constraints from action can influence the early stages of object selection, even in the absence of any explicit preparation for action. Here, we examined whether action properties of images can influence visual search, and whether such effects were modulated by hand preference. Observers searched for an oddball target among 3 distractors. The search arrays consisted either of images of graspable "handles" ("action-related" stimuli), or images that were otherwise identical to the handles but in which the semicircular fulcrum element was reoriented so that the stimuli no longer looked like graspable objects ("non-action-related" stimuli). In Experiment 1, right-handed observers, who have been shown previously to prefer to use the right hand over the left for manual tasks, were faster to detect targets in action-related versus non-action-related arrays, and showed a response time (reaction time [RT]) advantage for rightward- versus leftward-oriented action-related handles. In Experiment 2, left-handed observers, who have been shown to use the left and right hands relatively equally in manual tasks, were also faster to detect targets in the action-related versus non-action-related arrays, but RTs were equally fast for rightward- and leftward-oriented handle targets. Together, or results suggest that action properties in images, and constraints for action imposed by preferences for manual interaction with objects, can influence attentional selection in the context of visual search. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Gomez
- Department of Psychology, Program in Cognitive and Brain Sciences, The University of Nevada
| | - Jacqueline C Snow
- Department of Psychology, Program in Cognitive and Brain Sciences, The University of Nevada
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Le A, Vesia M, Yan X, Crawford JD, Niemeier M. Parietal area BA7 integrates motor programs for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination. J Neurophysiol 2017; 117:624-636. [PMID: 27832593 PMCID: PMC5288481 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00299.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2016] [Accepted: 11/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Skillful interaction with the world requires that the brain uses a multitude of sensorimotor programs and subroutines, such as for reaching, grasping, and the coordination of the two body halves. However, it is unclear how these programs operate together. Networks for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination might converge in common brain areas. For example, Brodmann area 7 (BA7) is known to activate in disparate tasks involving the three types of movements separately. Here, we asked whether BA7 plays a key role in integrating coordinated reach-to-grasp movements for both arms together. To test this, we applied transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to disrupt BA7 activity in the left and right hemispheres, while human participants performed a bimanual size-perturbation grasping task using the index and middle fingers of both hands to grasp a rectangular object whose orientation (and thus grasp-relevant width dimension) might or might not change. We found that TMS of the right BA7 during object perturbation disrupted the bimanual grasp and transport/coordination components, and TMS over the left BA7 disrupted unimanual grasps. These results show that right BA7 is causally involved in the integration of reach-to-grasp movements of the two arms. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Our manuscript describes a role of human Brodmann area 7 (BA7) in the integration of multiple visuomotor programs for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination. Our results are the first to suggest that right BA7 is critically involved in the coordination of reach-to-grasp movements of the two arms. The results complement previous reports of right-hemisphere lateralization for bimanual grasps.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ada Le
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Scarborough, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Michael Vesia
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Division of Neurology and Krembil Neuroscience Centre, Toronto Western Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Xiaogang Yan
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - J Douglas Crawford
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Neuroscience Graduate Diploma Program and Departments of Psychology, Biology, and Kinesiology & Health Sciences, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and
- Canadian Action and Perception Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Matthias Niemeier
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Scarborough, Toronto, Ontario, Canada;
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Colman HA, Remington RW, Kritikos A. Handedness and Graspability Modify Shifts of Visuospatial Attention to Near-Hand Objects. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0170542. [PMID: 28125635 PMCID: PMC5268391 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170542] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2016] [Accepted: 01/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
We examined how factors related to the internal representation of the hands (handedness and grasping affordances) influence the distribution of visuospatial attention near the body. Left and right handed participants completed a covert visual cueing task, discriminating between two target shapes. In Experiment 1, participants responded with either their dominant or non-dominant hand. In Experiment 2, the non-responding hand was positioned below one of two target placeholders, aligned with the shoulder. In Experiment 3 the near-monitor hand was positioned under the placeholder in the opposite region of hemispace, crossed over the body midline. For Experiments 2 & 3, in blocked trials the palmar and back-of hand surfaces were directed towards the target placeholder such that targets appeared towards either the graspable or non-graspable space of the hand respectively. In Experiment 2, both left and right handers displayed larger accuracy cueing effects for targets near versus distant from the graspable space of the right hand. Right handers also displayed larger response time cueing effects for objects near the graspable versus non-graspable region of their dominant hand but not for their non-dominant hands. These effects were not evident for left-handers. In Experiment 3, for right handers, accuracy biases for near hand targets were still evident when the hand was crossed over the body midline, and reflected hand proximity but not functional orientation biases. These findings suggest that biased visuospatial attention enhances object identity discrimination near hands and that these effects are particularly enhanced for right-handers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hayley A. Colman
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| | - Roger W. Remington
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota, United States of America
| | - Ada Kritikos
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Króliczak G, Piper BJ, Frey SH. Specialization of the left supramarginal gyrus for hand-independent praxis representation is not related to hand dominance. Neuropsychologia 2016; 93:501-512. [PMID: 27020138 PMCID: PMC5036996 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2015] [Revised: 02/27/2016] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Data from focal brain injury and functional neuroimaging studies implicate a distributed network of parieto-fronto-temporal areas in the human left cerebral hemisphere as playing distinct roles in the representation of meaningful actions (praxis). Because these data come primarily from right-handed individuals, the relationship between left cerebral specialization for praxis representation and hand dominance remains unclear. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to evaluate the hypothesis that strongly left-handed (right hemisphere motor dominant) adults also exhibit this left cerebral specialization. Participants planned familiar actions for subsequent performance with the left or right hand in response to transitive (e.g., "pounding") or intransitive (e.g. "waving") action words. In linguistic control trials, cues denoted non-physical actions (e.g., "believing"). Action planning was associated with significant, exclusively left-lateralized and extensive increases of activity in the supramarginal gyrus (SMg), and more focal modulations in the left caudal middle temporal gyrus (cMTg). This activity was hand- and gesture-independent, i.e., unaffected by the hand involved in subsequent action performance, and the type of gesture (i.e., transitive or intransitive). Compared directly with right-handers, left-handers exhibited greater involvement of the right angular gyrus (ANg) and dorsal premotor cortex (dPMC), which is indicative of a less asymmetric functional architecture for praxis representation. We therefore conclude that the organization of mechanisms involved in planning familiar actions is influenced by one's motor dominance. However, independent of hand dominance, the left SMg and cMTg are specialized for ideomotor transformations-the integration of conceptual knowledge and motor representations into meaningful actions. These findings support the view that higher-order praxis representation and lower-level motor dominance rely on dissociable mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory Króliczak
- Institute of Psychology, Action & Cognition Laboratory, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznań, Poland
| | - Brian J Piper
- Neuroscience Program, Bowdoin College, Brunswick, ME 04011, USA
| | - Scott H Frey
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Rehabilitation Neuroscience Laboratory; Brain Imaging Center, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Real and illusory issues in the illusion debate (Why two things are sometimes better than one): Commentary on Kopiske et al. (2016). Cortex 2016; 88:205-209. [PMID: 27498625 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2016] [Revised: 06/23/2016] [Accepted: 06/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
41
|
The functional subdivision of the visual brain: Is there a real illusion effect on action? A multi-lab replication study. Cortex 2016; 79:130-52. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2015] [Revised: 02/10/2016] [Accepted: 03/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
42
|
Philip BA, Frey SH. Increased functional connectivity between cortical hand areas and praxis network associated with training-related improvements in non-dominant hand precision drawing. Neuropsychologia 2016; 87:157-168. [PMID: 27212059 PMCID: PMC4903896 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/03/2016] [Revised: 04/05/2016] [Accepted: 05/16/2016] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Chronic forced use of the non-dominant left hand yields substantial improvements in the precision and quality of writing and drawing. These changes may arise from increased access by the non-dominant (right) hemisphere to dominant (left) hemisphere mechanisms specialized for end-point precision control. To evaluate this prediction, 22 healthy right-handed adults underwent resting state functional connectivity (FC) MRI scans before and after 10 days of training on a left hand precision drawing task. 89% of participants significantly improved left hand speed, accuracy, and smoothness. Smoothness gains were specific to the trained left hand and persistent: 6 months after training, 71% of participants exhibited above-baseline movement smoothness. Contrary to expectations, we found no evidence of increased FC between right and left hemisphere hand areas. Instead, training-related improvements in left hand movement smoothness were associated with increased FC between both sensorimotor hand areas and a left-lateralized parieto-prefrontal network implicated in manual praxis. By contrast, skill retention at 6 months was predicted by changes including decreased FC between the representation of the trained left hand and bilateral sensorimotor, parietal, and premotor cortices, possibly reflecting consolidation and a disengagement of early learning processes. These data indicate that modest amounts of training (< 200 min total) can induce substantial, persistent improvements the precision and quality of non-dominant hand control in healthy adults, supported by strengthened connectivity between bilateral sensorimotor hand areas and a left-lateralized parieto-prefrontal praxis network.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin A Philip
- Department of Psychology, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.
| | - Scott H Frey
- Department of Psychology, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
de Bruin N, Bryant DC, MacLean JN, Gonzalez CLR. Assessing Visuospatial Abilities in Healthy Aging: A Novel Visuomotor Task. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 8:7. [PMID: 26869918 PMCID: PMC4740775 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2015] [Accepted: 01/08/2016] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
This study examined the efficacy of a novel reaching-and-grasping task in determining visuospatial abilities across adulthood. The task required male and female young (18–25 years) and older adults (60–82 years) to replicate a series of complex models by locating and retrieving the appropriate building blocks from an array. The task allows visuospatial complexity to be manipulated independently from the visuomotor demands. Mental rotation and spatial visualization abilities were assessed. The results showed that the time taken to complete the tasks increased with increased mental rotation complexity. Patterns of hand use were also influenced by the complexity of the models being constructed with right hand use being greater for the less complex models. In addition, although older adults consistently performed the visuomotor tasks slower than the younger adults, their performance was comparable when expressed as the percent change in task demands. This is suggestive that spatial abilities are preserved in older adults. Given the ecologically validity, the described task is an excellent candidate for investigating: (1) developmental; (2) sex-based; and (3) pathology-based differences in spatial abilities in the visuomotor domain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie de Bruin
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Lethbridge Lethbridge, AB, Canada
| | - Devon C Bryant
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Lethbridge Lethbridge, AB, Canada
| | - Jessica N MacLean
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Lethbridge Lethbridge, AB, Canada
| | - Claudia L R Gonzalez
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Lethbridge Lethbridge, AB, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Stone KD, Gonzalez CLR. The contributions of vision and haptics to reaching and grasping. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1403. [PMID: 26441777 PMCID: PMC4584943 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01403] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2015] [Accepted: 09/02/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
This review aims to provide a comprehensive outlook on the sensory (visual and haptic) contributions to reaching and grasping. The focus is on studies in developing children, normal, and neuropsychological populations, and in sensory-deprived individuals. Studies have suggested a right-hand/left-hemisphere specialization for visually guided grasping and a left-hand/right-hemisphere specialization for haptically guided object recognition. This poses the interesting possibility that when vision is not available and grasping relies heavily on the haptic system, there is an advantage to use the left hand. We review the evidence for this possibility and dissect the unique contributions of the visual and haptic systems to grasping. We ultimately discuss how the integration of these two sensory modalities shape hand preference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kayla D Stone
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge AB, Canada
| | - Claudia L R Gonzalez
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge AB, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Stone KD, Gonzalez CLR. Manual preferences for visually- and haptically-guided grasping. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 160:1-10. [PMID: 26134414 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2014] [Revised: 04/30/2015] [Accepted: 06/18/2015] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Studies have shown that individuals exhibit a right-hand preference for grasping during visually-guided tasks. Recently, we have found that when vision is occluded right-hand preference decreases dramatically. It remains unknown however, if this decrease is a result of visual occlusion or the effects of relying only on haptic feedback. Therefore, in the present study, we sought to explore the contributions of vision and haptics (separately and in conjunction) to hand preference for grasping. Right- and left-handed individuals were tested on a block building task under four different visual and haptic conditions: 1) vision/normal haptic feedback (V/H), 2) no vision/normal haptic feedback (NV/H), 3) vision/constrained haptic feedback (V/Constrained-H), and 4) no vision/constrained haptic feedback (NV/Constrained-H). Vision was occluded using a blindfold and haptic feedback was constrained by asking participants to wear textured gloves. Right-handed individuals displayed a right-hand preference when vision was available (V/H and V/Constrained-H groups), but this preference was much greater when haptic feedback was constrained (V/Constrained-H group). When vision was occluded and haptic feedback was used to complete the task (NV/H) no hand preference was found. Finally hand preference was similar between the V/H and the NV/Constrained-H groups. For left-handed individuals, no differences in hand use were found between the different sensory groups, but the NV/H group showed a clear left-hand preference for haptically-guided grasping. The results suggest that haptics plays an important role in hand preference for grasping. Furthermore, they support a left-hand/right-hemisphere specialization for haptically-guided grasping (regardless of handedness) and a right-hand/left-hemisphere specialization for visually-guided grasping (at least in right-handed individuals).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kayla D Stone
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 3M4, Canada.
| | - Claudia L R Gonzalez
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 3M4, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Avery B, Cowper-Smith CD, Westwood DA. Spatial interactions between consecutive manual responses. Exp Brain Res 2015; 233:3283-90. [PMID: 26259747 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4396-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2015] [Accepted: 07/28/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
We have shown that the latency to initiate a reaching movement is increased if its direction is the same as a previous movement compared to movements that differ by 90° or 180° (Cowper-Smith and Westwood in Atten Percept Psychophys 75:1914-1922, 2013). An influential study (Taylor and Klein in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1639-1656, 2000), however, reported the opposite spatial pattern for manual keypress responses: repeated responses on the same side had reduced reaction time compared to responses on opposite sides. In order to determine whether there are fundamental differences in the patterns of spatial interactions between button-pressing responses and reaching movements, we compared both types of manual responses using common methods. Reaching movements and manual keypress responses were performed in separate blocks of trials using consecutive central arrow stimuli that directed participants to respond to left or right targets. Reaction times were greater for manual responses made to the same target as a previous response (M = 390 ms) as compared to the opposite target (M = 365 ms; similarity main effect: p < 0.001) regardless of whether the response was a reaching movement or a keypress response. This finding is broadly consistent with an inhibitory mechanism operating at the level of motor output that discourages movements that achieve the same spatial goal as a recent action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brittany Avery
- Division of Kinesiology, Dalhousie University, 6230 South Street, P.O. Box 15000, Halifax, NS, B3H 4R2, Canada
| | | | - David A Westwood
- Division of Kinesiology, Dalhousie University, 6230 South Street, P.O. Box 15000, Halifax, NS, B3H 4R2, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Action plans can interact to hinder or facilitate reach performance. Atten Percept Psychophys 2015; 77:2755-67. [DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0959-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
48
|
Briscoe R, Schwenkler J. Conscious Vision in Action. Cogn Sci 2015; 39:1435-67. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12226] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2012] [Revised: 08/27/2014] [Accepted: 08/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
49
|
Begliomini C, Sartori L, Miotto D, Stramare R, Motta R, Castiello U. Exploring manual asymmetries during grasping: a dynamic causal modeling approach. Front Psychol 2015; 6:167. [PMID: 25759677 PMCID: PMC4338815 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00167] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2014] [Accepted: 02/02/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Recording of neural activity during grasping actions in macaques showed that grasp-related sensorimotor transformations are accomplished in a circuit constituted by the anterior part of the intraparietal sulcus (AIP), the ventral (F5) and the dorsal (F2) region of the premotor area. In humans, neuroimaging studies have revealed the existence of a similar circuit, involving the putative homolog of macaque areas AIP, F5, and F2. These studies have mainly considered grasping movements performed with the right dominant hand and only a few studies have measured brain activity associated with a movement performed with the left non-dominant hand. As a consequence of this gap, how the brain controls for grasping movement performed with the dominant and the non-dominant hand still represents an open question. A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment has been conducted, and effective connectivity (dynamic causal modeling, DCM) was used to assess how connectivity among grasping-related areas is modulated by hand (i.e., left and right) during the execution of grasping movements toward a small object requiring precision grasping. Results underlined boosted inter-hemispheric couplings between dorsal premotor cortices during the execution of movements performed with the left rather than the right dominant hand. More specifically, they suggest that the dorsal premotor cortices may play a fundamental role in monitoring the configuration of fingers when grasping movements are performed by either the right and the left hand. This role becomes particularly evident when the hand less-skilled (i.e., the left hand) to perform such action is utilized. The results are discussed in light of recent theories put forward to explain how parieto-frontal connectivity is modulated by the execution of prehensile movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiara Begliomini
- Department of General Psychology and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova Padova, Italy
| | - Luisa Sartori
- Department of General Psychology and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova Padova, Italy
| | - Diego Miotto
- Department of Medicine, University of Padova Padova, Italy
| | | | | | - Umberto Castiello
- Department of General Psychology and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Netelenbos N, Gonzalez CLR. Is that graspable? Let your right hand be the judge. Brain Cogn 2014; 93:18-25. [PMID: 25483823 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2014] [Revised: 08/07/2014] [Accepted: 11/07/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
A right-hand preference for visually-guided grasping has been shown on numerous accounts. Grasping an object requires the integration of both visual and motor components of visuomotor processing. It has been suggested that the left hemisphere plays an integral role in visuomotor functions. The present study serves to investigate whether the visual processing of graspable objects, without any actual reaching or grasping movements, yields a right-hand (left-hemisphere) advantage. Further, we aim to address whether such an advantage is automatically evoked by motor affordances. Two groups of right-handed participants were asked to categorize objects presented on a computer monitor by responding on a keypad. The first group was asked to categorize visual stimuli as graspable (e.g. apple) or non-graspable (e.g. car). A second group categorized the same stimuli but as nature-made (e.g. apple) or man-made (e.g. car). Reaction times were measured in response to the visually presented stimuli. Results showed a right-hand advantage for graspable objects only when participants were asked to respond to the graspable/non-graspable categorization. When participants were asked to categorize objects as nature-made or man-made, a right-hand advantage for graspable objects did not emerge. The results suggest that motor affordances may not always be automatic and might require conscious representations that are appropriate for object interaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole Netelenbos
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, University of Lethbridge, 4401 University Drive W, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 3M4, Canada.
| | - Claudia L R Gonzalez
- The Brain in Action Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, University of Lethbridge, 4401 University Drive W, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 3M4, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|