1
|
Fargier R, Laganaro M. Referential and inferential production across the lifespan: different patterns and different predictive cognitive factors. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1237523. [PMID: 38022984 PMCID: PMC10643179 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1237523] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2023] [Accepted: 09/18/2023] [Indexed: 12/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction The ability to speak is grounded in general memory and control processes and likely changes across the lifespan. However, our knowledge on how word production abilities naturally evolve from childhood to old age remains marginally investigated. Our aim was to shed further light on this issue by exploiting the contrast between two ways to elicit word production: referential picture naming and inferential naming from definition. Methods We collected accuracy and production latencies in a picture naming task and in a naming from definition task from 130 participants ranging from 10 to 80 years old. Measures of vocabulary size, digit span memory, semantic and phonemic fluencies and processing speed were also collected. We used multivariate adaptative regression splines and regression models to characterize lifespan patterns of the two tasks. Results Patterns of increase in performance were similar for picture naming and naming from definition only from childhood to young adulthood. In the second half of the lifespan, significant decrease of performance was found in older adults for picture naming (from around 60 years-old) but not for naming from definition. Clearly, word production elicited with an inferential task (naming from definition) yields different age-related patterns than usually described in the literature with a referential task (picture naming). Discussion We discuss how cognitive processes such as visual-conceptual processes and lexical prediction may explain the differential pattern of results in aging in referential and inferential production tasks. We argue for more lifespan studies and the need to investigate language production beyond picture naming, in particular with respect to aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Marina Laganaro
- Neuropsycholinguistics Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Purpura A, Giorgianni D, Orrù G, Melis G, Sartori G. Identifying single-item faked responses in personality tests: A new TF-IDF-based method. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0272970. [PMID: 36007085 PMCID: PMC9410542 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0272970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2021] [Accepted: 07/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Faking in a psychological test is often observed whenever an examinee may gain an advantage from it. Although techniques are available to identify a faker, they cannot identify the specific questions distorted by faking. This work evaluates the effectiveness of term frequency-inverse document frequency (TF-IDF)—an information retrieval mathematical tool used in search engines and language representations—in identifying single-item faked responses. We validated the technique on three datasets containing responses to the 10-item Big Five questionnaire (total of 694 participants, respectively 221, 243, and 230) in three faking situations. Each participant responded twice, once faking to achieve an objective in one of three contexts (one to obtain child custody and two to land a job) and once honestly. The proposed TF-IDF model has proven very effective in separating honest from dishonest responses—with the honest ones having low TF-IDF values and the dishonest ones having higher values—and in identifying which of the 10 responses to the questionnaire were distorted in the dishonest condition. We also provide examples of the technique in a single-case evaluation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Dora Giorgianni
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| | - Graziella Orrù
- Department of Surgical, Medical, Molecular and Critical Area Pathology, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
| | - Giulia Melis
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
| | - Giuseppe Sartori
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Nickels L, Lampe LF, Mason C, Hameau S. Investigating the influence of semantic factors on word retrieval: Reservations, results and recommendations. Cogn Neuropsychol 2022; 39:113-154. [PMID: 35972430 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2022.2109958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
There is consensus that word retrieval starts with activation of semantic representations. However, in adults without language impairment, relatively little attention has been paid to the effects of the semantic attributes of to-be-retrieved words. This paper, therefore, addresses the question of which item-inherent semantic factors influence word retrieval. Specifically, it reviews the literature on a selection of these factors: imageability, concreteness, number of semantic features, typicality, intercorrelational density, featural distinctiveness, concept distinctiveness, animacy, semantic neighbourhood density, semantic similarity, operativity, valence, and arousal. It highlights several methodological challenges in this field, and has a focus on the insights from studies with people with aphasia where the effects of these variables are more prevalent. The paper concludes that further research simultaneously examining the effects of different semantic factors that are likely to affect lexical co-activation, and the interaction of these variables, would be fruitful, as would suitably scaled computational modelling of these effects in unimpaired language processing and in language impairment. Such research would enable the refinement of theories of semantic processing and word production, and potentially have implications for diagnosis and treatment of semantic and lexical impairments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lyndsey Nickels
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Macquarie Centre for Reading, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Leonie F Lampe
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Catherine Mason
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Macquarie Centre for Reading, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Solène Hameau
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia.,Macquarie Centre for Reading, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Abstract
Semantic feature production norms are a useful tool for researchers to have empirically collected data about the semantic representations of a particular population. As older adults have been shown to have certain differences in their semantic knowledge organization in comparison with younger adults, it is relevant for them to have their own normative data. Thus we present here the first Spanish semantic feature production norms for older adults. They contain information about the feature composition of 400 concrete concepts. We also provide information about some feature and concept variables as well as comparisons between young and old adults on these variables.
Collapse
|
5
|
Vivas J, Kogan B, Yerro M, Romanelli S, Vivas L. Describing the structure of concepts through different feature levels. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2020.1858840] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jorge Vivas
- Institute of Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National University of Mar del Plata, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Boris Kogan
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Faculty of Humanities, National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
| | - Matías Yerro
- Institute of Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National University of Mar del Plata, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Sofía Romanelli
- Faculty of Humanities, National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
| | - Leticia Vivas
- Institute of Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National University of Mar del Plata, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Navarro MC, Marmolejo-Ramos F, Vásquez V, Carrea B, Vélez JI, Mebarak Chams M. An Exploratory Study for Assessment of Multimodal Semantic Memory in Colombian Children. Int J Psychol Res (Medellin) 2020; 13:49-58. [PMID: 33329877 PMCID: PMC7735520 DOI: 10.21500/20112084.4847] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Semantic memory (SM) is a type of long-term memory associated with the storage of general information about the world. Here we assessed the characteristics of the SM battery, developed by Catricalà et al. (2013), in a sample of Colombian children. This battery was originally conceived to evaluate adults, and features six subtests that assess SM in different modalities, using a common set of 48 stimuli in both living and nonliving categories. The design of the current study is of a cross-sectional and exploratory type. The sample was composed of 111 children, 57 boys (51%) and 54 girls (49%), who were 6 (n = 68) and 7 (n = 43) years old and had no intellectual disability. Robust linear regression models and correlation networks were used. We found an effect of age on general intelligence after correcting for gender, and no differences on the six subtest scores after corrections for gender and age were performed. Furthermore, age was found to be positively associated with the naming of colored photographs (β = .75, p = .039), naming in response to an oral description (β = 1.81, p = .039), picture sorting at four levels (β = 7.22, p = .029), and sentence verification (β = 26.66, p = .01). In addition, there were differences between the results obtained in adults in the original study and in the children of our study. This exploratory study supports the feasibility of the Spanish translation of the Catricalà et al. (2013) battery to assess SM in children with a nonclinical condition. Future studies are needed to evaluate the psychometric properties of this SM battery, and to corroborate and expand our findings in a larger sample of control children, and in children with some degree of intellectual disability or suffering of some neurodegenerative or psychiatric conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- María C Navarro
- . Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia. Universidad del Norte Universidad del Norte Barranquilla Colombia
| | - Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos
- . Center for Change and Complexity in Learning, The University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia. University of South Australia University of South Australia Adelaide Australia
| | - Valentina Vásquez
- . Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia. Universidad del Norte Universidad del Norte Barranquilla Colombia
| | - Bárbara Carrea
- Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia. Pontificia Universidad Javeriana Pontificia Universidad Javeriana Bogotá Colombia
| | - Jorge I Vélez
- . Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia. Universidad del Norte Universidad del Norte Barranquilla Colombia
| | - Moisés Mebarak Chams
- . Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia. Universidad del Norte Universidad del Norte Barranquilla Colombia
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Antonucci LA, Nettis MA, Juckel G, Roser P, Pergola G, Thoma P. Selective recall deficits for heterogeneous associations in detoxified individuals with alcohol use disorder. Behav Brain Res 2020; 390:112688. [PMID: 32407822 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112688] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2020] [Revised: 04/29/2020] [Accepted: 05/01/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
AIM Alcohol use disorder (AUD) has been associated with recognition memory deficits. However, it remains unclear whether these deficits occur at the general recognition memory level or whether they selectively affect its subcomponents. Evidence suggests that recognition memory deficits may vary according to the heterogeneity of memory stimuli. Our aim was to investigate stimuli pair-dependent recognition memory deficits in AUD, using a cued recall task including homogeneous and heterogeneous stimuli pairs. METHODS Twenty-three patients with AUD (days since last alcohol consumption: 11.70 ± 10.20) and 23 healthy controls (HC) underwent a neuropsychological examination tapping attention, verbal fluency, logical, working and long-term memory, and a recognition and recall task involving both homogeneous (tool-tool) and heterogeneous (tool-scene, tool-animal) stimuli pairs. Group (AUD-HC) by condition (tool-tool, tool-scene, tool-animal) ANOVAs were performed on all neuropsychological indices. RESULTS In the neuropsychological examination, AUD individuals showed deficits in delayed recall and faster reaction times compared with HC. The administration of the recognition and recall task revealed specific performance decreases in cued recall occurring in the whole sample (AUD + HC) for heterogeneous (tool-scene, tool-animal) pairs compared with homogeneous pairs. Within this pattern, AUD patients showed a lower cued recall rate than HC only for tool-animal pairs. CONCLUSIONS Our results support the existence of specific recall/recollection deficits in AUD which occurred for heterogeneous, but not for homogeneous stimuli pairs. This finding calls for further neuroimaging investigations aimed at investigating the neurobiological substrate of (i) different recognition memory subcomponents, and (ii) the processing of stimuli with different degree or type of heterogeneity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Linda A Antonucci
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy; Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience and Sense Organs, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy; Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians University, Munich, Germany.
| | - Maria A Nettis
- Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, United Kingdom
| | - Georg Juckel
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Preventive Medicine, LWL University Hospital, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| | - Patrik Roser
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Preventive Medicine, LWL University Hospital, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| | - Giulio Pergola
- Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience and Sense Organs, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy; Clinical Neuropsychology, Neuropsychological Therapy Centre, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany; Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Baltimore, USA
| | - Patrizia Thoma
- Clinical Neuropsychology, Neuropsychological Therapy Centre, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Guest O, Caso A, Cooper RP. On Simulating Neural Damage in Connectionist Networks. COMPUTATIONAL BRAIN & BEHAVIOR 2020; 3:289-321. [PMID: 32766512 PMCID: PMC7381482 DOI: 10.1007/s42113-020-00081-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
A key strength of connectionist modelling is its ability to simulate both intact cognition and the behavioural effects of neural damage. We survey the literature, showing that models have been damaged in a variety of ways, e.g. by removing connections, by adding noise to connection weights, by scaling weights, by removing units and by adding noise to unit activations. While these different implementations of damage have often been assumed to be behaviourally equivalent, some theorists have made aetiological claims that rest on nonequivalence. They suggest that related deficits with different aetiologies might be accounted for by different forms of damage within a single model. We present two case studies that explore the effects of different forms of damage in two influential connectionist models, each of which has been applied to explain neuropsychological deficits. Our results indicate that the effect of simulated damage can indeed be sensitive to the way in which damage is implemented, particularly when the environment comprises subsets of items that differ in their statistical properties, but such effects are sensitive to relatively subtle aspects of the model's training environment. We argue that, as a consequence, substantial methodological care is required if aetiological claims about simulated neural damage are to be justified, and conclude more generally that implementation assumptions, including those concerning simulated damage, must be fully explored when evaluating models of neurological deficits, both to avoid over-extending the explanatory power of specific implementations and to ensure that reported results are replicable. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL The online version of this article (10.1007/s42113-020-00081-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olivia Guest
- Research Centre on Interactive Media, Smart Systems and Emerging Technologies — RISE, Nicosia, Cyprus
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Andrea Caso
- Center for Cognition, Computation and Modelling, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, London, UK
| | - Richard P. Cooper
- Center for Cognition, Computation and Modelling, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Vivas L, Montefinese M, Bolognesi M, Vivas J. Core features: measures and characterization for different languages. Cogn Process 2020; 21:651-667. [PMID: 32333125 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-020-00969-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2019] [Accepted: 03/17/2020] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
According to the feature-based view of semantic representation, concepts can be represented as distributed networks of semantic features, which contribute with different weights to determine the overall meaning of a concept. The study of semantic features, typically collected in property generation tasks, is enriched with measures indicating the informativeness and distinctiveness of a given feature for the related concepts. However, while these measures have been provided in several languages (e.g. Italian, Spanish and English), they have hardly been applied comparatively across languages. The purpose of this paper is to investigate language-related differences and similarities emerging from the semantic representation of aggregated core features. Features with higher salience for a set of concrete concepts are identified and described in terms of their feature type. Then, comparisons are made between domains (natural vs. artefacts) and languages (Italian, Spanish and English) and descriptive statistics are provided. These results show that the characterization of concrete concepts is overall fairly stable across languages, although interesting cross-linguistic differences emerged. We will discuss the implications of our findings in relation to the theoretical paradigm of semantic feature norms, as well as in relation to speakers' mutual understanding in multilingual settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leticia Vivas
- UNMDP, CONICET, Psychology Faculty, Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology Institute (IPSIBAT), Funes 3250, CP 7600, Mar del Plata, Argentina.
| | - Maria Montefinese
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 12, 35131, Padua, Italy.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London, WC1H 0AP, UK
| | - Marianna Bolognesi
- Faculty of Languages, Literatures and Modern Cultures, University of Bologna, Via Cartoleira 5, Bologna, Italy
| | - Jorge Vivas
- UNMDP, CONICET, Psychology Faculty, Basic and Applied Psychology and Technology Institute (IPSIBAT), Funes 3250, CP 7600, Mar del Plata, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Buchanan EM, De Deyne S, Montefinese M. A practical primer on processing semantic property norm data. Cogn Process 2019; 21:587-599. [PMID: 31768704 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-019-00939-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2019] [Accepted: 10/24/2019] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Semantic property listing tasks require participants to generate short propositions (e.g., [Formula: see text], [Formula: see text]) for a specific concept (e.g., DOG). This task is the cornerstone of the creation of semantic property norms which are essential for modeling, stimuli creation, and understanding similarity between concepts. Despite the wide applicability of semantic property norms for a large variety of concepts across different groups of people, the methodological aspects of the property listing task have received less attention, even though the procedure and processing of the data can substantially affect the nature and quality of the measures derived from them. The goal of this paper is to provide a practical primer on how to collect and process semantic property norms. We will discuss the key methods to elicit semantic properties and compare different methods to derive meaningful representations from them. This will cover the role of instructions and test context, property preprocessing (e.g., lemmatization), property weighting, and relationship encoding using ontologies. With these choices in mind, we propose and demonstrate a processing pipeline that transparently documents these steps, resulting in improved comparability across different studies. The impact of these choices will be demonstrated using intrinsic (e.g., reliability, number of properties) and extrinsic measures (e.g., categorization, semantic similarity, lexical processing). This practical primer will offer potential solutions to several long-standing problems and allow researchers to develop new property listing norms overcoming the constraints of previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin M Buchanan
- Harrisburg University of Science and Technology, 326 Market St., Harrisburg, PA, 17101, USA.
| | | | - Maria Montefinese
- University of Padova, Padua, Italy.,University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Montefinese M, Hallam G, Thompson HE, Jefferies E. The interplay between control processes and feature relevance: Evidence from dual-task methodology. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2019; 73:384-395. [PMID: 31476964 DOI: 10.1177/1747021819877163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Neuropsychological studies suggest a distinction between (a) semantic knowledge and (b) control processes that shape the retrieval of conceptual information to suit the task or context. These aspects of semantic cognition are specifically impaired in patients with semantic dementia and semantic aphasia, respectively. However, interactions between the structure of knowledge and control processes that are expected during semantic retrieval have not been fully characterised. In particular, domain-general executive resources may not have equal relevance for the capacity to promote weak yet task relevant features (i.e., "controlled retrieval) and to ignore or suppress distracting information (i.e., "selection"). Here, using a feature selection task, we tested the contribution of featural relevance to semantic performance in healthy participants under conditions of divided attention. Healthy participants showed greater dual-task disruption as the relevance value of the distractor feature linearly increased, supporting the emerging view that semantic relevance is one of the organising principles of the structure of semantic representation. Moreover, word frequency, and inter-correlational strength affected overall performance, but they did not show an interaction with dual-task conditions. These results suggest that domain-general control processes, disrupted by divided attention, are more important to the capacity to efficiently avoid distracting information during semantic decision-making than to the promotion of weak target features. The present study therefore provides novel information about the nature of the interaction between structured conceptual knowledge and control processes that support the retrieval of appropriate information and relates these results to a new theoretical framework, termed controlled semantic cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Montefinese
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK.,Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Glyn Hallam
- Department of Psychology, University of York, York, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Ursino M, Cuppini C, Cappa SF, Catricalà E. A feature-based neurocomputational model of semantic memory. Cogn Neurodyn 2018; 12:525-547. [PMID: 30483362 PMCID: PMC6233327 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-018-9494-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2018] [Revised: 06/19/2018] [Accepted: 06/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
According with a featural organization of semantic memory, this work is aimed at investigating, through an attractor network, the role of different kinds of features in the representation of concepts, both in normal and neurodegenerative conditions. We implemented new synaptic learning rules in order to take into account the role of partially shared features and of distinctive features with different saliency. The model includes semantic and lexical layers, coding, respectively for object features and word-forms. Connections among nodes are strongly asymmetrical. To account for the feature saliency, asymmetrical synapses were created using Hebbian rules of potentiation and depotentiation, setting different pre-synaptic and post-synaptic thresholds. A variable post-synaptic threshold, which automatically changed to reflect the feature frequency in different concepts (i.e., how many concepts share a feature), was used to account for partially shared features. The trained network solved naming tasks and word recognition tasks very well, exploiting the different role of salient versus marginal features in concept identification. In the case of damage, superordinate concepts were preserved better than the subordinate ones. Interestingly, the degradation of salient features, but not of marginal ones, prevented object identification. The model suggests that Hebbian rules, with adjustable post-synaptic thresholds, can provide a reliable semantic representation of objects exploiting the statistics of input features.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mauro Ursino
- Department of Electrical, Electronic and Information Engineering, University of Bologna, Viale Risorgimento 2, 40136 Bologna, Italy
| | - Cristiano Cuppini
- Department of Electrical, Electronic and Information Engineering, University of Bologna, Viale Risorgimento 2, 40136 Bologna, Italy
| | - Stefano F. Cappa
- NEtS Center, Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS, Pavia, Italy
- IRCCS S. Giovanni di Dio, Brescia, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Boboeva V, Brasselet R, Treves A. The Capacity for Correlated Semantic Memories in the Cortex. ENTROPY 2018; 20:e20110824. [PMID: 33266548 PMCID: PMC7512385 DOI: 10.3390/e20110824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2018] [Revised: 10/11/2018] [Accepted: 10/23/2018] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
A statistical analysis of semantic memory should reflect the complex, multifactorial structure of the relations among its items. Still, a dominant paradigm in the study of semantic memory has been the idea that the mental representation of concepts is structured along a simple branching tree spanned by superordinate and subordinate categories. We propose a generative model of item representation with correlations that overcomes the limitations of a tree structure. The items are generated through “factors” that represent semantic features or real-world attributes. The correlation between items has its source in the extent to which items share such factors and the strength of such factors: if many factors are balanced, correlations are overall low; whereas if a few factors dominate, they become strong. Our model allows for correlations that are neither trivial nor hierarchical, but may reproduce the general spectrum of correlations present in a dataset of nouns. We find that such correlations reduce the storage capacity of a Potts network to a limited extent, so that the number of concepts that can be stored and retrieved in a large, human-scale cortical network may still be of order 107, as originally estimated without correlations. When this storage capacity is exceeded, however, retrieval fails completely only for balanced factors; above a critical degree of imbalance, a phase transition leads to a regime where the network still extracts considerable information about the cued item, even if not recovering its detailed representation: partial categorization seems to emerge spontaneously as a consequence of the dominance of particular factors, rather than being imposed ad hoc. We argue this to be a relevant model of semantic memory resilience in Tulving’s remember/know paradigms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vezha Boboeva
- Cognitive Neuroscience, SISSA—International School for Advanced Studies, Via Bonomea 265, 34136 Trieste, Italy
| | - Romain Brasselet
- Cognitive Neuroscience, SISSA—International School for Advanced Studies, Via Bonomea 265, 34136 Trieste, Italy
| | - Alessandro Treves
- Cognitive Neuroscience, SISSA—International School for Advanced Studies, Via Bonomea 265, 34136 Trieste, Italy
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience/Centre for Neural Computation, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
- Correspondence:
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Chen H, Ning X, Wang L, Yang J. Acquiring New Factual Information: Effect of Prior Knowledge. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1734. [PMID: 30319482 PMCID: PMC6165899 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01734] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2018] [Accepted: 08/27/2018] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
One influential theory on object knowledge is feature-based model, which proposes that the object knowledge is organized by different feature types, such as sensory/perceptual and motor/functional ones. Previous studies have shown that prior knowledge enhances the processes of acquiring and remembering relevant information. However, whether the effect of prior knowledge is applied to different types of conceptual information over time remains unclear. In this study, we addressed this question by testing memory of different types of object features at various retention intervals. The level of prior knowledge was manipulated as object features from familiar and unfamiliar categories. In Experiments 1 and 2, sentences that described the perceptual and functional features of new words were presented. Sentences with episodic features were additionally presented in Experiment 2. The participants were then tested with recognition (Experiment 1) and recall (Experiment 2) tasks at different retention intervals. The results showed that prior knowledge enhanced memory for perceptual features but not for functional and episodic features. Such enhancement depended on the recollection process. In addition, the effect of prior knowledge on perceptual features remained stable over time. This study clarified how different types of new factual information were acquired and maintained and highlighted the importance of prior knowledge in acquiring new conceptual knowledge with the passage of time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haoyu Chen
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xueling Ning
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Lingwei Wang
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Jiongjiong Yang
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
Semantic feature norms (e.g., STIMULUS: car → RESPONSE: <has four wheels>) are commonly used in cognitive psychology to look into salient aspects of given concepts. Semantic features are typically collected in experimental settings and then manually annotated by the researchers into feature types (e.g., perceptual features, taxonomic features, etc.) by means of content analyses-that is, by using taxonomies of feature types and having independent coders perform the annotation task. However, the ways in which such content analyses are typically performed and reported are not consistent across the literature. This constitutes a serious methodological problem that might undermine the theoretical claims based on such annotations. In this study, we first offer a review of some of the released datasets of annotated semantic feature norms and the related taxonomies used for content analysis. We then provide theoretical and methodological insights in relation to the content analysis methodology. Finally, we apply content analysis to a new dataset of semantic features and show how the method should be applied in order to deliver reliable annotations and replicable coding schemes. We tackle the following issues: (1) taxonomy structure, (2) the description of categories, (3) coder training, and (4) sustainability of the coding scheme-that is, comparison of the annotations provided by trained versus novice coders. The outcomes of the project are threefold: We provide methodological guidelines for semantic feature classification; we provide a revised and adapted taxonomy that can (arguably) be applied to both concrete and abstract concepts; and we provide a dataset of annotated semantic feature norms.
Collapse
|
16
|
|
17
|
Abstract
How are the meanings of words, events, and objects represented and organized in the brain? This question, perhaps more than any other in the field, probes some of the deepest and most foundational puzzles regarding the structure of the mind and brain. Accordingly, it has spawned a field of inquiry that is diverse and multidisciplinary, has led to the discovery of numerous empirical phenomena, and has spurred the development of a wide range of theoretical positions. This special issue brings together the most recent theoretical developments from the leaders in the field, representing a range of viewpoints on issues of fundamental significance to a theory of meaning representation. Here we introduce the special issue by way of pulling out some key themes that cut across the contributions that form this issue and situating those themes in the broader literature. The core issues around which research on conceptual representation can be organized are representational format, representational content, the organization of concepts in the brain, and the processing dynamics that govern interactions between the conceptual system and sensorimotor representations. We highlight areas in which consensus has formed; for those areas in which opinion is divided, we seek to clarify the relation of theory and evidence and to set in relief the bridging assumptions that undergird current discussions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bradford Z Mahon
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Meliora Hall, Rochester, NY, 14627-0268, USA.
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA.
- Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA.
- Center for Language Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA.
| | - Gregory Hickok
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Spatio-temporal Dynamics of Referential and Inferential Naming: Different Brain and Cognitive Operations to Lexical Selection. Brain Topogr 2016; 30:182-197. [PMID: 27334987 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-016-0504-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2016] [Accepted: 06/15/2016] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
Picture naming tasks are largely used to elicit the production of specific words and sentences in psycholinguistic and neuroimaging research. However, the generation of lexical concepts from a visual input is clearly not the exclusive way speech production is triggered. In inferential speech encoding, the concept is not provided from a visual input, but is elaborated though semantic and/or episodic associations. It is therefore likely that the cognitive operations leading to lexical selection and word encoding are different in inferential and referential expressive language. In particular, in picture naming lexical selection might ensue from a simple association between a perceptual visual representation and a word with minimal semantic processes, whereas richer semantic associations are involved in lexical retrieval in inferential situations. Here we address this hypothesis by analyzing ERP correlates during word production in a referential and an inferential task. The participants produced the same words elicited from pictures or from short written definitions. The two tasks displayed similar electrophysiological patterns only in the time-period preceding the verbal response. In the stimulus-locked ERPs waveform amplitudes and periods of stable global electrophysiological patterns differed across tasks after the P100 component and until 400-500 ms, suggesting the involvement of different, task-specific neural networks. Based on the analysis of the time-windows affected by specific semantic and lexical variables in each task, we conclude that lexical selection is underpinned by a different set of conceptual and brain processes, with semantic processes clearly preceding word retrieval in naming from definition whereas the semantic information is enriched in parallel with word retrieval in picture naming.
Collapse
|
19
|
Xiao X, Dong Q, Chen C, Xue G. Neural pattern similarity underlies the mnemonic advantages for living words. Cortex 2016; 79:99-111. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Revised: 01/08/2016] [Accepted: 03/16/2016] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
|
20
|
Vivas L, MacIntyre M, Ricci L, Vivas J. Psycholinguistic variables involved in concept recall from the successive presentation of features / Variables psicolingüísticas involucradas en la evocación de conceptos a partir de la presentación de atributos sucesivos. STUDIES IN PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/02109395.2015.1078558] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022] Open
|
21
|
Zannino GD, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. The contribution of neurodegenerative diseases to the modelling of semantic memory: A new proposal and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia 2015; 75:274-90. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2015] [Revised: 06/15/2015] [Accepted: 06/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
22
|
Catricalà E, Della Rosa PA, Plebani V, Perani D, Garrard P, Cappa SF. Semantic feature degradation and naming performance. Evidence from neurodegenerative disorders. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2015; 147:58-65. [PMID: 26072002 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2014] [Revised: 04/13/2015] [Accepted: 05/18/2015] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
The failure to name an object in Alzheimer's disease (AD) and in the semantic variant of the primary progressive aphasia (sv-PPA) has been generally attributed to semantic memory loss, with a progressive degradation of semantic features. Not all features, however, may have the same relevance in picture naming. We analyzed the relationship between picture naming performance and the loss of semantic features in patients with AD with or without naming impairment, with sv-PPA and in matched controls, assessing the role of distinctiveness, semantic relevance and feature type (sensorial versus non-sensorial) with a sentence verification task. The results showed that distinctive features with high values of semantic relevance were lost only in all patients with naming impairment. The performance on the sensorial distinctive features with high relevance was the best predictor of naming performance only in sv-PPA, while no difference between sensorial and non-sensorial features was found in AD patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Pasquale A Della Rosa
- Institute of Molecular Bioimaging and Physiology, National Research Council (IBFM-CNR), Milan, Italy
| | - Valentina Plebani
- Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy
| | - Daniela Perani
- Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy; Nuclear Medicine Department, San Raffaele Hospital and Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy
| | - Peter Garrard
- Neuroscience Research Centre, Institute of Cardiovascular and Cell Sciences, St George's, University of London, Cranmer Terrace, London, UK
| | - Stefano F Cappa
- Institute for Advanced Study-IUSS Pavia, Pavia, Italy; Division of Neuroscience, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
A neural network for learning the meaning of objects and words from a featural representation. Neural Netw 2015; 63:234-53. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet.2014.11.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2013] [Revised: 11/21/2014] [Accepted: 11/25/2014] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
24
|
Catricalà E, Della Rosa PA, Plebani V, Vigliocco G, Cappa SF. Abstract and concrete categories? Evidences from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia 2014; 64:271-81. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2013] [Revised: 08/10/2014] [Accepted: 09/24/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
25
|
Abstract
Inductive inferences about objects, features, categories, and relations have been studied for many years, but there are few attempts to chart the range of inductive problems that humans are able to solve. We present a taxonomy of inductive problems that helps to clarify the relationships between familiar inductive problems such as generalization, categorization, and identification, and that introduces new inductive problems for psychological investigation. Our taxonomy is founded on the idea that semantic knowledge is organized into systems of objects, features, categories, and relations, and we attempt to characterize all of the inductive problems that can arise when these systems are partially observed. Recent studies have begun to address some of the new problems in our taxonomy, and future work should aim to develop unified theories of inductive reasoning that explain how people solve all of the problems in the taxonomy.
Collapse
|
26
|
Bruffaerts R, De Weer AS, De Grauwe S, Thys M, Dries E, Thijs V, Sunaert S, Vandenbulcke M, De Deyne S, Storms G, Vandenberghe R. Noun and knowledge retrieval for biological and non-biological entities following right occipitotemporal lesions. Neuropsychologia 2014; 62:163-74. [PMID: 25080190 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2013] [Revised: 07/11/2014] [Accepted: 07/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
We investigated the critical contribution of right ventral occipitotemporal cortex to knowledge of visual and functional-associative attributes of biological and non-biological entities and how this relates to category-specificity during confrontation naming. In a consecutive series of 7 patients with lesions confined to right ventral occipitotemporal cortex, we conducted an extensive assessment of oral generation of visual-sensory and functional-associative features in response to the names of biological and nonbiological entities. Subjects also performed a confrontation naming task for these categories. Our main novel finding related to a unique case with a small lesion confined to right medial fusiform gyrus who showed disproportionate naming impairment for nonbiological versus biological entities, specifically for tools. Generation of visual and functional-associative features was preserved for biological and non-biological entities. In two other cases, who had a relatively small posterior lesion restricted to primary visual and posterior fusiform cortex, retrieval of visual attributes was disproportionately impaired compared to functional-associative attributes, in particular for biological entities. However, these cases did not show a category-specific naming deficit. Two final cases with the largest lesions showed a classical dissociation between biological versus nonbiological entities during naming, with normal feature generation performance. This is the first lesion-based evidence of a critical contribution of the right medial fusiform cortex to tool naming. Second, dissociations along the dimension of attribute type during feature generation do not co-occur with category-specificity during naming in the current patient sample.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rose Bruffaerts
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium; Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - An-Sofie De Weer
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium
| | - Sophie De Grauwe
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium; Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Miek Thys
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium
| | - Eva Dries
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium
| | - Vincent Thijs
- Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Stefan Sunaert
- Radiology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Mathieu Vandenbulcke
- Brain and Emotion Laboratory Leuven, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium; Psychiatry Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Simon De Deyne
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Gerrit Storms
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Rik Vandenberghe
- Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Belgium; Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Chen L, Rogers TT. Revisiting domain-general accounts of category specificity in mind and brain. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2014; 5:327-44. [PMID: 26308567 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1283] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2013] [Revised: 11/14/2013] [Accepted: 01/15/2014] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Theories about the neural basis of semantic knowledge have been strongly influenced by reports that particular semantic categories can be differentially impaired by neuropathology and can differentially activate particular regions of cortex in brain imaging studies. One well-known interpretation of these data is that the brain has evolved distinct functional and anatomical modules for storing and retrieving knowledge about different kinds of things. We review the evidence supporting an alternative view: that category specificity arises from many heterogeneous factors and so tells us little directly about the cognitive and neural architecture of semantic memory. We consider four general hypotheses about domain-general causes of category-specific patterns, their roots in early work, and their reemergence in contemporary research. We argue that there is compelling evidence supporting each hypothesis, and that the different hypotheses together can explain most of the interesting data. We further suggest that such a multifactor domain-general approach to category specificity is appealing partly because it explains the important findings with reference to theoretical claims that are already widely accepted, and partly because it resolves several puzzles that arise under the alternative view. For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The authors have declared no conflicts of interest for this article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lang Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Timothy T Rogers
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
First come, last primed: FN400 reflects post-encoding editing of the memory trace. Behav Brain Res 2014; 266:63-76. [PMID: 24631391 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2013] [Revised: 02/23/2014] [Accepted: 02/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Explicit associative memory relies on different neural substrates depending on similarity of the items associated. Extant literature dissociates old/new effects elicited by homogeneous and heterogeneous pairs. However, current results scarcely address potential priming effects induced by the presentation sequence. In the present ERP study, participants learned associations between two pictures, which either belonged to the same semantic category (e.g., animal-animal), or to different categories (e.g., animal-scene). Pictures forming a pair were shown sequentially, allowing for investigation of the different neural processes related to presentation of the first and the second item. After the study phase, participants performed a recognition judgment. After recognition, participants were asked to recall the associated picture. During retrieval, between 260 and 350 ms post-stimulus there was a significant frontal effect of category (i.e., same-category versus different-category), but only for items shown first within a pair. In the 350-600 ms time window the parietal old/new effect was unaffected by semantic category, but was modulated by presentation order. Exploratory analyses revealed even earlier effects in the time windows 40-90 ms and 150-200 ms. This evidence supports the priming account of the FN400 and highlights the importance of sequence effects in electrophysiological activity during episodic retrieval.
Collapse
|
29
|
Reilly J, Harnish S, Garcia A, Hung J, Rodriguez AD, Crosson B. Lesion symptom mapping of manipulable object naming in nonfluent aphasia: can a brain be both embodied and disembodied? Cogn Neuropsychol 2014; 31:287-312. [PMID: 24839997 PMCID: PMC4091963 DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2014.914022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Embodied cognition offers an approach to word meaning firmly grounded in action and perception. A strong prediction of embodied cognition is that sensorimotor simulation is a necessary component of lexical-semantic representation. One semantic distinction where motor imagery is likely to play a key role involves the representation of manufactured artefacts. Many questions remain with respect to the scope of embodied cognition. One dominant unresolved issue is the extent to which motor enactment is necessary for representing and generating words with high motor salience. We investigated lesion correlates of manipulable relative to nonmanipulable name generation (e.g., name a school supply; name a mountain range) in patients with nonfluent aphasia (N = 14). Lesion volumes within motor (BA4, where BA = Brodmann area) and premotor (BA6) cortices were not predictive of category discrepancies. Lesion symptom mapping linked impairment for manipulable objects to polymodal convergence zones and to projections of the left, primary visual cortex specialized for motion perception (MT/V5+). Lesions to motor and premotor cortex were not predictive of manipulability impairment. This lesion correlation is incompatible with an embodied perspective premised on necessity of motor cortex for the enactment and subsequent production of motor-related words. These findings instead support a graded or "soft" approach to embodied cognition premised on an ancillary role of modality-specific cortical regions in enriching modality-neutral representations. We discuss a dynamic, hybrid approach to the neurobiology of semantic memory integrating both embodied and disembodied components.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jamie Reilly
- Eleanor M. Saffran Center for Cognitive Neuroscience Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania USA
| | - Stacy Harnish
- Department of Speech and Hearing Science The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio USA
| | - Amanda Garcia
- Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida USA
| | - Jinyi Hung
- Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida USA
| | - Amy D. Rodriguez
- Centre for Clinical Research Excellence in Aphasia Rehabilitation University of Queensland, Queensland, Australia
| | - Bruce Crosson
- Department of Neurology Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia USA
- Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation Atlanta VA Medical Center, Decatur, Georgia USA
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Seidenberg M, Kay CD, Woodard JL, Nielson KA, Smith JC, Kandah C, Guidotti Breting LM, Novitski J, Lancaster M, Matthews M, Hantke N, Butts A, Rao SM. Recognition of famous names predicts cognitive decline in healthy elders. Neuropsychology 2013; 27:333-42. [PMID: 23688215 DOI: 10.1037/a0032226] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The ability to recognize familiar people is impaired in both Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's Dementia (AD). In addition, both groups often demonstrate a time-limited temporal gradient (TG) in which well known people from decades earlier are better recalled than those learned recently. In this study, we examined the TG in cognitively intact elders for remote famous names (1950-1965) compared to more recent famous names (1995-2005). We hypothesized that the TG pattern on a famous name recognition task (FNRT) would predict future cognitive decline, and also show a significant correlation with hippocampal volume. METHOD Seventy-eight healthy elders (ages 65-90) with age-appropriate cognitive functioning at baseline were administered a FNRT. Follow-up testing 18 months later produced two groups: Declining (≥ 1 SD reduction on at least one of three measures) and Stable (< 1 SD). RESULTS The Declining group (N = 27) recognized fewer recent famous names than the Stable group (N = 51), although recognition for remote names was comparable. Baseline MRI volumes for both the left and right hippocampi were significantly smaller in the Declining group than the Stable group. Smaller baseline hippocampal volume was also significantly correlated with poorer performance for recent, but not remote famous names. Logistic regression analyses indicated that baseline TG performance was a significant predictor of group status (Declining vs. Stable) independent of chronological age and APOE ε4 inheritance. CONCLUSIONS The TG for famous name recognition may serve as an early preclinical cognitive marker of cognitive decline in healthy older individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Seidenberg
- Department of Psychology, Rosalind Franklin University, North Chicago, IL 60064, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
|
32
|
|
33
|
An Italian battery for the assessment of semantic memory disorders. Neurol Sci 2012; 34:985-93. [DOI: 10.1007/s10072-012-1181-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2012] [Accepted: 08/22/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
34
|
Cappelletti M, Butterworth B, Kopelman M. Numeracy skills in patients with degenerative disorders and focal brain lesions: a neuropsychological investigation. Neuropsychology 2012; 26:1-19. [PMID: 22122516 PMCID: PMC3248328 DOI: 10.1037/a0026328] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2010] [Revised: 09/29/2011] [Accepted: 10/03/2011] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To characterize the numerical profile of patients with acquired brain disorders. METHOD We investigated numeracy skills in 76 participants--40 healthy controls and 36 patients with neurodegenerative disorders (Alzheimer dementia, frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, progressive aphasia) and with focal brain lesions affecting parietal, frontal, and temporal areas as in herpes simplex encephalitis (HSE). All patients were tested with the same comprehensive battery of paper-and-pencil and computerized tasks assessing numerical abilities and calculation. Degenerative and HSE patients also performed nonnumerical semantic tasks. RESULTS Our results, based on nonparametric group statistics as well as on the analysis of individual patients, and all highly significant, show that: (a) all patients, including those with parietal lesions--a key brain area for numeracy processing--had intact processing of number quantity; (b) patients with impaired semantic knowledge had much better preserved numerical knowledge; and (c) most patients showed impaired calculation skills, with the exception of most semantic dementia and HSE patients. CONCLUSION Our results allow us, for the first time, to characterize the numeracy skills in patients with a variety of neurological conditions and to suggest that the pattern of numerical performance can vary considerably across different neurological populations. Moreover, the selective sparing of calculation skills in most semantic dementia and HSE suggest that numerical abilities are an independent component of the semantic system. Finally, our data suggest that, besides the parietal areas, other brain regions might be critical to the understanding and processing of numerical concepts.
Collapse
|
35
|
Wierenga CE, Stricker NH, McCauley A, Simmons A, Jak AJ, Chang YL, Nation DA, Bangen KJ, Salmon DP, Bondi MW. Altered brain response for semantic knowledge in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 2010; 49:392-404. [PMID: 21163275 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2010] [Revised: 12/03/2010] [Accepted: 12/07/2010] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Abstract
Word retrieval deficits are common in Alzheimer's disease (AD) and are thought to reflect a degradation of semantic memory. Yet, the nature of semantic deterioration in AD and the underlying neural correlates of these semantic memory changes remain largely unknown. We examined the semantic memory impairment in AD by investigating the neural correlates of category knowledge (e.g., living vs. nonliving) and featural processing (global vs. local visual information). During event-related fMRI, 10 adults diagnosed with mild AD and 22 cognitively normal (CN) older adults named aloud items from three categories for which processing of specific visual features has previously been dissociated from categorical features. Results showed widespread group differences in the categorical representation of semantic knowledge in several language-related brain areas. For example, the right inferior frontal gyrus showed selective brain response for nonliving items in the CN group but living items in the AD group. Additionally, the AD group showed increased brain response for word retrieval irrespective of category in Broca's homologue in the right hemisphere and rostral cingulate cortex bilaterally, which suggests greater recruitment of frontally mediated neural compensatory mechanisms in the face of semantic alteration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christina E Wierenga
- Research Service, Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA 92161, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Kemp C, Chang KMK, Lombardi L. Category and feature identification. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010; 133:216-33. [PMID: 20080224 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.11.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2008] [Revised: 11/07/2009] [Accepted: 11/16/2009] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper considers a family of inductive problems where reasoners must identify familiar categories or features on the basis of limited information. Problems of this kind are encountered, for example, when word learners acquire novel labels for pre-existing concepts. We develop a probabilistic model of identification and evaluate it in three experiments. Our first two experiments explore problems where a single category or feature must be identified, and our third experiment explores cases where participants must combine several pieces of information in order to simultaneously identify a category and a feature. Humans readily solve all of these problems, and we show that our model accounts for human inferences better than several alternative approaches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charles Kemp
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Category-specific neural processing for naming pictures of animals and naming pictures of tools: an ALE meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2009; 48:409-18. [PMID: 19800353 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2009] [Revised: 09/23/2009] [Accepted: 09/25/2009] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Using activation-likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis, we identified brain areas that are invoked when people name pictures of animals and pictures of tools. We found that naming animals and naming tools invoked separate distributed networks in the brain. Specifically, we found that naming animals invoked greater responses than naming tools in frontal lobe structures that are typically modulated by emotional content and task demands, and in a number of visual areas in the ventral stream. In contrast, naming tools invoked greater responses in a different set of areas in the ventral stream than those invoked by naming animals. Naming tools also invoked greater responses than naming animals in motor areas in the frontal lobe as well as in sensory areas in the parietal lobe. The only overlapping sites of activation that we found for naming these two categories of objects were in the left pars triangularis, the left inferior temporal gyrus, and the left parahippocampal gyrus. Taken together, our meta-analysis reveals that animals and tools are categorically represented in visual areas but show convergence in higher-order associative areas in the temporal and frontal lobes in regions that are typically regarded as being involved in memory and/or semantic processing. Our results also reveal that naming tools not only engages visual areas in the ventral stream but also a fronto-parietal network associated with tool use. Whether or not this network associated with tool use contributes directly to recognition will require further investigation.
Collapse
|
38
|
Abstract
PURPOSE OF REVIEW The neural basis of semantic memory has not only theoretical interest, but also implications for several neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia. RECENT FINDINGS The main focus of functional imaging studies is to disentangle the contribution of several interacting factors to the landscape of cerebral activation observed in normal individuals performing tasks requiring access to semantic knowledge (e.g. picture naming, word comprehension sentence verification). The main factors found to play a role are the modality of stimulus presentation (e.g. visual, verbal); the domain to which the stimuli belong (e.g. concrete or abstract entities; natural kinds or artifacts); and the type of knowledge accessed (e.g. visual properties vs. action properties). Another area of investigation is the neural correlates of semantic processes, such as selection among alternatives or information retrieval. SUMMARY The results of imaging studies confirm the neuropsychological concept that semantic knowledge and its usage depend upon a widely distributed network of brain areas. Specificity within this extended network, which includes a number of areas involved in perceptual and action processing, as well as the language areas, is related both to the type of knowledge and to different conceptual domains. The heterogeneous pattern of semantic memory dysfunction in neurological disorder may be a consequence of this complex neural organization.
Collapse
|
39
|
Abstract
One of the most provocative and exciting issues in cognitive science is how neural specificity for semantic categories of common objects arises in the functional architecture of the brain. More than two decades of research on the neuropsychological phenomenon of category-specific semantic deficits has generated detailed claims about the organization and representation of conceptual knowledge. More recently, researchers have sought to test hypotheses developed on the basis of neuropsychological evidence with functional imaging. From those two fields, the empirical generalization emerges that object domain and sensory modality jointly constrain the organization of knowledge in the brain. At the same time, research within the embodied cognition framework has highlighted the need to articulate how information is communicated between the sensory and motor systems, and processes that represent and generalize abstract information. Those developments point toward a new approach for understanding category specificity in terms of the coordinated influences of diverse regions and cognitive systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bradford Z. Mahon
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02318
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, CIMeC, University of Trento, Rovereto (TN), Italy 38068
| | - Alfonso Caramazza
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02318
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, CIMeC, University of Trento, Rovereto (TN), Italy 38068
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Kropff E, Treves A. Uninformative memories will prevail: the storage of correlated representations and its consequences. HFSP JOURNAL 2007; 1:249-62. [PMID: 19404425 DOI: 10.2976/1.2793335] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2007] [Accepted: 09/11/2007] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Autoassociative networks were proposed in the 80's as simplified models of memory function in the brain, using recurrent connectivity with Hebbian plasticity to store patterns of neural activity that can be later recalled. This type of computation has been suggested to take place in the CA3 region of the hippocampus and at several levels in the cortex. One of the weaknesses of these models is their apparent inability to store correlated patterns of activity. We show, however, that a small and biologically plausible modification in the "learning rule" (associating to each neuron a plasticity threshold that reflects its popularity) enables the network to handle correlations. We study the stability properties of the resulting memories (in terms of their resistance to the damage of neurons or synapses), finding a novel property of autoassociative networks: not all memories are equally robust, and the most informative are also the most sensitive to damage. We relate these results to category-specific effects in semantic memory patients, where concepts related to "non-living things" are usually more resistant to brain damage than those related to "living things," a phenomenon suspected to be rooted in the correlation between representations of concepts in the cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emilio Kropff
- International School of Advanced Studies, Cognitive Neuroscience Sector, via Beirut 4 Trieste FVG, Italy 34014
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Zahn R, Garrard P, Talazko J, Gondan M, Bubrowski P, Juengling F, Slawik H, Dykierek P, Koester B, Hull M. Patterns of regional brain hypometabolism associated with knowledge of semantic features and categories in Alzheimer's disease. J Cogn Neurosci 2007; 18:2138-51. [PMID: 17129196 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Abstract
The study of semantic memory in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) has raised important questions about the representation of conceptual knowledge in the human brain. It is still unknown whether semantic memory impairments are caused by localized damage to specialized regions or by diffuse damage to distributed representations within nonspecialized brain areas. To our knowledge, there have been no direct correlations of neuroimaging of in vivo brain function in AD with performance on tasks differentially addressing visual and functional knowledge of living and nonliving concepts. We used a semantic verification task and resting 18-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography in a group of mild to moderate AD patients to investigate this issue. The four task conditions required semantic knowledge of (1) visual, (2) functional properties of living objects, and (3) visual or (4) functional properties of nonliving objects. Visual property verification of living objects was significantly correlated with left posterior fusiform gyrus metabolism (Brodmann's area [BA] 37/19). Effects of visual and functional property verification for non-living objects largely overlapped in the left anterior temporal (BA 38/20) and bilateral premotor areas (BA 6), with the visual condition extending more into left lateral precentral areas. There were no associations with functional property verification for living concepts. Our results provide strong support for anatomically separable representations of living and nonliving concepts, as well as visual feature knowledge of living objects, and against distributed accounts of semantic memory that view visual and functional features of living and nonliving objects as distributed across a common set of brain areas.
Collapse
|
42
|
Abstract
PURPOSE This article discusses a novel approach for treatment of lexical retrieval deficits in aphasia in which treatment begins with complex, rather than simple, lexical stimuli. This treatment considers the semantic complexity of items within semantic categories, with a focus on their featural detail. Method and Results Previous work on training items within animate categories (S. Kiran & C. K. Thompson, 2003b) and preliminary work aimed at items within inanimate categories are discussed in this article. Both these studies indicate that training atypical category items that entail features inherent in the category prototype as well as distinctive features that are not characteristic of the category prototype results in generalization to untrained typical examples which entail only features consistent with the category prototype. Conversely, training typical examples does not result in generalization to untrained atypical examples. In this article, it is argued that atypical items are more complex than typical items within a category, and a theoretical framework for this dimension of semantic complexity is discussed. Then, evidence from treatment studies that support this complexity hierarchy is presented. Potential patient- and stimulus-specific factors that may influence the success of this treatment approach are also discussed. CONCLUSIONS The applications of semantic complexity to treatment of additional semantic categories and functional applications of this approach are proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Swathi Kiran
- CMA 7.206, Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Spironelli C, Angrilli A. Language lateralization in phonological, semantic and orthographic tasks: A slow evoked potential study. Behav Brain Res 2006; 175:296-304. [PMID: 17045661 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2006] [Revised: 08/23/2006] [Accepted: 08/28/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Most of literature on language has shown how different word-classes activate distinct neural networks within linguistic cortical areas. The present investigation aimed to demonstrate that, by means of slow evoked potentials and using the same set of words in different tasks, it is possible to activate cortical networks that are spatially and temporally distinguished. Twenty healthy subjects had to evaluate, in a word pair matching session, whether two words rhymed (phonological task), were semantically related (semantic task) or were written in the same letter case (orthographic task). Slow wave amplitude was computed in three relevant time windows: the last 0.5 s of first word presentation (W1), the initial contingent negative variation (iCNV) and the terminal CNV (tCNV). During W1 and iCNV intervals, both the orthographic and the phonological tasks were left lateralized. Furthermore, the phonological task was more lateralized than the orthographic because of a greater inhibition of the right hemisphere, whereas the orthographic task was characterized by a greater bilateral posterior activation. During the tCNV, only the phonological task remained left lateralized while orthographic and semantic were bilaterally distributed. Although the use of the same set of words tends to activate widely overlapped networks, in the present research task manipulation was effective in demonstrating task dependent differences in brain lateralization. Thus, the present paradigm and the adopted tasks are especially suited for studying deficit and recovery of language in patients affected by linguistic disorders such as developmental dyslexia and aphasia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiara Spironelli
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Sartori G, Gnoato F, Mariani I, Prioni S, Lombardi L. Semantic relevance, domain specificity and the sensory/functional theory of category-specificity. Neuropsychologia 2006; 45:966-76. [PMID: 17069865 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2006] [Revised: 08/25/2006] [Accepted: 08/30/2006] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
According to the sensory/functional theory of semantic memory, Living items rely more on Sensory knowledge than Non-living ones. The sensory/functional explanation of category-specificity assumes that semantic features are organised on the basis of their content. We report here a study on DAT patients with impaired performance on Living items and tests of Sensory knowledge, and show that this impairment not only disappears after parcelling out semantic relevance, but is also reversed if this parameter is appropriately manipulated. Although semantic relevance model predicts these results [Sartori, G., & Lombardi, L. (2004). Semantic relevance and semantic disorders. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 439-452], they run counter to both the sensory/functional theory and the domain-specific theory of category-specific impairment.
Collapse
|
45
|
Morrow LI, Duffy MF. The representation of ontological category concepts as affected by healthy aging: normative data and theoretical implications. Behav Res Methods 2006; 37:608-25. [PMID: 16629293 DOI: 10.3758/bf03192731] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Attributes associated with concept representations, such as familiarity, typicality, and age of acquisition, have been shown to be important influences on lexical-semantic processing. In most previous studies of healthy and pathological aging, these attributes are not equated for younger and older adults separately on the stimuli used. In this study, normative data were collected to test whether there exist any age differences in these attributes. The results demonstrate that the ratings given by younger and older adults on natural and manmade category items correlated positively. However, age differences were also apparent, whereby older adults provided higher ratings overall than younger adults. Suggestions and hypotheses are presented to explain this pattern of age differences, which relate to how category concepts may be represented by healthy younger and older adults. Also, the possible implications for these differential age ratings on lexical-semantic processing are discussed. The age differences apparent in this study demonstrate the need to consider age-appropriate normative ratings in the selection of stimuli for use in lexical-semantic processing studies of aging, and the normative data presented provide a means of equating category stimuli. The complete list of all the means is available at www.psychonomic.org/archive.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lorna I Morrow
- Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, 58 Hillhead St., Glasgow, G12 8QB Scotland.
| | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Mechelli A, Sartori G, Orlandi P, Price CJ. Semantic relevance explains category effects in medial fusiform gyri. Neuroimage 2006; 30:992-1002. [PMID: 16343950 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2005] [Revised: 10/10/2005] [Accepted: 10/14/2005] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
We used functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging to explore the neural correlates of semantic relevance in 12 healthy participants performing a picture-naming task. In addition, we tested the hypothesis that category effects typically found in functional imaging can be partly explained in terms of different semantic relevance for animals and artefacts. We report that semantic relevance modulates neuronal responses in the medial fusiform gyrus bilaterally. As predicted, category effects in this region are strongly modulated by the semantic relevance of the items. Specifically, the effect of artefacts > animals is greatly reduced when the two categories are matched for semantic relevance. Thus, the present study demonstrates that neuronal responses during concept retrieval are modulated by the semantic relevance of the features. It also suggests that increased activation in the medial fusiform gyrus typically found for artefacts > animals can be explained by different semantic relevance for animal and artefact items.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Mechelli
- Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, 103 Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Semenza C. Living, non-living and other things. What can be learned nowadays from category-specific deficits? J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006; 12:272-4. [PMID: 16573860 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617706060395] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Warrington and Shallice's (1984) report on patient J.B.R. [4] was the first and most influential study about the living/nonliving semantic dissociation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carlo Semenza
- Department of Psychology, University of Trieste, Via S. Anastasio 12, 34124 Trieste, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Caramazza A, Mahon BZ. The organisation of conceptual knowledge in the brain: The future's past and some future directions. Cogn Neuropsychol 2006; 23:13-38. [PMID: 21049320 DOI: 10.1080/02643290542000021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
49
|
Zannino GD, Perri R, Pasqualetti P, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. (Category-specific) semantic deficit in Alzheimer's patients: The role of semantic distance. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44:52-61. [PMID: 15904938 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2004] [Revised: 03/01/2005] [Accepted: 04/19/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
A semantic battery comprising items from living and nonliving categories was administered to a sample of subjects suffering from probable Alzheimer's type dementia (n=20). The results were analyzed to test the role of semantic distance in predicting group accuracy and its possible causal link with the phenomenon of a category-specific deficit for living things in the experimental population. Our findings confirm a category effect favoring nonliving items (over and above the role of confounding variables) in patients with Alzheimer's disease and support the major role of an imbalance of semantic distance in the testing material in the genesis of this phenomenon.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gian Daniele Zannino
- I.R.C.C.S. Santa Lucia, Laboratorio di Neurologia Clinica e Comportamentale, Via Ardeatina 306, 00179 Rome, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Sartori G, Mameli F, Polezzi D, Lombardi L. An ERP study of low and high relevance semantic features. Brain Res Bull 2005; 69:182-6. [PMID: 16533668 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.11.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2005] [Revised: 11/30/2005] [Accepted: 11/30/2005] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
It is believed that the N400 elicited by concepts belonging to living is larger than N400 to non-living. This is considered as evidence that concepts are organized, in the brain, on the basis of categories. We conducted a feature-verification experiment where living and non-living concepts were matched for relevance of semantic features. Relevance is a measure of the contribution of semantic features to the "core" meaning of a concept. We found that when relevance is low the N400 is large. In addition, we found that when the two categories of living and non-living are equated for relevance the seemingly category effect at behavioral and neural level disappeared. In sum, N400 is sensitive, rather than to categories, to semantic features, thus showing that previously reported effects of semantic categories may arise as a consequence of the differing relevance of concepts belonging to living and non-living categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giuseppe Sartori
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Via Venezia, 8, 35100 Padova, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|