1
|
Knight RS, Chen T, Center EG, Gratton G, Fabiani M, Savazzi S, Mazzi C, Beck DM. Bypassing input to V1 in visual awareness: A TMS-EROS investigation. Neuropsychologia 2024; 198:108864. [PMID: 38521150 PMCID: PMC11194103 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108864] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2023] [Revised: 02/07/2024] [Accepted: 03/18/2024] [Indexed: 03/25/2024]
Abstract
Early visual cortex (V1-V3) is believed to be critical for normal visual awareness by providing the necessary feedforward input. However, it remains unclear whether visual awareness can occur without further involvement of early visual cortex, such as re-entrant feedback. It has been challenging to determine the importance of feedback activity to these areas because of the difficulties in dissociating this activity from the initial feedforward activity. Here, we applied single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the left posterior parietal cortex to elicit phosphenes in the absence of direct visual input to early visual cortex. Immediate neural activity after the TMS pulse was assessed using the event-related optical signal (EROS), which can measure activity under the TMS coil without artifacts. Our results show that: 1) The activity in posterior parietal cortex 50 ms after TMS was related to phosphene awareness, and 2) Activity related to awareness was observed in a small portion of V1 140 ms after TMS, but in contrast (3) Activity in V2 was a more robust correlate of awareness. Together, these results are consistent with interactive models proposing that sustained and recurrent loops of activity between cortical areas are necessary for visual awareness to emerge. In addition, we observed phosphene-related activations of the anteromedial cuneus and lateral occipital cortex, suggesting a functional network subserving awareness comprising these regions, the parietal cortex and early visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ramisha S Knight
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois.405 N Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL, USA; Aptima, Inc. 2555 University Blvd, Fairborn, OH, USA
| | - Tao Chen
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois.405 N Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Illinois. 601 E John Street, Champaign, IL, USA.
| | - Evan G Center
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois.405 N Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Illinois. 601 E John Street, Champaign, IL, USA; Center for Ubiquitous Computing, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
| | - Gabriele Gratton
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois.405 N Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Illinois. 601 E John Street, Champaign, IL, USA
| | - Monica Fabiani
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois.405 N Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Illinois. 601 E John Street, Champaign, IL, USA
| | - Silvia Savazzi
- Perception and Awareness (PandA) Lab, Department of Neuroscience, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
| | - Chiara Mazzi
- Perception and Awareness (PandA) Lab, Department of Neuroscience, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
| | - Diane M Beck
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois.405 N Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Illinois. 601 E John Street, Champaign, IL, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Qubad M, Barnes-Scheufler CV, Schaum M, Raspor E, Rösler L, Peters B, Schiweck C, Goebel R, Reif A, Bittner RA. Improved correspondence of fMRI visual field localizer data after cortex-based macroanatomical alignment. Sci Rep 2022; 12:14310. [PMID: 35995943 PMCID: PMC9395433 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-17909-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2021] [Accepted: 08/02/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Studying the visual system with fMRI often requires using localizer paradigms to define regions of interest (ROIs). However, the considerable interindividual variability of the cerebral cortex represents a crucial confound for group-level analyses. Cortex-based alignment (CBA) techniques reliably reduce interindividual macroanatomical variability. Yet, their utility has not been assessed for visual field localizer paradigms, which map specific parts of the visual field within retinotopically organized visual areas. We evaluated CBA for an attention-enhanced visual field localizer, mapping homologous parts of each visual quadrant in 50 participants. We compared CBA with volume-based alignment and a surface-based analysis, which did not include macroanatomical alignment. CBA led to the strongest increase in the probability of activation overlap (up to 86%). At the group level, CBA led to the most consistent increase in ROI size while preserving vertical ROI symmetry. Overall, our results indicate that in addition to the increased signal-to-noise ratio of a surface-based analysis, macroanatomical alignment considerably improves statistical power. These findings confirm and extend the utility of CBA for the study of the visual system in the context of group analyses. CBA should be particularly relevant when studying neuropsychiatric disorders with abnormally increased interindividual macroanatomical variability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mishal Qubad
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Catherine V Barnes-Scheufler
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Michael Schaum
- Leibniz Institute for Resilience Research, Mainz, Germany
| | - Eva Raspor
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Lara Rösler
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany.,Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Benjamin Peters
- Institute of Medical Psychology, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany.,Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Carmen Schiweck
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Rainer Goebel
- Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.,Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Andreas Reif
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Robert A Bittner
- Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy and Brain Imaging Center, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany. .,Ernst Strüngmann Institute for Neuroscience (ESI) in Cooperation With Max Planck Society, Frankfurt am Main, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Shioiri S, Sasada T, Nishikawa R. Visual attention around a hand location localized by proprioceptive information. Cereb Cortex Commun 2022; 3:tgac005. [PMID: 35224493 PMCID: PMC8867302 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgac005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2021] [Revised: 01/14/2022] [Accepted: 01/17/2022] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Facilitation of visual processing has been reported in the space near the hand. To understand the underlying mechanism of hand proximity attention, we conducted experiments that isolated hand-related effects from top–down attention, proprioceptive information from visual information, the position effect from the influence of action, and the distance effect from the peripersonal effect. The flash-lag effect was used as an index of attentional modulation. Because the results showed that the flash-lag effect was smaller at locations near the hand, we concluded that there was a facilitation effect of the visual stimuli around the hand location identified through proprioceptive information. This was confirmed by conventional reaction time measures. We also measured steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP) in order to investigate the spatial properties of hand proximity attention and top–down attention. The results showed that SSVEP reflects the effect of top–down attention but not that of hand proximity attention. This suggests that the site of hand proximity attention is at a later stage of visual processing, assuming that SSVEP responds to neural activities at the early stages. The results of left-handers differed from those of right-handers, and this is discussed in relation to handedness variation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Satoshi Shioiri
- Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
- Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Takumi Sasada
- Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Ryota Nishikawa
- Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Baumgarten S, Hoberg T, Lohmann T, Mazinani B, Walter P, Koutsonas A. Fullfield and extrafoveal visual evoked potentials in healthy eyes: reference data for a curved OLED display. Doc Ophthalmol 2022; 145:247-262. [PMID: 36087163 PMCID: PMC9653365 DOI: 10.1007/s10633-022-09897-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2022] [Accepted: 08/24/2022] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE Visual evoked potentials (VEP) present an important diagnostic tool in various ophthalmologic and neurologic diseases. Quantitative response data varied among patients but are also dependent on the recording and stimulating equipment. We established VEP reference values for our setting which was recently modified by using a curved OLED display as visual stimulator. Distinction is made between fullfield (FF) and extrafoveal (EF) conduction, and the effect of sex, age and lens status was determined. METHODS This prospective cross-sectional study included 162 healthy eyes of 162 test persons older than 10 years. A fullfield pattern-reversal visual evoked potential (FF-PR-VEP) with two stimulus sizes (ss) (20.4' and 1.4°) as well as an extrafoveal pattern onset-offset VEP (EF-P-ON/OFF-VEP) (ss 1.4° and 2.8°) was derived in accordance with the International Society for Clinical Electrophysiology of Vision guidelines. Amplitudes and latencies were recorded, and the mean values as well as standard deviations were calculated. Age- and sex-dependent influences and the difference between phakic and pseudophakic eyes were examined. A subanalysis of EF-P-ON/OFF-VEP and fullfield pattern onset-offset VEP (FF-P-ON/OFF-VEP) was performed. A 55-inch curved OLED display (LG55EC930V, LG Electronics Inc., Seoul, South Korea) was used as visual stimulator. RESULTS Mean P100 latency of the FF-PR-VEP was 103.81 ± 7.77 ms (ss 20.4') and 102.58 ± 7.26 ms (ss 1.4°), and mean C2 latency of the EF-P-ON/OFF-VEP was 102.95 ± 11.84 ms (ss 1.4°) and 113.58 ± 9.87 ms (ss 2.8°). For all stimulation settings (FF-PR-VEP, EF-P-ON/OFF-VEP), a significant effect of age with longer latencies and smaller amplitudes in older subjects and higher amplitudes in women was observed. We saw no significant difference in latency or amplitude between phakic and pseudophakic eyes and between EF-P-ON/OFF-VEP and FF-P-ON/OFF-VEP. CONCLUSIONS A curved OLED visual stimulator is well suited to obtain VEP response curves with a reasonable interindividual variability. We found significant effects of age and gender in our responses but no effect of the lens status. EF-P-ON/OFF-VEP tends to show smaller amplitudes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabine Baumgarten
- grid.1957.a0000 0001 0728 696XDepartment of Ophthalmology, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Tabea Hoberg
- grid.1957.a0000 0001 0728 696XDepartment of Ophthalmology, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Tibor Lohmann
- grid.1957.a0000 0001 0728 696XDepartment of Ophthalmology, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Babac Mazinani
- grid.1957.a0000 0001 0728 696XDepartment of Ophthalmology, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Peter Walter
- grid.1957.a0000 0001 0728 696XDepartment of Ophthalmology, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| | - Antonis Koutsonas
- grid.1957.a0000 0001 0728 696XDepartment of Ophthalmology, RWTH Aachen University, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zhou X, Xu M, Xiao X, Wang Y, Jung TP, Ming D. Detection of fixation points using a small visual landmark for brain-computer interfaces. J Neural Eng 2021; 18. [PMID: 34130268 DOI: 10.1088/1741-2552/ac0b51] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2021] [Accepted: 06/15/2021] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
Objective.The speed of visual brain-computer interfaces (v-BCIs) has been greatly improved in recent years. However, the traditional v-BCI paradigms require users to directly gaze at the intensive flickering items, which would cause severe problems such as visual fatigue and excessive visual resource consumption in practical applications. Therefore, it is imperative to develop a user-friendly v-BCI.Approach.According to the retina-cortical relationship, this study developed a novel BCI paradigm to detect the fixation point of eyes using a small visual stimulus that subtended only 0.6° in visual angle and was out of the central visual field. Specifically, the visual stimulus was treated as a landmark to judge the eccentricity and polar angle of the fixation point. Sixteen different fixation points were selected around the visual landmark, i.e. different combinations of two eccentricities (2° and 4°) and eight polar angles (0,π4,π2,3π4,π,5π4,3π2and7π4). Twelve subjects participated in this study, and they were asked to gaze at one out of the 16 points for each trial. A multi-class discriminative canonical pattern matching (Multi-DCPM) algorithm was proposed to decode the user's fixation point.Main results.We found the visual stimulation landmark elicited different spatial event-related potential patterns for different fixation points. Multi-DCPM could achieve an average accuracy of 66.2% with a standard deviation of 15.8% for the classification of the sixteen fixation points, which was significantly higher than traditional algorithms (p⩽0.001). Experimental results of this study demonstrate the feasibility of using a small visual stimulus as a landmark to track the relative position of the fixation point.Significance.The proposed new paradigm provides a potential approach to alleviate the problem of irritating stimuli in v-BCIs, which can broaden the applications of BCIs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaoyu Zhou
- The Laboratory of Neural Engineering & Rehabilitation, Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China
| | - Minpeng Xu
- The Laboratory of Neural Engineering & Rehabilitation, Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China.,The Tianjin International Joint Research Center for Neural Engineering, Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China
| | - Xiaolin Xiao
- The Laboratory of Neural Engineering & Rehabilitation, Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China.,The Tianjin International Joint Research Center for Neural Engineering, Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China
| | - Yijun Wang
- The State Key Laboratory on Integrated Optoelectronics, Institute of Semiconductors, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
| | - Tzyy-Ping Jung
- The Laboratory of Neural Engineering & Rehabilitation, Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China.,The Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, University of California, San Diego, CA, United States of America
| | - Dong Ming
- The Laboratory of Neural Engineering & Rehabilitation, Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Precision Instruments and Optoelectronics Engineering, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China.,The Tianjin International Joint Research Center for Neural Engineering, Academy of Medical Engineering and Translational Medicine, Tianjin University, Tianjin, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Herde L, Uhl J, Rauss K. Anatomic and functional asymmetries interactively shape human early visual cortex responses. J Vis 2020; 20:3. [PMID: 32503040 PMCID: PMC7416905 DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.6.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Early visual processing is surprisingly flexible even in the adult brain. This flexibility involves both long-term structural plasticity and online adaptations conveyed by top-down feedback. Although this view is supported by rich evidence from both human behavioral studies and invasive electrophysiology in nonhuman models, it has proven difficult to close the gap between species. In particular, it remains debated whether noninvasive measures of neural activity can capture top-down modulations of the earliest stages of processing in the human visual cortex. We previously reported modulations of retinotopic C1, the earliest component of the human visual evoked potential. However, these effects were selectively observed in the upper visual field (UVF). Here we test whether this asymmetry is linked to an interaction between differences in spatial resolution across the visual field and the specific stimuli used in previous studies. We measured visual evoked potentials in response to task-irrelevant, high-contrast textures of different densities in a comparatively large sample of healthy volunteers (N = 31) using high-density electroencephalogram. Our results show differential response profiles for upper and lower hemifields, with UVF responses saturating at higher stimulus densities. In contrast, lower visual field responses did not increase, and even showed a tendency toward a decrease at the highest density tested. We propose that these findings reflect feature- and task-specific pooling of signals from retinotopic regions with different sensitivity profiles. Such complex interactions between anatomic and functional asymmetries need to be considered to resolve whether human early visual cortex activity is modulated by top-down factors.
Collapse
|
7
|
Norcia AM, Yakovleva A, Hung B, Goldberg JL. Dynamics of Contrast Decrement and Increment Responses in Human Visual Cortex. Transl Vis Sci Technol 2020; 9:6. [PMID: 32953246 PMCID: PMC7476656 DOI: 10.1167/tvst.9.10.6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/29/2020] [Accepted: 08/10/2020] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose The goal of the present experiments was to determine whether electrophysiologic response properties of the ON and OFF visual pathways observed in animal experimental models can be observed in humans. Methods Steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs) were recorded in response to equivalent magnitude contrast increments and decrements presented within a probe-on-pedestal Westheimer sensitization paradigm. The probes were modulated with sawtooth temporal waveforms at a temporal frequency of 3 or 2.73 Hz. SSVEP response waveforms and response spectra for incremental and decremental stimuli were analyzed as a function of stimulus size and visual field location in 67 healthy adult participants. Results SSVEPs recorded at the scalp differ between contrast decrements and increments of equal Weber contrast: SSVEP responses were larger in amplitude and shorter in latency for contrast decrements than for contrast increments. Both increment and decrement responses were larger for displays that were scaled for cortical magnification. Conclusions In a fashion that parallels results from the early visual system of cats and monkeys, two key properties of ON versus OFF pathways found in single-unit recordings are recapitulated at the population level of activity that can be observed with scalp electrodes, allowing differential assessment of ON and OFF pathway activity in human. Translational Relevance As data from preclinical models of visual pathway dysfunction point to differential damage to subtypes of retinal ganglion cells, this approach may be useful in future work on disease detection and treatment monitoring.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anthony M Norcia
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | | | - Bethany Hung
- Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Contrast Normalization Accounts for Binocular Interactions in Human Striate and Extra-striate Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 2020; 40:2753-2763. [PMID: 32060172 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2043-19.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2019] [Revised: 01/10/2020] [Accepted: 02/04/2020] [Indexed: 01/23/2023] Open
Abstract
During binocular viewing, visual inputs from the two eyes interact at the level of visual cortex. Here we studied binocular interactions in human visual cortex, including both sexes, using source-imaged steady-state visual evoked potentials over a wide range of relative contrast between two eyes. The ROIs included areas V1, V3a, hV4, hMT+, and lateral occipital cortex. Dichoptic parallel grating stimuli in each eye modulated at distinct temporal frequencies allowed us to quantify spectral components associated with the individual stimuli from monocular inputs (self-terms) and responses due to interaction between the inputs from the two eyes (intermodulation [IM] terms). Data with self-terms revealed an interocular suppression effect, in which the responses to the stimulus in one eye were reduced when a stimulus was presented simultaneously to the other eye. The suppression magnitude varied depending on visual area, and the relative contrast between the two eyes. Suppression was strongest in V1 and V3a (50% reduction) and was least in lateral occipital cortex (20% reduction). Data with IM terms revealed another form of binocular interaction, compared with self-terms. IM response was strongest at V1 and was least in hV4. Fits of a family of divisive gain control models to both self- and IM-term responses within each cortical area indicated that both forms of binocular interaction shared a common gain control nonlinearity. However, our model fits revealed different patterns of binocular interaction along the cortical hierarchy, particularly in terms of excitatory and suppressive contributions.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Using source-imaged steady-state visual evoked potentials and frequency-domain analysis of dichoptic stimuli, we measured two forms of binocular interactions: one is associated with the individual stimuli that represent interocular suppression from each eye, and the other is a direct measure of interocular interaction between inputs from the two eyes. We demonstrated that both forms of binocular interactions share a common gain control mechanism in striate and extra-striate cortex. Furthermore, our model fits revealed different patterns of binocular interaction along the visual cortical hierarchy, particularly in terms of excitatory and suppressive contributions.
Collapse
|
9
|
Greene HH, Brown JM, Strauss GP. Shorter fixation durations for up-directed saccades during saccadic exploration: A meta-analysis. J Eye Mov Res 2020; 12:10.16910/jemr.12.8.5. [PMID: 33828778 PMCID: PMC7881898 DOI: 10.16910/jemr.12.8.5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Utilizing 23 datasets, we report a meta-analysis of an asymmetry in presaccadic fixation durations for saccades directed above and below eye fixation during saccadic exploration. For inclusion in the meta-analysis, saccadic exploration of complex visual displays had to have been made without gaze-contingent manipulations. Effect sizes for the asymmetry were quantified as Hedge's g. Pooled effect sizes indicated significant asymmetries such that during saccadic exploration in a variety of tasks, presaccadic fixation durations for saccades directed into the upper visual field were reliably shorter than presaccadic fixation durations for saccades into the lower visual field. It is contended that the asymmetry is robust and important for efforts aimed at modelling when a saccade is initiated as a function of ensuing saccade direction.
Collapse
|
10
|
Center EG, Knight R, Fabiani M, Gratton G, Beck DM. Examining the role of feedback in TMS-induced visual suppression: A cautionary tale. Conscious Cogn 2019; 75:102805. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102805] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2019] [Revised: 07/04/2019] [Accepted: 08/10/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
11
|
Chen J, Li Z, Hong B, Maye A, Engel AK, Zhang D. A Single-Stimulus, Multitarget BCI Based on Retinotopic Mapping of Motion-Onset VEPs. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 2019; 66:464-470. [DOI: 10.1109/tbme.2018.2849102] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
12
|
Yousefi R, Sereshkeh AR, Chau T. Exploiting error-related potentials in cognitive task based BCI. Biomed Phys Eng Express 2018. [DOI: 10.1088/2057-1976/aaee99] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
13
|
Landwehr K. Titchener's T with flanks. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2018; 191:244-250. [PMID: 30343097 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2018] [Revised: 10/04/2018] [Accepted: 10/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Flanks were added to Titchener's (1901) T-illusion figure to test its susceptibility to context stimuli. The addition of a second divided line yielded H-type figures, and the addition of a second undivided line, +-type figures. The lengths of the Ts' undivided lines was expected to be overestimated relative to the lengths of the divided lines, when all lines were about equally long, and the illusion was expected to become smaller when one or two gaps had been introduced between the lines. Results conformed to the predictions. The amount of illusion was larger for the no-gap H than the T, and was almost annihilated with the two-gaps H, with 3 out of 14 observers showing an inverse response bias. The +-type stimuli produced analogous results. Findings are interpreted in terms of the nonequivalence of the endpoints of the stimuli's lines, which are thought to elicit different responses in end-inhibited cortical neurons, thereby affecting length estimates.
Collapse
|
14
|
Schuck K, Niedeggen M, Kerschreiter R. Violated Expectations in the Cyberball Paradigm: Testing the Expectancy Account of Social Participation With ERP. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1762. [PMID: 30319487 PMCID: PMC6167485 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01762] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2018] [Accepted: 08/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous social exclusion experiments identified two factors affecting the participants' evaluation of participation in a virtual ball tossing game (cyberball): ball reception probability and vertical position of the participant's avatar on the screen. The P3 component in the event-related brain potentials (ERPs) indicated that both factors moderate subjective expectancies on social participation. The present research builds on an expectancy model explaining these effects and tests whether its predictions - established in a within-participant design - also hold in a between-participant design more common in behavioral cyberball studies. Participants were randomly assigned to four conditions which differed in ball reception probability (16% vs. 26%) and the avatar's vertical position (inferior vs. superior). To track the state of expectancy of involvement online, we recorded the ERP response evoked by ball receptions of the participant. Retrospectively, social involvement and social need threat were rated in a questionnaire. As hypothesized, low ball reception probability elicited enlarged P3 amplitudes in the ERPs, increased negative mood, and threatened social needs. For participants at inferior position, ERP and questionnaire effects were less expressed. This effect of verticality can be traced back to an adjustment in the expected involvement as signaled by a differential adaptation of the P3 amplitude within an experimental run. These results confirm that the predictions of an expectancy model also apply to cyberball studies using a between-participant design. However, the comparison with the results of previous within-participant design studies suggests that the sensitivity of the adjustment processes critically depends on the choice of the experimental design.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katharina Schuck
- Division of Experimental Psychology and Neuropsychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Free University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Michael Niedeggen
- Division of Experimental Psychology and Neuropsychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Free University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Rudolf Kerschreiter
- Division of Social, Organizational, and Economic Psychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Free University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Pitzalis S, Strappini F, Bultrini A, Di Russo F. Detailed spatiotemporal brain mapping of chromatic vision combining high-resolution VEP with fMRI and retinotopy. Hum Brain Mapp 2018. [PMID: 29536594 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.24046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Neuroimaging studies have identified so far, several color-sensitive visual areas in the human brain, and the temporal dynamics of these activities have been separately investigated using the visual-evoked potentials (VEPs). In the present study, we combined electrophysiological and neuroimaging methods to determine a detailed spatiotemporal profile of chromatic VEP and to localize its neural generators. The accuracy of the present co-registration study was obtained by combining standard fMRI data with retinotopic and motion mapping data at the individual level. We found a sequence of occipito activities more complex than that typically reported for chromatic VEPs, including feed-forward and reentrant feedback. Results showed that chromatic human perception arises by the combined activity of at the least five parieto-occipital areas including V1, LOC, V8/VO, and the motion-sensitive dorsal region MT+. However, the contribution of V1 and V8/VO seems dominant because the re-entrant activity in these areas was present more than once (twice in V8/VO and thrice in V1). This feedforward and feedback chromatic processing appears delayed compared with the luminance processing. Associating VEPs and neuroimaging measures, we showed for the first time a complex spatiotemporal pattern of activity, confirming that chromatic stimuli produce intricate interactions of many different brain dorsal and ventral areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabrina Pitzalis
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico,", Rome, Italy.,Santa Lucia Foundation, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
| | | | - Alessandro Bultrini
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico,", Rome, Italy
| | - Francesco Di Russo
- Department of Movement, Human and Health Sciences, University of Rome "Foro Italico,", Rome, Italy.,Santa Lucia Foundation, IRCCS, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Structural and functional correlates of visual field asymmetry in the human brain by diffusion kurtosis MRI and functional MRI. Neuroreport 2018; 27:1225-31. [PMID: 27631541 DOI: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000682] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Human visual performance has been observed to show superiority in localized regions of the visual field across many classes of stimuli. However, the underlying neural mechanisms remain unclear. This study aims to determine whether the visual information processing in the human brain is dependent on the location of stimuli in the visual field and the corresponding neuroarchitecture using blood-oxygenation-level-dependent functional MRI (fMRI) and diffusion kurtosis MRI, respectively, in 15 healthy individuals at 3 T. In fMRI, visual stimulation to the lower hemifield showed stronger brain responses and larger brain activation volumes than the upper hemifield, indicative of the differential sensitivity of the human brain across the visual field. In diffusion kurtosis MRI, the brain regions mapping to the lower visual field showed higher mean kurtosis, but not fractional anisotropy or mean diffusivity compared with the upper visual field. These results suggested the different distributions of microstructural organization across visual field brain representations. There was also a strong positive relationship between diffusion kurtosis and fMRI responses in the lower field brain representations. In summary, this study suggested the structural and functional brain involvements in the asymmetry of visual field responses in humans, and is important to the neurophysiological and psychological understanding of human visual information processing.
Collapse
|
17
|
Attention is allocated closely ahead of the target during smooth pursuit eye movements: Evidence from EEG frequency tagging. Neuropsychologia 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
|
18
|
Si J, Zhang X, Zhang Y, Jiang T. Hemispheric differences in electrical and hemodynamic responses during hemifield visual stimulation with graded contrasts. BIOMEDICAL OPTICS EXPRESS 2017; 8:2018-2035. [PMID: 28736653 PMCID: PMC5516812 DOI: 10.1364/boe.8.002018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/09/2017] [Revised: 02/15/2017] [Accepted: 02/17/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
A multimodal neuroimaging technique based on electroencephalography (EEG) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) was used with horizontal hemifield visual stimuli with graded contrasts to investigate the retinotopic mapping more fully as well as to explore hemispheric differences in neuronal activity, the hemodynamic response, and the neurovascular coupling relationship in the visual cortex. The fNIRS results showed the expected activation over the contralateral hemisphere for both the left and right hemifield visual stimulations. However, the EEG results presented a paradoxical lateralization, with the maximal response located over the ipsilateral hemisphere but with the polarity inversed components located over the contralateral hemisphere. Our results suggest that the polarity inversion as well as the latency advantage over the contralateral hemisphere cause the amplitude of the VEP over the contralateral hemisphere to be smaller than that over the ipsilateral hemisphere. Both the neuronal and hemodynamic responses changed logarithmically with the level of contrast in the hemifield visual stimulations. Moreover, the amplitudes and latencies of the visual evoked potentials (VEPs) were linearly correlated with the hemodynamic responses despite differences in the slopes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juanning Si
- Brainnetome Center, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
- National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
| | - Xin Zhang
- Brainnetome Center, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
- National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
| | - Yujin Zhang
- Brainnetome Center, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
- National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
| | - Tianzi Jiang
- Brainnetome Center, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
- National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
- Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu 625014, China
- CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100190, China
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Williams RJ, Reutens DC, Hocking J. Influence of BOLD Contributions to Diffusion fMRI Activation of the Visual Cortex. Front Neurosci 2016; 10:279. [PMID: 27445654 PMCID: PMC4923189 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00279] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2016] [Accepted: 06/06/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Reliance on the hemodynamic response as a surrogate marker of neural activity imposes an intrinsic limit on the spatial specificity of functional MRI. An alternative approach based on diffusion-weighted functional MRI (DfMRI) has been reported as a contrast less reliant on hemodynamic effects, however current evidence suggests that both hemodynamic and unique neural sources contribute to the diffusion signal. Here we compare activation patterns obtained with the standard blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast to DfMRI in order to gain a deeper understanding of how the BOLD proportion contributes to the observable diffusion signal. Both individual and group-level activation patterns obtained with DfMRI and BOLD to a visual field stimulation paradigm were analyzed. At the individual level, the DfMRI contrast showed a strong, positive relationship between the volumes of cortex activated in response to quadrant- and hemi-field visual stimulation. This was not observed in the corresponding BOLD experiment. Overall, the DfMRI response indicated less between-subject variability, with random effects analyses demonstrating higher statistical values at the peak voxel for DfMRI. Furthermore, the spatial extent of the activation was more restricted to the primary visual region for DfMRI than BOLD. However, the diffusion signal was sensitive to the hemodynamic response in a manner dependent on experimental manipulation. It was also limited by its low signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), demonstrating lower sensitivity than BOLD. Together these findings both support DfMRI as a contrast that bears a closer spatial relationship to the underlying neural activity than BOLD, and raise important caveats regarding its utilization. Models explaining the DfMRI signal change need to consider the dynamic vascular contributions that may vary with neural activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca J Williams
- Hotchkiss Brain Institute and Department of Radiology, University of CalgaryCalgary, AB, Canada; Centre for Advanced Imaging, The University of QueenslandSt. Lucia, QLD, Australia; Queensland Brain Institute, The University of QueenslandSt. Lucia, QLD, Australia; Centre for Clinical Research, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - David C Reutens
- Centre for Advanced Imaging, The University of Queensland St. Lucia, QLD, Australia
| | - Julia Hocking
- School of Psychology and Counselling, Faculty of Health, Queensland University of Technology Kelvin Grove, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Salmela VR, Henriksson L, Vanni S. Radial Frequency Analysis of Contour Shapes in the Visual Cortex. PLoS Comput Biol 2016; 12:e1004719. [PMID: 26866917 PMCID: PMC4750910 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004719] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2015] [Accepted: 12/17/2015] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Cumulative psychophysical evidence suggests that the shape of closed contours is analysed by means of their radial frequency components (RFC). However, neurophysiological evidence for RFC-based representations is still missing. We investigated the representation of radial frequency in the human visual cortex with functional magnetic resonance imaging. We parametrically varied the radial frequency, amplitude and local curvature of contour shapes. The stimuli evoked clear responses across visual areas in the univariate analysis, but the response magnitude did not depend on radial frequency or local curvature. Searchlight-based, multivariate representational similarity analysis revealed RFC specific response patterns in areas V2d, V3d, V3AB, and IPS0. Interestingly, RFC-specific representations were not found in hV4 or LO, traditionally associated with visual shape analysis. The modulation amplitude of the shapes did not affect the responses in any visual area. Local curvature, SF-spectrum and contrast energy related representations were found across visual areas but without similar specificity for visual area that was found for RFC. The results suggest that the radial frequency of a closed contour is one of the cortical shape analysis dimensions, represented in the early and mid-level visual areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viljami R. Salmela
- Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Division of Cognitive and Neuropsychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
- Advanced Magnetic Imaging Centre, Aalto NeuroImaging, Aalto University School of Science, Espoo, Finland
- * E-mail:
| | - Linda Henriksson
- Advanced Magnetic Imaging Centre, Aalto NeuroImaging, Aalto University School of Science, Espoo, Finland
- Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University, Espoo, Finland
| | - Simo Vanni
- Advanced Magnetic Imaging Centre, Aalto NeuroImaging, Aalto University School of Science, Espoo, Finland
- Clinical Neurosciences, Neurology, University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Helsinki, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Mannion DJ. Sensitivity to the visual field origin of natural image patches in human low-level visual cortex. PeerJ 2015; 3:e1038. [PMID: 26131378 PMCID: PMC4485252 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.1038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2015] [Accepted: 05/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Asymmetries in the response to visual patterns in the upper and lower visual fields (above and below the centre of gaze) have been associated with ecological factors relating to the structure of typical visual environments. Here, we investigated whether the content of the upper and lower visual field representations in low-level regions of human visual cortex are specialised for visual patterns that arise from the upper and lower visual fields in natural images. We presented image patches, drawn from above or below the centre of gaze of an observer navigating a natural environment, to either the upper or lower visual fields of human participants (n = 7) while we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure the magnitude of evoked activity in the visual areas V1, V2, and V3. We found a significant interaction between the presentation location (upper or lower visual field) and the image patch source location (above or below fixation); the responses to lower visual field presentation were significantly greater for image patches sourced from below than above fixation, while the responses in the upper visual field were not significantly different for image patches sourced from above and below fixation. This finding demonstrates an association between the representation of the lower visual field in human visual cortex and the structure of the visual input that is likely to be encountered below the centre of gaze.
Collapse
|
22
|
ARCARO M, KASTNER S. Topographic organization of areas V3 and V4 and its relation to supra-areal organization of the primate visual system. Vis Neurosci 2015; 32:E014. [PMID: 26241035 PMCID: PMC4900470 DOI: 10.1017/s0952523815000115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Areas V3 and V4 are commonly thought of as individual entities in the primate visual system, based on definition criteria such as their representation of visual space, connectivity, functional response properties, and relative anatomical location in cortex. Yet, large-scale functional and anatomical organization patterns not only emphasize distinctions within each area, but also links across visual cortex. Specifically, the visuotopic organization of V3 and V4 appears to be part of a larger, supra-areal organization, clustering these areas with early visual areas V1 and V2. In addition, connectivity patterns across visual cortex appear to vary within these areas as a function of their supra-areal eccentricity organization. This complicates the traditional view of these regions as individual functional "areas." Here, we will review the criteria for defining areas V3 and V4 and will discuss functional and anatomical studies in humans and monkeys that emphasize the integration of individual visual areas into broad, supra-areal clusters that work in concert for a common computational goal. Specifically, we propose that the visuotopic organization of V3 and V4, which provides the criteria for differentiating these areas, also unifies these areas into the supra-areal organization of early visual cortex. We propose that V3 and V4 play a critical role in this supra-areal organization by filtering information about the visual environment along parallel pathways across higher-order cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M.J. ARCARO
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08544
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08544
| | - S. KASTNER
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08544
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08544
| |
Collapse
|