1
|
Different Educational Interventions on Individual Cognition of Garbage Classification Based on EEG Monitoring. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2022; 19:ijerph19148567. [PMID: 35886418 PMCID: PMC9323723 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph19148567] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/18/2022] [Revised: 07/08/2022] [Accepted: 07/10/2022] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Improvement in an individuals’ cognition is the key to promote garbage classification. This study takes university students as the research subjects, through three educational interventions, including the self-learning, heuristic learning, and interactive learning ways, to seek the most effective intervention based upon event-related potentials (ERPs) that is beneficial to enhance cognition of garbage classification. The results show that the experimental subjects induced P300 and LPP components, representing attentional changes and cognitive conflicts in classification judgments. There are differences in the amplitudes and peak latency of the two components corresponding to different interventions, indicating that the three educational interventions are able to improve the individual’s cognition level of garbage classification within a certain period of time. The interactive-learning intervention triggers the largest amplitudes of P300 and LPP, as well as the smallest peak latency, indicating its effect is the best. Such results provide insight into the design for an appropriate strategy in garbage classification education. The study also shows that an EEG signal can be used as the endogenous neural indicator to measure the performance of garbage classification under different educational interventions.
Collapse
|
2
|
Levy J, Goldstein A, Influs M, Masalha S, Feldman R. Neural Rhythmic Underpinnings of Intergroup Bias: Implications for Peace-Building Attitudes and Dialogue. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2021; 17:408-420. [PMID: 34519338 PMCID: PMC8972238 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsab106] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2021] [Revised: 08/25/2021] [Accepted: 09/12/2021] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Intergroup bias is a ubiquitous socio-cognitive phenomenon that, while sustaining human dependence on group living, often leads to prejudice, inequity, and violence; yet, its neural underpinnings remain unclear. Framed within the Israeli-Palestinian conflict and targeting youth, this study utilized magnetoencephalography to describe intrinsic neural oscillatory processes that represent the intergroup bias and may link with engagement in peacemaking in order to shed further light on the neural mechanisms underpinning intergroup conflict. Across the oscillatory spectrum, from very low to very high frequency bands, the only rhythm found to underlie the intergroup bias was the alpha rhythm. Alpha was continuously activated across the task and integrated a rapid perceptual component in occipital cortex with a top-down cognitive-control component in medial cingulate cortex. These components were distinctly associated with real-life intergroup dialog style and expressed attitudes that promote active engagement in peacemaking. Our findings suggest that the cortical alpha rhythm plays a crucial role in sustaining intergroup bias and address its impact on concrete intergroup experiences. Results highlight the need to provide opportunities for active peace-building dialog to youth reared amidst intractable conflicts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Levy
- Interdisciplinary Center, Herzliya 46150, Israel.,Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University, Espoo 02150, Finland
| | - Abraham Goldstein
- Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center and Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| | - Moran Influs
- Interdisciplinary Center, Herzliya 46150, Israel
| | | | - Ruth Feldman
- Interdisciplinary Center, Herzliya 46150, Israel.,Child Study Center, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ibanez A, Parra MA, Butler C. The Latin America and the Caribbean Consortium on Dementia (LAC-CD): From Networking to Research to Implementation Science. J Alzheimers Dis 2021; 82:S379-S394. [PMID: 33492297 PMCID: PMC8293660 DOI: 10.3233/jad-201384] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
In comparison with other regions, dementia prevalence in Latin America is growing rapidly, along with the consequent clinical, social, and economic burden upon patients and their families. The combination of fragile health care systems, large social inequalities, and isolated clinical and research initiatives makes the coordination of efforts imperative. The Latin America and the Caribbean Consortium on Dementia (LAC-CD) is a regional organization overseeing and promoting clinical and research activities on dementia. Here, we first provide an overview of the consortium, highlighting the antecedents and current mission. Then, we present the consortium’s regional research, including the multi-partner consortium to expand dementia research in Latin America (ReDLat), which aims to identify the unique genetic, social, and economic factors that drive Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal dementia presentation in LAC relative to the US. We describe an extension of ReDLat which aims to develop affordable markers of disease subtype and severity using high density EEG. We introduce current initiatives promoting regional diagnosis, visibility, and capacity, including the forthcoming launch of the Latin American Brain Health Institute (BrainLat). We discuss LAC-CD-led advances in brain health diplomacy, including an assessment of responses to the impact of COVID-19 on people with dementia and examining the knowledge of public policies among experts in the region. Finally, we present the current knowledge-to-action framework, which paves the way for a future regional action plan. Coordinated actions are crucial to forging strong regional bonds, supporting the implementation of regional dementia plans, improving health systems, and expanding research collaborations across Latin America.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustin Ibanez
- Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California San Francisco (UCSF), San Francisco, CA, USA.,Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CNC), Universidad de San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina.,National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina.,Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Barranquilla, Colombia.,Latin American Institute for Brain Health (BrainLat), Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), Universidad Adolfo Ibanez, Santiago de Chile, Chile
| | - Mario A Parra
- Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Barranquilla, Colombia.,School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK
| | - Christopher Butler
- Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, UK.,Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.,Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva, Buenos Aires, Argentina.,Departamento de Neurología, Pontificia Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Does gaze direction of fearful faces facilitate the processing of threat? An ERP study of spatial precuing effects. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2021; 21:837-851. [PMID: 33846951 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-021-00890-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/03/2021] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
Eye gaze is very important for attentional orienting in social life. By adopting the event-related potential (ERP) technique, we explored whether attentional orienting of eye gaze is modulated by emotional congruency between facial expressions and the targets in a spatial cuing task. Faces with different emotional expressions (fearful/angry/happy/neutral) directing their eye gaze to the left or right were used as cues, indicating the possible location of subsequent targets. Targets were line drawings of animals, which could be either threatening or neutral. Participants indicated by choice responses whether the animal would fit inside a shoebox in real life or not. Reaction times to targets were faster after valid compared with invalid cues, showing the typical eye gaze cuing effect. Analyses of the late positive potential (LPP) elicited by targets revealed a significant modulation of the gaze cuing effect by emotional congruency. Threatening targets elicited larger LPPs when validly cued by gaze in faces with negative (fearful and angry) expressions. Similarly, neutral targets showed larger LPPs when validly cued by faces with neutral expressions. Such effects were not present after happy face cues. Source localization in the LPP time window revealed that for threatening targets, the activity of right medial frontal gyrus could be related to a larger gaze-orienting effect for the fearful than the angry condition. Our findings provide electrophysiological evidence for the modulation of gaze cuing effects by emotional congruency.
Collapse
|
5
|
Trujillo N, Gómez D, Trujillo S, López JD, Ibáñez A, Parra MA. Attentional bias during emotional processing: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence from an Emotional Flanker Task. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0249407. [PMID: 33798215 PMCID: PMC8018632 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249407] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2020] [Accepted: 03/18/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Threatening stimuli seem to capture attention more swiftly than neutral stimuli. This attention bias has been observed under different experimental conditions and with different types of stimuli. It remains unclear whether this adaptive behaviour reflects the function of automatic or controlled attention mechanisms. Additionally, the spatiotemporal dynamics of its neural correlates are largely unknown. The present study investigates these issues using an Emotional Flanker Task synchronized with EEG recordings. A group of 32 healthy participants saw response-relevant images (emotional scenes from IAPS or line drawings of objects) flanked by response-irrelevant distracters (i.e., emotional scenes flanked by line drawings or vice versa). We assessed behavioural and ERP responses drawn from four task conditions (Threat-Central, Neutral-Central, Threat-Peripheral, and Neutral-Peripheral) and subjected these responses to repeated-measures ANOVA models. When presented as response-relevant targets, threatening images attracted faster and more accurate responses. They did not affect response accuracy to targets when presented as response-irrelevant flankers. However, response times were significantly slower when threatening images flanked objects than when neutral images were shown as flankers. This result replicated the well-known Emotional Flanker Effect. Behavioural responses to response-relevant threatening targets were accompanied by significant modulations of ERP activity across all time-windows and regions of interest and displayed some meaningful correlations. The Emotional Flanker Effect was accompanied by a modulation over parietal and central-parietal regions within a time-window between 550-690ms. Such a modulation suggests that the attentional disruption to targets caused by response-irrelevant threatening flankers appears to reflect less neural resources available, which are seemingly drawn away by distracting threatening flankers. The observed spatiotemporal dynamics seem to concur with understanding of the important adaptive role attributed to threat-related attention bias.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalia Trujillo
- Neuroscience Group, University of Antioquia UdeA, Medellín, Colombia
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia UdeA Medellín, Medellín, Colombia
| | - Diana Gómez
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia UdeA Medellín, Medellín, Colombia
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de Antioquia UdeA, Medellín, Colombia
| | - Sandra Trujillo
- Neuroscience Group, University of Antioquia UdeA, Medellín, Colombia
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia UdeA Medellín, Medellín, Colombia
| | - José David López
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de Antioquia UdeA, Medellín, Colombia
| | - Agustín Ibáñez
- Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California San Francisco (UCSF), San Francisco, California, United States of America
- Trinity College Dublin (TCD), Dublin, Ireland
- Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CNC), Universidad de San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Latin American Institute for Brain Health (BrainLat), Universidad Adolfo Ibanez, Santiago, Chile
| | - Mario A. Parra
- Neuroscience Group, University of Antioquia UdeA, Medellín, Colombia
- School of Psychological Sciences & Health, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, United Kingdom
- Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Ibanez A, Yokoyama JS, Possin KL, Matallana D, Lopera F, Nitrini R, Takada LT, Custodio N, Sosa Ortiz AL, Avila-Funes JA, Behrens MI, Slachevsky A, Myers RM, Cochran JN, Brusco LI, Bruno MA, Brucki SMD, Pina-Escudero SD, Okada de Oliveira M, Donnelly Kehoe P, Garcia AM, Cardona JF, Santamaria-Garcia H, Moguilner S, Duran-Aniotz C, Tagliazucchi E, Maito M, Longoria Ibarrola EM, Pintado-Caipa M, Godoy ME, Bakman V, Javandel S, Kosik KS, Valcour V, Miller BL. The Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat): Driving Multicentric Research and Implementation Science. Front Neurol 2021; 12:631722. [PMID: 33776890 PMCID: PMC7992978 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2021.631722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2020] [Accepted: 02/15/2021] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Dementia is becoming increasingly prevalent in Latin America, contrasting with stable or declining rates in North America and Europe. This scenario places unprecedented clinical, social, and economic burden upon patients, families, and health systems. The challenges prove particularly pressing for conditions with highly specific diagnostic and management demands, such as frontotemporal dementia. Here we introduce a research and networking initiative designed to tackle these ensuing hurdles, the Multi-partner consortium to expand dementia research in Latin America (ReDLat). First, we present ReDLat's regional research framework, aimed at identifying the unique genetic, social, and economic factors driving the presentation of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease in Latin America relative to the US. We describe ongoing ReDLat studies in various fields and ongoing research extensions. Then, we introduce actions coordinated by ReDLat and the Latin America and Caribbean Consortium on Dementia (LAC-CD) to develop culturally appropriate diagnostic tools, regional visibility and capacity building, diplomatic coordination in local priority areas, and a knowledge-to-action framework toward a regional action plan. Together, these research and networking initiatives will help to establish strong cross-national bonds, support the implementation of regional dementia plans, enhance health systems' infrastructure, and increase translational research collaborations across the continent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustin Ibanez
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Universidad de San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- School of Psychology, Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, Latin American Institute for Brain Health (BrainLat), Universidad Adolfo Ibanez, Adolfo Ibanez University, Santiago, Chile
| | - Jennifer S. Yokoyama
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| | - Katherine L. Possin
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| | - Diana Matallana
- Psychiatry Department, School of Medicine, Aging Institute, Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia
- Memory and Cognition Clinic, Intellectus, Hospital Universitario San Ignacio, Bogotá, Colombia
- Mental Health Unit, Hospital Universitario Santa Fe de Bogotá, Bogotá, Colombia
| | - Francisco Lopera
- Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
| | - Ricardo Nitrini
- Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Unit, Hospital das Clinicas, University of São Paulo Medical School, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Leonel T. Takada
- Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Unit, Hospital das Clinicas, University of São Paulo Medical School, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Nilton Custodio
- Unit Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Prevention, Cognitive Neurology Center, Peruvian Institute of Neurosciences, Lima, Perú
| | - Ana Luisa Sosa Ortiz
- Instituto Nacional de Neurologia y Neurocirugia MVS, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico, Mexico, Mexico
| | - José Alberto Avila-Funes
- Department of Geriatrics, Instituto Nacional de Ciencias Médicas y Nutrición Salvador Zubirán, Mexico, Mexico
- Univ. Bordeaux, Inserm, Bordeaux Population Health Research Center, Bordeaux, France
| | - Maria Isabel Behrens
- Centro de Investigación Clínica Avanzada, Hospital Clínico, Facultad de Medicina Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
- Departamento de Neurología y Neurocirugía, Hospital Clínico Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
- Departamento de Neurociencia, Facultad de Medicina Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
- Clínica Alemana Santiago, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile
| | - Andrea Slachevsky
- Clínica Alemana Santiago, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile
- Geroscience Center for Brain Health and Metabolism (GERO), Santiago, Chile
- Neuropsychology and Clinical Neuroscience Laboratory, Physiopathology Department, Institute of Biomedical Sciences, Neuroscience and East Neuroscience, Santiago, Chile
- Faculty of Medicine, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile
- Memory and Neuropsychiatric Clinic (CMYN) Neurology Department, Faculty of Medicine, Hospital del Salvador, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | - Richard M. Myers
- Hudson Alpha Institute for Biotechnology, Huntsville, AL, United States
| | | | - Luis Ignacio Brusco
- Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- ALZAR – Alzheimer, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Martin A. Bruno
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Facultad Ciencias Médicas, Instituto Ciencias Biomédicas, Universidad Católica de Cuyo, San Juan, Argentina
| | - Sonia M. D. Brucki
- Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Unit, Hospital das Clinicas, University of São Paulo Medical School, São Paulo, Brazil
- Hospital Santa Marcelina, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Stefanie Danielle Pina-Escudero
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| | - Maira Okada de Oliveira
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Unit, Hospital das Clinicas, University of São Paulo Medical School, São Paulo, Brazil
- Hospital Santa Marcelina, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
| | - Patricio Donnelly Kehoe
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Multimedia Signal Processing Group - Neuroimage Division, French-Argentine International Center for Information and Systems Sciences, Rosario, Argentina
| | - Adolfo M. Garcia
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Universidad de San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Faculty of Education, National University of Cuyo, Mendoza, Argentina
| | | | - Hernando Santamaria-Garcia
- Memory and Cognition Clinic, Intellectus, Hospital Universitario San Ignacio, Bogotá, Colombia
- Ph.D. Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, Physiology, Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia
| | - Sebastian Moguilner
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
| | - Claudia Duran-Aniotz
- School of Psychology, Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, Latin American Institute for Brain Health (BrainLat), Universidad Adolfo Ibanez, Adolfo Ibanez University, Santiago, Chile
| | - Enzo Tagliazucchi
- Departamento de Física, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Marcelo Maito
- Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Universidad de San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | - Maritza Pintado-Caipa
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Unit Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Prevention, Cognitive Neurology Center, Peruvian Institute of Neurosciences, Lima, Perú
| | - Maria Eugenia Godoy
- Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Universidad de San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Vera Bakman
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
| | - Shireen Javandel
- Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| | - Kenneth S. Kosik
- Department of Molecular, Cellular, and Developmental Biology, Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States
| | - Victor Valcour
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| | - Bruce L. Miller
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- Department of Neurology, Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Brusa A, Bordone G, Proverbio AM. Measuring implicit mental representations related to ethnic stereotypes with ERPs: An exploratory study. Neuropsychologia 2021; 155:107808. [PMID: 33636156 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107808] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2020] [Revised: 01/27/2021] [Accepted: 02/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The present investigation used ERPs to detect the activation of implicit stereotypical representations associated to different ethnic groups, by means of an implicit paradigm. 285 sentences were presented to 20 Italian Caucasian participants while EEG signals were recorded from 128 scalp sites. Sentences could either violate (Incongruent condition), non-violate (Congruent condition) or be neutral (Neutral condition) with regard to stereotypical concepts concerning non-Caucasian ethnic groups (e.g. Asians, Africans, Arabs). No awareness or judgment about stereotypes was required. Participants were engaged in a fictitious task, ignoring the overall study's purpose. The results showed that Incongruent terminal words elicited a greater anterior N400 response (300-500 ms) compared to Congruent and Neutral words, reflecting a difficulty in integrating the information incongruent with pre-existing stereotypical knowledge. The participant's individual amplitude values of the N400-Difference Wave (Incongruent - Congruent), showed a direct correlation with the individual racism scores obtained at the Subtle and Blatant Prejudice Scale, administered at the end of the experimental session. Intra-cortical sources explaining the N400 involved areas of the social cognition network such as the medial frontal cortex (BA10) and the inferior temporal gyrus (BA20) which are known to support processing of information about other people and impression formation. Moreover, Congruent terminal words evoked a greater P300 response (500-600 ms) compared to the other conditions, possibly reflecting the merging of incoming inputs with anticipated semantic information. A late post-N400 frontal positivity (650-800 ms) was found to be larger to sentences concerning other-race characters (ether congruent or incongruent) compared to sentences involving own-race characters (neutral). The study corroborated the effectiveness of neurophysiological measures to assess implicit complex semantic representations and circumventing social desirability-related problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alessandra Brusa
- Neuro-Mi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza Dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1, 20126, Milan, Italy.
| | - Giorgia Bordone
- Neuro-Mi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza Dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1, 20126, Milan, Italy.
| | - Alice Mado Proverbio
- Neuro-Mi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Piazza Dell'Ateneo Nuovo, 1, 20126, Milan, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Tschuemperlin RM, Batschelet HM, Moggi F, Koenig T, Roesner S, Keller A, Pfeifer P, Soravia LM, Stein M. The Neurophysiology of Implicit Alcohol Associations in Recently Abstinent Patients With Alcohol Use Disorder: An Event-Related Potential Study Considering Gender Effects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2020; 44:2031-2044. [PMID: 32880981 PMCID: PMC7693094 DOI: 10.1111/acer.14444] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2020] [Accepted: 08/17/2020] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Neuroscientific models of alcohol use disorders (AUDs) postulate an imbalance between automatic, implicit, and controlled (conscious) processes. Implicit associations towards alcohol indicate the automatically attributed appeal of alcohol-related stimuli. First, behavioral studies indicate that negative alcohol associations are less pronounced in patients compared to controls, but potential neurophysiological differences remain unexplored. This study investigates neurophysiological correlates of implicit alcohol associations in recently abstinent patients with AUD for the first time, including possible gender effects. METHODS A total of 62 patients (40 males and 22 females) and 21 controls performed an alcohol valence Implicit Association Test, combining alcohol-related pictures with positive (incongruent condition) or negative (congruent condition) words, while brain activity was recorded using 64-channel electroencephalography. Event-related potentials (ERPs) for alcohol-negative and alcohol-positive trials were computed. Microstate analyses investigated the effects of group (patients, controls) and condition (incongruent, congruent); furthermore, possible gender effects in patients were analyzed. Significant effects were localized with standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic topography analysis. RESULTS Although no behavioral group differences were found, ERPs of patients and controls were characterized by distinct microstates from 320 ms onwards. ERPs between conditions differed only in patients with higher signal strength during incongruent trials. Around 600 ms, controls displayed higher signal strength than patients. A gender effect mirrored this pattern with enhanced signal strength in females as opposed to male patients. Around 690 ms, a group-by-valence interaction indicated enhanced signal strength in congruent compared to incongruent trials, which was more pronounced in controls. CONCLUSIONS For patients with AUD, the pattern, timing, and source localization of effects suggest greater effort regarding semantic and self-relevant integration around 400 ms during incongruent trials and attenuated emotional processing during the late positive potential timeframe. Interestingly, this emotional attenuation seemed reduced in female patients, thus corroborating the importance of gender-sensitive research and potential treatment of AUD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raphaela Martina Tschuemperlin
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.,Clinic Suedhang,, Kirchlindach, Switzerland.,Graduate School for Health Sciences, (RMT, HMB), University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Hallie Margareta Batschelet
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.,Graduate School for Health Sciences, (RMT, HMB), University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Franz Moggi
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Thomas Koenig
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Susanne Roesner
- Addiction Treatment Center, (SR, AK), Forel Clinic, Ellikon an der Thur, Switzerland
| | - Anne Keller
- Addiction Treatment Center, (SR, AK), Forel Clinic, Ellikon an der Thur, Switzerland
| | - Philippe Pfeifer
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| | - Leila Maria Soravia
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.,Clinic Suedhang,, Kirchlindach, Switzerland
| | - Maria Stein
- From the, Translational Research Center, (RMT, HMB, FM, TK, PP, LMS, MS), University Hospital of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.,Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, (MS), Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Lahtinen A, Juvonen K, Lapveteläinen A, Kolehmainen M, Lindholm M, Tanila H, Kantanen T, Sinikallio S, Karhunen L, Närväinen J. Metabolic state as a modulator of neural event-related potentials for food stimuli in an implicit association test. Physiol Behav 2019; 209:112589. [DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112589] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2019] [Revised: 06/14/2019] [Accepted: 06/22/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
10
|
Cui X, Cheng Q, Lin W, Lin J, Mo L. Different influences of facial attractiveness on judgments of moral beauty and moral goodness. Sci Rep 2019; 9:12152. [PMID: 31434984 PMCID: PMC6704261 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-48649-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2019] [Accepted: 08/09/2019] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Are beauty and goodness the same? The relationship between beauty and goodness has long been a controversial issue in the fields of philosophy, aesthetics, ethics and psychology. Although many empirical studies have explored moral judgment and aesthetic judgment separately, only a few studies have compared the two. Whether these two judgments are two different processes or the same process with two different labels remains unclear. To answer this question, the present study directly compared the influence of facial attractiveness on judgments of moral goodness and moral beauty and revealed distinct contributions of imaging perceptions to these two judgments. The results showed that in the moral beauty judgment task, participants gave higher scores to characters with attractive faces compared with characters with unattractive faces, and larger P200 and LPP were elicited in the unattractive-face condition compared with the attractive-face condition; while in the moral goodness judgment task, there was no significant difference between the two conditions of either behaviour or ERP data. These findings offer important insights into the understanding and comparison of the processes of moral judgment and aesthetic judgment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xuan Cui
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Qiuping Cheng
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Wuji Lin
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Jiabao Lin
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China
| | - Lei Mo
- Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, 510631, China.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Increased moral condemnation of accidental harm in institutionalized adolescents. Sci Rep 2018; 8:11609. [PMID: 30072749 PMCID: PMC6072742 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-29956-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2017] [Accepted: 07/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Social deprivation, as faced by children in institutional rearing, involves socio-cognitive deficits that may persist into adolescence. In particular, two relevant domains which prove sensitive to pre-adult neurodevelopment are theory of mind (ToM) and moral judgment (a complex skill which partially depend upon ToM). However, no study has assessed moral evaluation in adolescents with a history of institutional care, let alone its relationship with ToM skills. The present study aims to bridge this gap, focusing on moral evaluation of harmful actions in institutionalized adolescents (IAs). Relative to adolescents raised with their biological families, IAs exhibited less willingness to exculpate protagonists for accidental harms, suggesting an under-reliance on information about a person's (innocent) intentions. Moreover, such abnormalities in IAs were associated with ToM impairments. Taken together, our findings extend previous findings of delayed ToM under social deprivation, further showing that the development of moral cognition is also vulnerable to the impact of institutionalization. These results could pave the way for novel research on the role of institutional rearing in ToM and moral development during adolescence.
Collapse
|
12
|
Trujillo SP, Valencia S, Trujillo N, Ugarriza JE, Rodríguez MV, Rendón J, Pineda DA, López JD, Ibañez A, Parra MA. Atypical Modulations of N170 Component during Emotional Processing and Their Links to Social Behaviors in Ex-combatants. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:244. [PMID: 28588462 PMCID: PMC5440593 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2017] [Accepted: 04/25/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotional processing (EP) is crucial for the elaboration and implementation of adaptive social strategies. EP is also necessary for the expression of social cognition and behavior (SCB) patterns. It is well-known that war contexts induce socio-emotional atypical functioning, in particular for those who participate in combats. Thus, ex-combatants represent an ideal non-clinical population to explore EP modulation and to evaluate its relation with SCB. The aim of this study was to explore EP and its relation with SCB dimensions such as empathy, theory of mind and social skills in a sample of 50 subjects, of which 30 were ex-combatants from illegally armed groups in Colombia, and 20 controls without combat experience. We adapted an Emotional Recognition Task for faces and words and synchronized it with electroencephalographic recording. Ex-combatants presented with higher assertion skills and showed more pronounced brain responses to faces than Controls. They did not show the bias toward anger observed in control participants whereby the latter group was more likely to misclassify neutral faces as angry. However, ex-combatants showed an atypical word valence processing. That is, words with different emotions yielded no differences in N170 modulations. SCB variables were successfully predicted by neurocognitive variables. Our results suggest that in ex-combatants the links between EP and SCB functions are reorganized. This may reflect neurocognitive modulations associated to chronic exposure to war experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra P Trujillo
- Doctoral Program in Psychology, Department of Experimental Psychology, Universidad de GranadaGranada, Spain.,GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Stella Valencia
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Natalia Trujillo
- GISAME, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia.,Neuroscience Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Juan E Ugarriza
- Facultad de Jurisprudencia, Universidad del RosarioBogotá, Colombia
| | - Mónica V Rodríguez
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Jorge Rendón
- Neuroscience Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia.,Neuropsychology and Behavior Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia.,Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht UniversityMaastricht, Netherlands
| | - David A Pineda
- Neuropsychology and Behavior Group, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - José D López
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de Antioquia (UdeA),Medellín, Colombia
| | - Agustín Ibañez
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia.,Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo IbañezSantiago, Chile.,National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina.,Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Translational and Cognitive Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina.,ACR Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, SydneyNSW, Australia
| | - Mario A Parra
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia.,Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Heriot-Watt UniversityEdinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Trujillo S, Trujillo N, Lopez JD, Gomez D, Valencia S, Rendon J, Pineda DA, Parra MA. Social Cognitive Training Improves Emotional Processing and Reduces Aggressive Attitudes in Ex-combatants. Front Psychol 2017; 8:510. [PMID: 28428767 PMCID: PMC5382221 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00510] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2016] [Accepted: 03/20/2017] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
Emotional processing (EP) is a complex cognitive function necessary to successfully adjust to social environments where we need to interpret and respond to cues that convey threat or reward signals. Ex-combatants have consistently shown atypical EP as well as poor social interactions. Available reintegration programs aim to facilitate the re-adaptation of ex-combatants to their communities. However, they do not incorporate actions to improve EP and to enhance cognitive-emotional regulation. The present study was aimed at evaluating the usefulness of an intervention focused on Social Cognitive Training (SCT), which was designed to equip ex-combatants enrolled in the Social Reintegration Route with EP and social cognition skills. A group of 31 ex-combatants (mean age of 37.2, 29 men) from Colombian illegal armed groups were recruited into this study. Of these, 16 were invited to take part in a SCT and the other continued with the conventional reintegration intervention. Both groups underwent 12 training sessions in a period 12–14 weeks. They were assessed with a comprehensive protocol which included Psychosocial, Behavioral, and Emotion Processing instruments. The scores on these instruments prior to and after the intervention were compared within and between groups. Both groups were matched at baseline. Ex-combatants receiving the SCT experienced significant improvements in EP and a reduction in aggressive attitudes, effects not observed in those continuing the conventional reintegration intervention. This is the first study that achieves such outcomes in such a population using SCT intervention. We discuss the implications of such results toward better social reintegration strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra Trujillo
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia.,Doctoral Program in Psychology, Department of Psychology, Universidad de GranadaGranada, España.,Department of Experimental Psychology, University of GranadaGranada, Spain
| | - Natalia Trujillo
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia.,Grupo de Neurociencias, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellin, Colombia
| | - Jose D Lopez
- SISTEMIC, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia
| | - Diana Gomez
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia
| | - Stella Valencia
- Grupo de Investigación en Salud Mental, Facultad Nacional de Salud Pública, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellín, Colombia
| | - Jorge Rendon
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht UniversityMaastricht, Netherlands
| | - David A Pineda
- Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Universidad de AntioquiaMedellin, Colombia
| | - Mario A Parra
- School of Social Sciences, Psychology, Heriot-Watt UniversityEdinburgh, UK.,Cerebro, Cognición y Procesos Sociales, Psicologia, Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Adolfi F, Couto B, Richter F, Decety J, Lopez J, Sigman M, Manes F, Ibáñez A. Convergence of interoception, emotion, and social cognition: A twofold fMRI meta-analysis and lesion approach. Cortex 2017; 88:124-142. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 100] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2016] [Revised: 11/16/2016] [Accepted: 12/20/2016] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
|
15
|
García AM, Sedeño L, Herrera Murcia E, Couto B, Ibáñez A. A Lesion-Proof Brain? Multidimensional Sensorimotor, Cognitive, and Socio-Affective Preservation Despite Extensive Damage in a Stroke Patient. Front Aging Neurosci 2017; 8:335. [PMID: 28119603 PMCID: PMC5222788 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00335] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2016] [Accepted: 12/23/2016] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
In this study, we report an unusual case of mutidimensional sensorimotor, cognitive, and socio-affective preservation in an adult with extensive, acquired bilateral brain damage. At age 43, patient CG sustained a cerebral hemorrhage and a few months later, she suffered a second (ischemic) stroke. As a result, she exhibited extensive damage of the right hemisphere (including frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital regions), left Sylvian and striatal areas, bilateral portions of the insula and the amygdala, and the splenium. However, against all probability, she was unimpaired across a host of cognitive domains, including executive functions, attention, memory, language, sensory perception (e.g., taste recognition and intensity discrimination), emotional processing (e.g., experiencing of positive and negative emotions), and social cognition skills (prosody recognition, theory of mind, facial emotion recognition, and emotional evaluation). Her functional integrity was further confirmed through neurological examination and contextualized observation of her performance in real-life tasks. In sum, CG's case resists straightforward classifications, as the extent and distribution of her lesions would typically produce pervasive, multidimensional deficits. We discuss the rarity of this patient against the backdrop of other reports of atypical cognitive preservation, expound the limitations of several potential accounts, and highlight the challenges that the case poses for current theories of brain organization and resilience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adolfo M García
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET)Buenos Aires, Argentina; Faculty of Elementary and Special Education, National University of CuyoMendoza, Argentina
| | - Lucas Sedeño
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET)Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | - Blas Couto
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro University Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Agustín Ibáñez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience, INECO Foundation, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET)Buenos Aires, Argentina; Universidad Autónoma del CaribeBarranquilla, Colombia; Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo IbáñezSantiago de Chile, Chile; Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research CouncilSydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Pfister R, Wirth R, Schwarz KA, Foerster A, Steinhauser M, Kunde W. The electrophysiological signature of deliberate rule violations. Psychophysiology 2016; 53:1870-1877. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12771] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2016] [Accepted: 09/12/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Roland Pfister
- Department of Psychology; Julius Maximilians University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | - Robert Wirth
- Department of Psychology; Julius Maximilians University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | - Katharina A. Schwarz
- Department of Psychology; Julius Maximilians University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | - Anna Foerster
- Department of Psychology; Julius Maximilians University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| | - Marco Steinhauser
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt; Eichstätt Germany
| | - Wilfried Kunde
- Department of Psychology; Julius Maximilians University of Würzburg; Würzburg Germany
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Schindler S, Wolff W. Cerebral Correlates of Automatic Associations Towards Performance Enhancing Substances. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1923. [PMID: 26733914 PMCID: PMC4686700 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01923] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2015] [Accepted: 11/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The direct assessment of explicit attitudes toward performance enhancing substances, for example Neuroenhancement or doping in sports, can be affected by social desirability biases and cheating attempts. According to Dual Process Theories of cognition, indirect measures like the Implicit Association Test (IAT) measure automatic associations toward a topic (as opposed to explicit attitudes measured by self-report measures). Such automatic associations are thought to occur rapidly and to evade voluntary control. However, whether or not such indirect tests actually reflect automatic associations is difficult to validate. Electroencephalography (EEG) has a superior time resolution which can differentiate between highly automatic compared to more elaborate processing stages. We therefore used EEG to examine on which processing stages cortical differences between negative or positive attitudes to doping occur, and whether or not these differences can be related to BIAT scores. We tested 42 university students (31 females, 24.43 ± 3.17 years old), who were requested to complete a brief doping IAT (BIAT) on attitudes toward doping. Cerebral activity during doping BIAT completion was assessed using high-density EEG. Behaviorally, participants D-scores exhibited negative attitudes toward doping, represented by faster reaction times in the doping + dislike pairing task. Event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed earliest effects between 200 and 300 ms. Here, a relatively larger occipital positivity was found for the doping + dislike pairing task. Further, in the LPP time range between 400 and 600 ms a larger late positive potential was found for the doping + dislike pairing task over central regions. These LPP amplitude differences were successfully predicting participants' BIAT D-scores. Results indicate that event-related potentials differentiate between positive and negative doping attitudes at stages of mid-latency. However, it seems that IAT scores can be predicted only by the later occurring LPP. Our study is the first to investigate the cerebral correlates that contribute to test scores obtained in the indirect testing of automatic associations toward doping. The implications of our results for the broader NE concept are discussed in light of the conceptual similarity of doping and NE.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Schindler
- Department of Psychology, Bielefeld UniversityBielefeld, Germany
- Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology, Bielefeld UniversityBielefeld, Germany
| | - Wanja Wolff
- Division of Sport and Exercise Psychology, University of PotsdamPotsdam, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Healy GF, Boran L, Smeaton AF. Neural Patterns of the Implicit Association Test. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:605. [PMID: 26635570 PMCID: PMC4656831 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2015] [Accepted: 10/20/2015] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
The Implicit Association Test (IAT) is a reaction time based categorization task that measures the differential associative strength between bipolar targets and evaluative attribute concepts as an approach to indexing implicit beliefs or biases. An open question exists as to what exactly the IAT measures, and here EEG (Electroencephalography) has been used to investigate the time course of ERPs (Event-related Potential) indices and implicated brain regions in the IAT. IAT-EEG research identifies a number of early (250-450 ms) negative ERPs indexing early-(pre-response) processing stages of the IAT. ERP activity in this time range is known to index processes related to cognitive control and semantic processing. A central focus of these efforts has been to use IAT-ERPs to delineate the implicit and explicit factors contributing to measured IAT effects. Increasing evidence indicates that cognitive control (and related top-down modulation of attention/perceptual processing) may be components in the effective measurement of IAT effects, as factors such as physical setting or task instruction can change an IAT measurement. In this study we further implicate the role of proactive cognitive control and top-down modulation of attention/perceptual processing in the IAT-EEG. We find statistically significant relationships between D-score (a reaction-time based measure of the IAT-effect) and early ERP-time windows, indicating where more rapid word categorizations driving the IAT effect are present, they are at least partly explainable by neural activity not significantly correlated with the IAT measurement itself. Using LORETA, we identify a number of brain regions driving these ERP-IAT relationships notably involving left-temporal, insular, cingulate, medial frontal and parietal cortex in time regions corresponding to the N2- and P3-related activity. The identified brain regions involved with reduced reaction times on congruent blocks coincide with those of previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Graham F Healy
- Insight Centre for Data Analytics, School of Computing, Dublin City University Dublin, Ireland
| | - Lorraine Boran
- School of Nursing and Human Sciences, Dublin City University Dublin, Ireland
| | - Alan F Smeaton
- Insight Centre for Data Analytics, School of Computing, Dublin City University Dublin, Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Wu X, Chen J, Jia T, Ma W, Zhang Y, Deng Z, Yang L. Cognitive Bias by Gender Interaction on N170 Response to Emotional Facial Expressions in Major and Minor Depression. Brain Topogr 2015; 29:232-42. [PMID: 26239020 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-015-0444-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2015] [Accepted: 07/29/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
States of depression are considered to relate to a cognitive bias reactivity to emotional events. Moreover, gender effect may influence differences in emotional processing. The current study is to investigate whether there is an interaction of cognitive bias by gender on emotional processing in minor depression (MiD) and major depression (MaD). N170 component was obtained during a visual emotional oddball paradigm to manipulate the processing of emotional information in 33 MiD, 36 MaD, and 32 controls (CN). Compared with CN, in male, both MiD and MaD had lower N170 amplitudes for happy faces, but MaD had higher N170 amplitudes for sad faces; in female, both MiD and MaD had lower N170 amplitudes for happy and neutral faces, but higher N170 amplitudes for sad faces. Compared with MaD in male, MiD had higher N170 amplitudes for happy faces, lower N170 amplitudes for sad faces; in female, MiD only had higher N170 amplitudes for sad faces. Interestingly, a negative relationship was observed between N170 amplitude and the HDRS score for identification of happy faces in depressed patients while N170 amplitude was positively correlated with the HDRS score for sad faces identification. These results provide novel evidence for the mood-brightening effect with an interaction of cognitive bias by gender on emotional processing. It further suggests that female depression may be more vulnerable than male during emotional face processing with the unconscious negative cognitive bias and depressive syndromes may exist on a spectrum of severity on emotional face processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xingqu Wu
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China
| | - Jiu Chen
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China. .,Department of Neurology of Affiliated ZhongDa Hospital, Medical School and Institute of Neuropsychiatry of Southeast University, 87 DingJiaQiao Road, Nanjing, 210009, Jiangsu, People's Republic of China.
| | - Ting Jia
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China
| | - Wentao Ma
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China
| | - Yan Zhang
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China
| | - Zihe Deng
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China
| | - Laiqi Yang
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People's Liberation Army, No. 45, Dongfeng Road, Baoji, 721004, Shaanxi, People's Republic of China.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Wu L, Gu R, Cai H, Zhang J. Electrophysiological evidence for executive control and efficient categorization involved in implicit self-evaluation. Soc Neurosci 2015; 11:153-63. [DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1044673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
21
|
Chen J, Ma W, Zhang Y, Wu X, Wei D, Liu G, Deng Z, Yang L, Zhang Z. Distinct facial processing related negative cognitive bias in first-episode and recurrent major depression: evidence from the N170 ERP component. PLoS One 2014; 9:e109176. [PMID: 25314024 PMCID: PMC4196777 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2014] [Accepted: 09/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND States of depression are associated with increased sensitivity to negative events. For this novel study, we have assessed the relationship between the number of depressive episodes and the dysfunctional processing of emotional facial expressions. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS We used a visual emotional oddball paradigm to manipulate the processing of emotional information while event-related brain potentials were recorded in 45 patients with first episode major depression (F-MD), 40 patients with recurrent major depression (R-MD), and 46 healthy controls (HC). Compared with the HC group, F-MD patients had lower N170 amplitudes when identifying happy, neutral, and sad faces; R-MD patients had lower N170 amplitudes when identifying happy and neutral faces, but higher N170 amplitudes when identifying sad faces. F-MD patients had longer N170 latencies when identifying happy, neutral, and sad faces relative to the HC group, and R-MD patients had longer N170 latencies when identifying happy and neutral faces, but shorter N170 latencies when identifying sad faces compared with F-MD patients. Interestingly, a negative relationship was observed between N170 amplitude and the depressive severity score for identification of happy faces in R-MD patients while N170 amplitude was positively correlated with the depressive severity score for identification of sad faces in F-MD and R-MD patients. Additionally, the deficits of N170 amplitude for sad faces positively correlated with the number of depressive episodes in R-MD patients. CONCLUSION/SIGNIFICANCE These results provide new evidence that having more recurrent depressive episodes and serious depressive states are likely to aggravate the already abnormal processing of emotional facial expressions in patients with depression. Moreover, it further suggests that the impaired processing as indexed by N170 amplitude for positive face identification may be a potentially useful biomarker for predicting propagation of depression while N170 amplitude for negative face identification could be a potential biomarker for depression recurrence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiu Chen
- Neurologic Department of Affiliated ZhongDa Hospital, Neuropsychiatric Institute and Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, Jiangsu Province, China
| | - Wentao Ma
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Yan Zhang
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Xingqu Wu
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Dunhong Wei
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Guangxiong Liu
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Zihe Deng
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Laiqi Yang
- Center for Mental Disease Control and Prevention, Third Hospital of the People’s Liberation Army, Baoji, Shaanxi Province, China
| | - Zhijun Zhang
- Neurologic Department of Affiliated ZhongDa Hospital, Neuropsychiatric Institute and Medical School of Southeast University, Nanjing, Jiangsu Province, China
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Tobón C, Ibañez A, Velilla L, Duque J, Ochoa J, Trujillo N, Decety J, Pineda D. Emotional processing in Colombian ex-combatants and its relationship with empathy and executive functions. Soc Neurosci 2014; 10:153-65. [DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2014.969406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
23
|
Amodio DM, Bartholow BD, Ito TA. Tracking the dynamics of the social brain: ERP approaches for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9:385-93. [PMID: 24319116 PMCID: PMC3980796 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 101] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/17/2013] [Accepted: 11/28/2013] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Event-related potential (ERP) approaches to social cognitive and affective neuroscience (SCAN) are not as widely used as other neuroimaging techniques, yet they offer several unique advantages. In particular, the high temporal resolution of ERP measures of neural activity make them ideally suited for studying the dynamic interplay of rapidly unfolding cognitive and affective processes. In this article, we highlight the utility of ERP methods for scientists investigating questions of SCAN. We begin with a brief description of the physiological basis of ERPs and discussion of methodological practices. We then discuss how ERPs may be used to address a range of questions concerning social perception, social cognition, attitudes, affect and self-regulation, with examples of research that has used the ERP approach to contribute important theoretical advances in these areas. Whether used alone or in combination with other techniques, the ERP is an indispensable part of the social and affective neuroscientist's methodological toolkit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David M Amodio
- Department of Psychology and Neural Science, New York University, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Ibáñez A, Aguado J, Baez S, Huepe D, Lopez V, Ortega R, Sigman M, Mikulan E, Lischinsky A, Torrente F, Cetkovich M, Torralva T, Bekinschtein T, Manes F. From neural signatures of emotional modulation to social cognition: individual differences in healthy volunteers and psychiatric participants. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 9:939-50. [PMID: 23685775 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst067] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
Abstract
It is commonly assumed that early emotional signals provide relevant information for social cognition tasks. The goal of this study was to test the association between (a) cortical markers of face emotional processing and (b) social-cognitive measures, and also to build a model which can predict this association (a and b) in healthy volunteers as well as in different groups of psychiatric patients. Thus, we investigated the early cortical processing of emotional stimuli (N170, using a face and word valence task) and their relationship with the social-cognitive profiles (SCPs, indexed by measures of theory of mind, fluid intelligence, speed processing and executive functions). Group comparisons and individual differences were assessed among schizophrenia (SCZ) patients and their relatives, individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), individuals with euthymic bipolar disorder (BD) and healthy participants (educational level, handedness, age and gender matched). Our results provide evidence of emotional N170 impairments in the affected groups (SCZ and relatives, ADHD and BD) as well as subtle group differences. Importantly, cortical processing of emotional stimuli predicted the SCP, as evidenced by a structural equation model analysis. This is the first study to report an association model of brain markers of emotional processing and SCP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustín Ibáñez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UKLaboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, C
| | - Jaume Aguado
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Sandra Baez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - David Huepe
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Vladimir Lopez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Rodrigo Ortega
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UKLaboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, C
| | - Mariano Sigman
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UKLaboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, C
| | - Ezequiel Mikulan
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Alicia Lischinsky
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Fernando Torrente
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Marcelo Cetkovich
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Teresa Torralva
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Tristan Bekinschtein
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Facundo Manes
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience (LANCYS), UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience (NUFIN), Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile, National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Parc Sanitari Joan de Deu-SSM, CIBERSAM, Universitat de Barcelona, Sant Boi, Barcelona, Spain, Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, y Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Programa de Doctorado en Psicología, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Departamento de Física, FCEN, UBA and IFIBA, Conicet, Pabellón 1, Ciudad Universitaria, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Alte. Juan Saenz Valiente 1010, Buenos Aires C1428BIJ, Argentina, and MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Tracking the implicit self using event-related potentials. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2013; 13:885-99. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-013-0169-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
|
26
|
Insular networks for emotional processing and social cognition: Comparison of two case reports with either cortical or subcortical involvement. Cortex 2013; 49:1420-34. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 76] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2012] [Revised: 04/27/2012] [Accepted: 08/10/2012] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
27
|
Amoruso L, Gelormini C, Aboitiz F, Alvarez González M, Manes F, Cardona JF, Ibanez A. N400 ERPs for actions: building meaning in context. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:57. [PMID: 23459873 PMCID: PMC3586681 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2012] [Accepted: 02/12/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Converging neuroscientific evidence suggests the existence of close links between language and sensorimotor cognition. Accordingly, during the comprehension of meaningful actions, our brain would recruit semantic-related operations similar to those associated with the processing of language information. Consistent with this view, electrophysiological findings show that the N400 component, traditionally linked to the semantic processing of linguistic material, can also be elicited by action-related material. This review outlines recent data from N400 studies that examine the understanding of action events. We focus on three specific domains, including everyday action comprehension, co-speech gesture integration, and the semantics involved in motor planning and execution. Based on the reviewed findings, we suggest that both negativities (the N400 and the action-N400) reflect a common neurocognitive mechanism involved in the construction of meaning through the expectancies created by previous experiences and current contextual information. To shed light on how this process is instantiated in the brain, a testable contextual fronto-temporo-parietal model is proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucía Amoruso
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Carlos Gelormini
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Francisco Aboitiz
- Department of Psychiatry, Medical School, Interdisciplinary Center of Neuroscience, Pontificia Universidad CatólicaSantiago, Chile
| | | | - Facundo Manes
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Juan F. Cardona
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Agustín Ibanez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Laboratory of Cognitive and Social Neuroscience, Universidad Diego PortalesSantiago, Chile
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Peng J, Qu C, Gu R, Luo YJ. Description-based reappraisal regulate the emotion induced by erotic and neutral images in a Chinese population. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 6:355. [PMID: 23335894 PMCID: PMC3541519 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00355] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2012] [Accepted: 12/21/2012] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous emotion-regulation research has shown that the late positive potential (LPP) is sensitive to the down-regulation of emotion; however, whether LPP is also sensitive to the up-regulation of emotion remains unclear. The present study examined the description-based reappraisal effects on the up-regulation of positive emotions induced by erotic and neutral images in a Chinese population. Self-reported ratings and event-related potential (ERP) were recorded when subjects viewed pleasant and neutral images, which were shown after either a neutral or positive description. Self-reported results showed that images following positive descriptions were rated as more pleasant compared to images following neutral descriptions. ERP results revealed that the P2, P3, and slow wave (SW) components were larger for erotic pictures than for neutral pictures, while the positive description condition yielded attenuated erotic image-induced P2, P3 and SW and increased SW induced by neutral images. The results demonstrated that description-based reappraisal, as a method of reappraisal, significantly modulates the emotional experience and ERP responses to erotic and neutral images.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiaxin Peng
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University Beijing, China
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Baez S, Rattazzi A, Gonzalez-Gadea ML, Torralva T, Vigliecca NS, Decety J, Manes F, Ibanez A. Integrating intention and context: assessing social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:302. [PMID: 23162450 PMCID: PMC3492863 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00302] [Citation(s) in RCA: 70] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2012] [Accepted: 10/19/2012] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Deficits in social cognition are an evident clinical feature of the Asperger syndrome (AS). Although many daily life problems of adults with AS are related to social cognition impairments, few studies have conducted comprehensive research in this area. The current study examined multiple domains of social cognition in adults with AS assessing the executive functions (EF) and exploring the intra and inter-individual variability. Fifteen adult's diagnosed with AS and 15 matched healthy controls completed a battery of social cognition tasks. This battery included measures of emotion recognition, theory of mind (ToM), empathy, moral judgment, social norms knowledge, and self-monitoring behavior in social settings. We controlled for the effect of EF and explored the individual variability. The results indicated that adults with AS had a fundamental deficit in several domains of social cognition. We also found high variability in the social cognition tasks. In these tasks, AS participants obtained mostly subnormal performance. EF did not seem to play a major role in the social cognition impairments. Our results suggest that adults with AS present a pattern of social cognition deficits characterized by the decreased ability to implicitly encode and integrate contextual information in order to access to the social meaning. Nevertheless, when social information is explicitly presented or the situation can be navigated with abstract rules, performance is improved. Our findings have implications for the diagnosis and treatment of individuals with AS as well as for the neurocognitive models of this syndrome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra Baez
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Pontifical Catholic University of ArgentinaBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Alexia Rattazzi
- Argentinean Program for Children, Adolescents and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders (PANAACEA)Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - María L. Gonzalez-Gadea
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Teresa Torralva
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Nora Silvana Vigliecca
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Research Centre of the Faculty of Philosophy and Humanities, National University of CórdobaCórdoba, Argentina
| | - Jean Decety
- Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, and Center for Cognitive and Social Neuroscience, University of ChicagoChicago, IL, USA
| | - Facundo Manes
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Agustin Ibanez
- Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro UniversityBuenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research CouncilBuenos Aires, Argentina
- Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Universidad Diego PortalesSantiago, Chile
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Ibanez A, Melloni M, Huepe D, Helgiu E, Rivera-Rei A, Canales-Johnson A, Baker P, Moya A. What event-related potentials (ERPs) bring to social neuroscience? Soc Neurosci 2012; 7:632-49. [DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2012.691078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
31
|
Ibanez A, Urquina H, Petroni A, Baez S, Lopez V, do Nascimento M, Herrera E, Guex R, Hurtado E, Blenkmann A, Beltrachini L, Gelormini C, Sigman M, Lischinsky A, Torralva T, Torrente F, Cetkovich M, Manes F. Neural processing of emotional facial and semantic expressions in euthymic bipolar disorder (BD) and its association with theory of mind (ToM). PLoS One 2012; 7:e46877. [PMID: 23056505 PMCID: PMC3466207 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046877] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2012] [Accepted: 09/10/2012] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Adults with bipolar disorder (BD) have cognitive impairments that affect face processing and social cognition. However, it remains unknown whether these deficits in euthymic BD have impaired brain markers of emotional processing. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS We recruited twenty six participants, 13 controls subjects with an equal number of euthymic BD participants. We used an event-related potential (ERP) assessment of a dual valence task (DVT), in which faces (angry and happy), words (pleasant and unpleasant), and face-word simultaneous combinations are presented to test the effects of the stimulus type (face vs word) and valence (positive vs. negative). All participants received clinical, neuropsychological and social cognition evaluations. ERP analysis revealed that both groups showed N170 modulation of stimulus type effects (face > word). BD patients exhibited reduced and enhanced N170 to facial and semantic valence, respectively. The neural source estimation of N170 was a posterior section of the fusiform gyrus (FG), including the face fusiform area (FFA). Neural generators of N170 for faces (FG and FFA) were reduced in BD. In these patients, N170 modulation was associated with social cognition (theory of mind). CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE This is the first report of euthymic BD exhibiting abnormal N170 emotional discrimination associated with theory of mind impairments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustin Ibanez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Melloni M, Urbistondo C, Sedeño L, Gelormini C, Kichic R, Ibanez A. The extended fronto-striatal model of obsessive compulsive disorder: convergence from event-related potentials, neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:259. [PMID: 23015786 PMCID: PMC3449438 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2012] [Accepted: 08/30/2012] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
In this work, we explored convergent evidence supporting the fronto-striatal model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (FSMOCD) and the contribution of event-related potential (ERP) studies to this model. First, we considered minor modifications to the FSMOCD model based on neuroimaging and neuropsychological data. We noted the brain areas most affected in this disorder -anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), basal ganglia (BG), and orbito-frontal cortex (OFC) and their related cognitive functions, such as monitoring and inhibition. Then, we assessed the ERPs that are directly related to the FSMOCD, including the error-related negativity (ERN), N200, and P600. Several OCD studies present enhanced ERN and N2 responses during conflict tasks as well as an enhanced P600 during working memory (WM) tasks. Evidence from ERP studies (especially regarding ERN and N200 amplitude enhancement), neuroimaging and neuropsychological findings suggests abnormal activity in the OFC, ACC, and BG in OCD patients. Moreover, additional findings from these analyses suggest dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal cortex involvement, which might be related to executive function (EF) deficits. Thus, these convergent results suggest the existence of a self-monitoring imbalance involving inhibitory deficits and executive dysfunctions. OCD patients present an impaired ability to monitor, control, and inhibit intrusive thoughts, urges, feelings, and behaviors. In the current model, this imbalance is triggered by an excitatory role of the BG (associated with cognitive or motor actions without volitional control) and inhibitory activity of the OFC as well as excessive monitoring of the ACC to block excitatory impulses. This imbalance would interact with the reduced activation of the parietal-DLPC network, leading to executive dysfunction. ERP research may provide further insight regarding the temporal dynamics of action monitoring and executive functioning in OCD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margherita Melloni
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) and Institute of Neuroscience, Favaloro University Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Ibáñez A, Cardona JF, Dos Santos YV, Blenkmann A, Aravena P, Roca M, Hurtado E, Nerguizian M, Amoruso L, Gómez-Arévalo G, Chade A, Dubrovsky A, Gershanik O, Kochen S, Glenberg A, Manes F, Bekinschtein T. Motor-language coupling: direct evidence from early Parkinson's disease and intracranial cortical recordings. Cortex 2012; 49:968-84. [PMID: 22482695 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2012] [Revised: 02/28/2012] [Accepted: 02/28/2012] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Language and action systems are functionally coupled in the brain as demonstrated by converging evidence using Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), and lesion studies. In particular, this coupling has been demonstrated using the action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE) in which motor activity and language interact. The ACE task requires participants to listen to sentences that described actions typically performed with an open hand (e.g., clapping), a closed hand (e.g., hammering), or without any hand action (neutral); and to press a large button with either an open hand position or closed hand position immediately upon comprehending each sentence. The ACE is defined as a longer reaction time (RT) in the action-sentence incompatible conditions than in the compatible conditions. Here we investigated direct motor-language coupling in two novel and uniquely informative ways. First, we measured the behavioural ACE in patients with motor impairment (early Parkinson's disease - EPD), and second, in epileptic patients with direct electrocorticography (ECoG) recordings. In experiment 1, EPD participants with preserved general cognitive repertoire, showed a much diminished ACE relative to non-EPD volunteers. Moreover, a correlation between ACE performance and action-verb processing (kissing and dancing test - KDT) was observed. Direct cortical recordings (ECoG) in motor and language areas (experiment 2) demonstrated simultaneous bidirectional effects: motor preparation affected language processing (N400 at left inferior frontal gyrus and middle/superior temporal gyrus), and language processing affected activity in movement-related areas (motor potential at premotor and M1). Our findings show that the ACE paradigm requires ongoing integration of preserved motor and language coupling (abolished in EPD) and engages motor-temporal cortices in a bidirectional way. In addition, both experiments suggest the presence of a motor-language network which is not restricted to somatotopically defined brain areas. These results open new pathways in the fields of motor diseases, theoretical approaches to language understanding, and models of action-perception coupling.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustín Ibáñez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO); Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Brown SBRE, van Steenbergen H, Band GPH, de Rover M, Nieuwenhuis S. Functional significance of the emotion-related late positive potential. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:33. [PMID: 22375117 PMCID: PMC3287021 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2011] [Accepted: 02/14/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The late positive potential (LPP) is an event-related potential (ERP) component over visual cortical areas that is modulated by the emotional intensity of a stimulus. However, the functional significance of this neural modulation remains elusive. We conducted two experiments in which we studied the relation between LPP amplitude, subsequent perceptual sensitivity to a non-emotional stimulus (Experiment 1) and visual cortical excitability, as reflected by P1/N1 components evoked by this stimulus (Experiment 2). During the LPP modulation elicited by unpleasant stimuli, perceptual sensitivity was not affected. In contrast, we found some evidence for a decreased N1 amplitude during the LPP modulation, a decreased P1 amplitude on trials with a relatively large LPP, and consistent negative (but non-significant) across-subject correlations between the magnitudes of the LPP modulation and corresponding changes in d-prime or P1/N1 amplitude. The results provide preliminary evidence that the LPP reflects a global inhibition of activity in visual cortex, resulting in the selective survival of activity associated with the processing of the emotional stimulus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen B R E Brown
- Department of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Leiden University Leiden, Netherlands
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Beta receptor-mediated modulation of the late positive potential in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012; 219:971-9. [PMID: 21847570 PMCID: PMC3266504 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-011-2426-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2011] [Accepted: 07/25/2011] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
Abstract
RATIONALE Electrophysiological studies have identified a scalp potential, the late positive potential (LPP), which is modulated by the emotional intensity of observed stimuli. Previous work has shown that the LPP reflects the modulation of activity in extrastriate visual cortical structures, but little is known about the source of that modulation. OBJECTIVES The present study investigated whether beta-adrenergic receptors are involved in the generation of the LPP. METHODS We used a genetic individual differences approach (experiment 1) and a pharmacological manipulation (experiment 2) to test the hypothesis that the LPP is modulated by the activation of β-adrenergic receptors. RESULTS In experiment 1, we found that LPP amplitude depends on allelic variation in the β1-receptor gene polymorphism. In experiment 2, we found that LPP amplitude was modulated by the β-blocker propranolol in a direction dependent on subjects' level of trait anxiety: In participants with lower trait anxiety, propranolol led to a (nonsignificant) decrease in the LPP modulation; in participants with higher trait anxiety, propranolol increased the emotion-related LPP modulation. CONCLUSIONS These results provide initial support for the hypothesis that the LPP reflects the downstream effects, in visual cortical areas, of β-receptor-mediated activation of the amygdala.
Collapse
|
36
|
Ibáñez A, Petroni A, Urquina H, Torrente F, Torralva T, Hurtado E, Guex R, Blenkmann A, Beltrachini L, Muravchik C, Baez S, Cetkovich M, Sigman M, Lischinsky A, Manes F. Cortical deficits of emotional face processing in adults with ADHD: Its relation to social cognition and executive function. Soc Neurosci 2011; 6:464-81. [DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2011.620769] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
|
37
|
Jessen S, Kotz SA. The temporal dynamics of processing emotions from vocal, facial, and bodily expressions. Neuroimage 2011; 58:665-74. [PMID: 21718792 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 114] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2011] [Revised: 04/01/2011] [Accepted: 06/13/2011] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- S Jessen
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1A, 04107 Leipzig, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Facial and semantic emotional interference: a pilot study on the behavioral and cortical responses to the Dual Valence Association Task. Behav Brain Funct 2011; 7:8. [PMID: 21489277 PMCID: PMC3087672 DOI: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2010] [Accepted: 04/13/2011] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Integration of compatible or incompatible emotional valence and semantic information is an essential aspect of complex social interactions. A modified version of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) called Dual Valence Association Task (DVAT) was designed in order to measure conflict resolution processing from compatibility/incompatibly of semantic and facial valence. The DVAT involves two emotional valence evaluative tasks which elicits two forms of emotional compatible/incompatible associations (facial and semantic). Methods Behavioural measures and Event Related Potentials were recorded while participants performed the DVAT. Results Behavioural data showed a robust effect that distinguished compatible/incompatible tasks. The effects of valence and contextual association (between facial and semantic stimuli) showed early discrimination in N170 of faces. The LPP component was modulated by the compatibility of the DVAT. Conclusions Results suggest that DVAT is a robust paradigm for studying the emotional interference effect in the processing of simultaneous information from semantic and facial stimuli.
Collapse
|
39
|
Theories and Theorizers: A Contextual Approach to Theories of Cognition. Integr Psychol Behav Sci 2011; 45:223-46. [DOI: 10.1007/s12124-011-9156-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
40
|
Ibáñez A, Toro P, Cornejo C, Urquina H, Manes F, Weisbrod M, Schröder J. High contextual sensitivity of metaphorical expressions and gesture blending: A video event-related potential design. Psychiatry Res 2011; 191:68-75. [PMID: 21129937 DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/07/2008] [Revised: 08/05/2010] [Accepted: 08/12/2010] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Human communication in a natural context implies the dynamic coordination of contextual clues, paralinguistic information and literal as well as figurative language use. In the present study we constructed a paradigm with four types of video clips: literal and metaphorical expressions accompanied by congruent and incongruent gesture actions. Participants were instructed to classify the gesture accompanying the expression as congruent or incongruent by pressing two different keys while electrophysiological activity was being recorded. We compared behavioral measures and event related potential (ERP) differences triggered by the gesture stroke onset. Accuracy data showed that incongruent metaphorical expressions were more difficult to classify. Reaction times were modulated by incongruent gestures, by metaphorical expressions and by a gesture-expression interaction. No behavioral differences were found between the literal and metaphorical expressions when the gesture was congruent. N400-like and LPC-like (late positive complex) components from metaphorical expressions produced greater negativity. The N400-like modulation of metaphorical expressions showed a greater difference between congruent and incongruent categories over the left anterior region, compared with the literal expressions. More importantly, the literal congruent as well as the metaphorical congruent categories did not show any difference. Accuracy, reaction times and ERPs provide convergent support for a greater contextual sensitivity of the metaphorical expressions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustín Ibáñez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neurosciences, Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Buenos Aires, Argentina.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Ibáñez A, Gleichgerrcht E, Hurtado E, González R, Haye A, Manes FF. Early Neural Markers of Implicit Attitudes: N170 Modulated by Intergroup and Evaluative Contexts in IAT. Front Hum Neurosci 2010; 4:188. [PMID: 21079750 PMCID: PMC2978037 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2010] [Accepted: 09/22/2010] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The Implicit Association Test (IAT) is the most popular measure to evaluate implicit attitudes. Nevertheless, its neural correlates are not yet fully understood. We examined event related potentials (ERPs) in response to face- and word processing while indigenous and non-indigenous participants performed an IAT displaying faces (ingroup and outgroup members) and words (positive and negative valence) as targets of category judgments. The N170 component was modulated by valence of words and by ingroup/outgroup face categorization. Contextual effects (face–words implicitly associated in the task) had an influence on the N170 amplitude modulation. On the one hand, in face categorization, right N170 showed differences according to the association between social categories of faces and affective valence of words. On the other, in word categorization, left N170 presented a similar modulation when the task implied a negative-valence associated with ingroup faces. Only indigenous participants showed a significant IAT effect and N170 differences. Our results demonstrate an early ERP blending of stimuli processing with both intergroup and evaluative contexts, suggesting an integration of contextual information related to intergroup attitudes during the early stages of word and face processing. To our knowledge, this is the first report of early ERPs during an ethnicity IAT, opening a new branch of exchange between social neuroscience and social psychology of attitudes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agustín Ibáñez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neurosciences, Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Williams JK, Themanson JR. Neural correlates of the implicit association test: evidence for semantic and emotional processing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2010; 6:468-76. [PMID: 20601422 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsq065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The implicit association test (IAT) has been widely used in social cognitive research over the past decade. Controversies have arisen over what cognitive processes are being tapped into using this task. While most models use behavioral (RT) results to support their claims, little research has examined neurocognitive correlates of these behavioral measures. The present study measured event-related brain potentials (ERPs) of participants while completing a gay-straight IAT in order to further understand the processes involved in a typical group bias IAT. Results indicated significantly smaller N400 amplitudes and significantly larger LPP amplitudes for compatible trials than for incompatible trials, suggesting that both the semantic and emotional congruence of stimuli paired together in an IAT trial contribute to the typical RT differences found, while no differences were present for earlier ERP components including the N2. These findings are discussed with respect to early and late processing in group bias IATs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John K Williams
- Rosemead School of Psychology, Biola University, 13800 Biola Avenue, La Mirada, CA 90639, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
San Martín R, Manes F, Hurtado E, Isla P, Ibañez A. Size and probability of rewards modulate the feedback error-related negativity associated with wins but not losses in a monetarily rewarded gambling task. Neuroimage 2010; 51:1194-204. [PMID: 20302950 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 102] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2009] [Revised: 03/06/2010] [Accepted: 03/09/2010] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
|
44
|
Dufey M, Hurtado E, Fernández AM, Manes F, Ibáñez A. Exploring the relationship between vagal tone and event-related potentials in response to an affective picture task. Soc Neurosci 2010; 6:48-62. [PMID: 20419566 DOI: 10.1080/17470911003691402] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
The present study is the first to investigate the relationship between vagal tone level and event-related potentials (ERPs) in adults. Numerous studies have shown a relationship between vagal tone and the individual differences between a variety of psychophysiological, affective, and social outcomes. This suggests that vagal tone can be related to how people process relevant affective social information at the brain level. This study aimed to assess whether the ERP response varies between high and low vagal tone groups, in the face of salient affective information. In the experimental cohort, two groups were separated according to their vagal tone level. ERPs were recorded while individuals performed an affective picture task that included positive, neutral, and negative emotional stimuli. Differences between the high and low vagal tone groups were observed at the early posterior negativity for both positive and negative valences, and at the late positive potential for all the categories. It can be concluded that differences between high and low vagal tone levels are related to differences in the ERPs at early, middle, and late latencies. The results are discussed with respect to the effect of differences between the vagal tone conditions on various stages of information-processing.
Collapse
|
45
|
Barutta J, Gleichgerrcht E, Cornejo C, Ibáñez A. Neurodynamics of Mind: The Arrow Illusion of Conscious Intentionality as Downward Causation. Integr Psychol Behav Sci 2010; 44:127-43. [DOI: 10.1007/s12124-010-9117-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2009] [Accepted: 03/02/2010] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
46
|
Barutta J, Aravena P, Ibáñez A. The Machine Paradigm and Alternative Approaches in Cognitive Science. Integr Psychol Behav Sci 2010; 44:176-83. [DOI: 10.1007/s12124-010-9116-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|