1
|
Noschese EJ. It all made sense: ASL-first approach in classroom practice. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2024:enae045. [PMID: 39471359 DOI: 10.1093/jdsade/enae045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2024] [Revised: 09/10/2024] [Accepted: 10/08/2024] [Indexed: 11/01/2024]
Abstract
This qualitative study delves into the innovative Bilingual Grammar Curriculum. The curriculum aims to enhance bilingualism and linguistic skills among deaf students by integrating American Sign Language instruction and written language grammatical structures. The researcher conducted in-depth interviews with 12 experienced educators of the deaf who have implemented the curriculum in their teaching practices. Employing a thematic analysis approach, the researcher sought to uncover the educators' perceptions, experiences, and insights regarding the curriculum's effectiveness and its impact on bilingual language acquisition for deaf learners. Findings revealed a diverse range or perspectives, highlighting both the advantages and challenges of the curriculum's implementation. Themes emerged encompassing the curriculum's alignment with linguistic theories, its adaptability to diverse learning needs, the role of educator training, and the incorporation of cultural nuances within bilingual instruction. The study contributes valuable insights to the field of deaf education, shedding light on the intricate dynamics of bilingual grammar pedagogy and providing recommendations for curriculum refinement and educator professional development. Ultimately, this research underscores the significance of innovative approaches in enhancing the linguistic abilities and educational experiences of deaf and hard-of-hearing students within a bilingual framework.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Jo Noschese
- College of Education, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Pan Y, Xiao Y. Language and executive function in Mandarin-speaking deaf and hard-of-hearing children aged 3-5. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2024:enae037. [PMID: 39277795 DOI: 10.1093/jdsade/enae037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2023] [Revised: 08/09/2024] [Accepted: 08/27/2024] [Indexed: 09/17/2024]
Abstract
The study aimed to explore spoken language and executive function (EF) characteristics in 3-5-year-old prelingually deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) children, and evaluate the impact of demographic variables and EF on spoken language skills. 48 DHH children and 48 typically developing children who use auditory-oral communication were recruited. All participants underwent EF tests, including auditory working memory (WM), inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and the EF performance reported by parents. Using Mandarin Clinical Evaluation of Language for Preschoolers (MCELP), vocabulary comprehension, sentence comprehension, vocabulary naming, sentence structure imitation, and story narration were evaluated only in the DHH group, and their results were compared with the typical developmental level provided by MCELP. Results showed that DHH children exhibit deficiencies in different spoken language domains and EF components. While the spoken language skills of DHH children tend to improve as they age, a growing proportion of individuals fail to reach the typical developmental level. The spoken language ability in DHH children was positively correlated with age and EFs, and negatively correlated with aided hearing threshold, while auditory WM could positively predict their spoken language performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuchen Pan
- School of Chinese Language and Culture, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, Jiangsu, China
| | - Yongtao Xiao
- School of Medical Technology and Information Engineering, Zhejiang Chinese Medical University, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sun L, Dong Q, Du X, Wei D. Are Deaf College Students More Sensitive to Unfair Information? Evidence from an ERP Study. Brain Sci 2024; 14:788. [PMID: 39199481 PMCID: PMC11352934 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14080788] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2024] [Revised: 07/28/2024] [Accepted: 07/29/2024] [Indexed: 09/01/2024] Open
Abstract
To better understand the individual differences in fairness, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to explore the fairness characteristics of deaf college students through the ultimatum game task. Behaviorally, the significant main effect of the proposal type was found, which meant both deaf and hearing college students showed a lower acceptance rate for the more unfair proposal. Interestingly, we found a significant interaction between group and proposal type in the early stage (N1). Moreover, in the deaf college group, N1 (induced by moderately and very unfair proposals) was significantly larger than that of fair proposals. However, we found that deaf college students had smaller amplitudes on P2 and P3 than hearing college students. These results suggested that deaf college students might pursue more equity strongly so they are more sensitive to unfair information in the early stage. In a word, we should provide more fair allocations for deaf college students in our harmonious society.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Le Sun
- College of Educational Science, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing 401331, China; (L.S.); (Q.D.)
| | - Qi Dong
- College of Educational Science, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing 401331, China; (L.S.); (Q.D.)
| | - Xue Du
- College of Educational Science, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing 401331, China; (L.S.); (Q.D.)
| | - Dongtao Wei
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kotowicz J, Banaszkiewicz A, Dzięgiel-Fivet G, Emmorey K, Marchewka A, Jednoróg K. Neural underpinnings of sentence reading in deaf, native sign language users. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2024; 255:105447. [PMID: 39079468 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2024.105447] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2023] [Revised: 05/29/2024] [Accepted: 07/17/2024] [Indexed: 08/11/2024]
Abstract
The goal of this study was to investigate sentence-level reading circuits in deaf native signers, a unique group of deaf people who are immersed in a fully accessible linguistic environment from birth, and hearing readers. Task-based fMRI, functional connectivity and lateralization analyses were conducted. Both groups exhibited overlapping brain activity in the left-hemispheric perisylvian regions in response to a semantic sentence task. We found increased activity in left occipitotemporal and right frontal and temporal regions in deaf readers. Lateralization analyses did not confirm more rightward asymmetry in deaf individuals. Deaf readers exhibited weaker functional connectivity between inferior frontal and middle temporal gyri and enhanced coupling between temporal and insular cortex. In conclusion, despite the shared functional activity within the semantic reading network across both groups, our results suggest greater reliance on cognitive control processes for deaf readers, possibly resulting in greater effort required to perform the task in this group.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Anna Banaszkiewicz
- Laboratory of Brain Imaging, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland.
| | - Gabriela Dzięgiel-Fivet
- Laboratory of Language Neurobiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Karen Emmorey
- Laboratory for Language and Cognitive Neuroscience, San Diego State University, San Diego, USA
| | - Artur Marchewka
- Laboratory of Brain Imaging, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Katarzyna Jednoróg
- Laboratory of Language Neurobiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Izquierdo-Condoy JS, Sánchez Abadiano LE, Sánchez W, Rodríguez I, De La Cruz Matías K, Paz C, Ortiz-Prado E. Exploring healthcare barriers and satisfaction levels among deaf individuals in Ecuador: A video-based survey approach. Disabil Health J 2024; 17:101622. [PMID: 38580501 DOI: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2024.101622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2023] [Revised: 03/15/2024] [Accepted: 03/27/2024] [Indexed: 04/07/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Approximately 80% of Deaf individuals live in low- and middle-income countries, where health systems often overlook their specific needs. This communication gap can result in misdiagnosis and inappropriate treatment, impacting their overall satisfaction with healthcare services. OBJECTIVES This study aims to uncover barriers to healthcare access and preferences among the Deaf population in Ecuador, and the role of communication barriers in shaping satisfaction levels with healthcare services. METHODS The study gathered data from 386 participants through online surveys, focusing on demographic characteristics, healthcare experiences, communication methods, and levels of satisfaction. Descriptive statistics and association analyses were employed to analyze the data. RESULTS The study reveals that nearly all participants possessed an officially recognized disability (95.9%) and 53.9% reported hereditary deafness. Ecuadorian sign language was predominant (60.9%). Communication barriers were evident, with 65.0% having trouble understanding medical instructions, and 66.6% identifying a deficiency of tools for Deaf individuals in healthcare settings. Satisfaction levels were associated with the presence of interpreters during medical care, with 46.6% expressing a preference for interpreters over other communication methods. Additionally, private healthcare facilities were perceived as providing better services, despite being less frequently accessed (38.9%). Dissatisfaction was evident, particularly in aspects of communication and physician courtesy. CONCLUSIONS This study underscores the importance of tailoring healthcare services to address the unique needs of the Deaf population. Communication barriers emerged as a central obstacle, necessitating the inclusion of sign language interpreters and improvement of resources. The study's implications extend to healthcare equity in developing nations, emphasizing the significance of patient-centered care and inclusive healthcare practices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Wilson Sánchez
- Facultad de Ciencias Médicas, Universidad Central del Ecuador, Quito, 170136, Ecuador
| | - Ivonne Rodríguez
- Facultad de Ciencias Médicas, Universidad Central del Ecuador, Quito, 170136, Ecuador
| | | | - Clara Paz
- Grupo de Investigación Bienestar, Salud y Sociedad, Universidad de las Américas, Quito, 170137, Ecuador
| | - Esteban Ortiz-Prado
- One Health Research Group, Universidad de las Américas, Quito, 170137, Ecuador.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Ocuto OL. Deaf children, home language environments, and reciprocal-contingent family interactions. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2024; 29:322-334. [PMID: 38159302 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enad063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2023] [Revised: 10/24/2023] [Accepted: 12/06/2023] [Indexed: 01/03/2024]
Abstract
Engaged communication between mother and a child in their early developmental stages is one of the predictors of children's development of higher-order thinking skills. For deaf children, this engaged communication between mother and child hinges on the home language environment (HLE) being fully accessible to the child. This research uses agogical phenomenology in exploring the lived experiences of participants' HLE where sign language is used, with particular focus on the opportunities for extended discourse. Data were collected via semistructured interviews with the deaf children and their parents and observations in the HLEs of five signing families with at least one deaf child in the southwestern United States. The aim of this study was to document and provide insights into how language use in deaf children's HLE can impact their knowledge development; these insights uncovered the essence of reciprocal and contingent family interactions as a central aspect of the deaf child's HLE. It is hoped that the qualitative phenomenological findings will frame subsequent quantitative investigations of the variability in language access to home language components.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Oscar L Ocuto
- Department of Education, Gallaudet University, 800 Florida Avenue NE, Washington, DC, United States
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Curtin M, Cruice M, Morgan G, Herman R. Assessing parent-child interaction with deaf and hard of hearing infants aged 0-3 years: An international multi-professional e-Delphi. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0301722. [PMID: 38683866 PMCID: PMC11057743 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0301722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2023] [Accepted: 03/21/2024] [Indexed: 05/02/2024] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Most deaf babies are born to hearing families who do not yet have the skills to communicate effectively with their child. Adaptations to communication are important because the quality of parent-child interaction (PCI) predicts how a deaf child develops language. Teachers of Deaf children and Speech and Language Therapists support families with communication in the home. Currently, there are no assessments that appraise how a parent interacts with their deaf baby. Previous research has identified which parent behaviours and approaches are used in PCI assessments in research and practice. The current paper forms consensus on the core content and best practices of a new PCI tool for deaf children aged 0-3 years. METHODS An international sample of expert academics and practitioners (n = 83) were recruited to take part in a two-round modified electronic Delphi study. Participants were presented with 69 statements focusing on (i) which parent behaviours were important in assessment (ii) the methods to be used in PCI assessment. Participants rated the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement on a five-point Likert scale and gave comments to support their response. Consensus was defined as >80% of participants rating the statement as a (4) 'highly important' or a (5) 'essential'. If consensus was not reached, participant comments were used to generate new statements which were rated in the second round. This project involved a patient and public involvement (PPI) group of hearing and deaf parents and professionals to design and guide the study. RESULTS Consensus was achieved on 52 statements and ranged from 80-99%. A further six statements were additionally included. Within the 58 statements included, 36 were parent behaviours which centred on the parent's observation of, and response to, their child's behaviour and/or language. The remaining 22 statements focused on methods used in the assessment such as parents having their PCI filmed, parents having the opportunity to review the video and assess themselves alongside a professional, and parents being involved in subsequent goal setting. CONCLUSIONS This e-Delphi presented the parent behaviours and methods of assessment to be included in a new PCI tool for deaf children. Future co-production work and acceptability and feasibility testing are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Curtin
- Speech and Language Therapy (Paediatrics, Community), Homerton Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
- Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Madeline Cruice
- Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Gary Morgan
- Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Psychology and Education Sciences, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Rosalind Herman
- Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Hall S, Ballard M. Deaf patients' preferred communication in clinical settings: implications for healthcare providers. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2024; 29:170-186. [PMID: 38160399 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enad061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2023] [Revised: 11/28/2023] [Accepted: 11/29/2023] [Indexed: 01/03/2024]
Abstract
Deaf patients who communicate in American Sign Language (ASL) experience communication challenges leading to medical errors, treatment delays, and health disparities. Research on Deaf patient communication preferences is sparse. Researchers conducted focus groups based on the Health Belief Model with culturally Deaf patients and interpreters. The ASL focus groups were interpreted and transcribed into written English, verified by a third-party interpreting agency, and uploaded into NVivo. Deductive coding was used to identify communication methods and inductive coding was used to identify themes within each. Writing back-and-forth introduced challenges related to English proficiency, medical terminology, poor penmanship, and tendencies of providers to abbreviate. Participants had various speechreading abilities and described challenges with mask mandates. Multiple issues were identified with family and friends as proxy interpreters, including a lack of training, confidentiality issues, emotional support, and patient autonomy. Video remote interpreter challenges included technical, environmental, and interpreter qualification concerns. Participants overwhelmingly preferred on-site interpreters for communication clarity. While there was a preference for direct care, many acknowledged this is not always feasible due to lack of providers fluent in ASL. Access to on-site interpreters is vital for many Deaf patients to provide full access to critical medical information. Budget allocation for on-call interpreters is important in emergency settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Hall
- Department of Public Health, Utah Valley University, Orem, UT, United States
| | - Michael Ballard
- Department of Languages and Culture, Utah Valley University, Orem, UT, United States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Curtin M, Wakefield T, Herman R, Morgan G, Cruice M. "It doesn't matter if we're the most amazing professionals in the world…" A qualitative study of professionals' perspectives on parent-child interaction assessment with deaf infants. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1315220. [PMID: 38500650 PMCID: PMC10944883 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1315220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/10/2023] [Accepted: 02/12/2024] [Indexed: 03/20/2024] Open
Abstract
Introduction Parent child interaction (PCI) is positively associated with deaf children's language development. However, there are no known, deaf-specific tools to observe how a parent interacts with their deaf child aged 0-3 years. Without a framework for professionals to use with families, it is unknown how professionals assess PCI, what they assess, why they assess, and how the assessment results relate to case management. Methods Eighteen hearing and deaf professionals, who work with deaf and hard of hearing infants aged 0-3 years and their families, attended online focus groups. The aim of the study was to gain insight into the professional assessment of PCI. Data were analyzed using thematic analysis. Findings Six themes were generated from the dataset. Professionals discussed how central parents were in the support offered to families in the home, the importance of knowing and understanding the individual family, and accounting for and supporting parental wellbeing. Descriptions on how to administer a best practice PCI assessment included which parent behaviors to assess and how to make adaptations for different populations. Professionals shared how the assessment and review process could be used to inform and upskill parents through video reflection and goal setting. Discussion This study provides insight into the mechanisms and motivations for professionals assessing the interactive behaviors of parents who have deaf children aged 0-3. Professionals acknowledged that family life is multi-faceted, and that support is most meaningful to families when professionals worked with these differences and incorporated them into assessment, goal setting, and intervention plans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Curtin
- Speech and Language Therapy, Homerton Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
- Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Tina Wakefield
- National Deaf Children’s Society, London, United Kingdom
| | - Rosalind Herman
- Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Gary Morgan
- Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Psychology and Education Sciences, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Madeline Cruice
- Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Delcenserie A, Genesee F, Champoux F. Exposure to sign language prior and after cochlear implantation increases language and cognitive skills in deaf children. Dev Sci 2024:e13481. [PMID: 38327110 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13481] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2023] [Revised: 11/06/2023] [Accepted: 11/18/2023] [Indexed: 02/09/2024]
Abstract
Recent evidence suggests that deaf children with CIs exposed to nonnative sign language from hearing parents can attain age-appropriate vocabularies in both sign and spoken language. It remains to be explored whether deaf children with CIs who are exposed to early nonnative sign language, but only up to implantation, also benefit from this input and whether these benefits also extend to memory abilities, which are strongly linked to language development. The present study examined the impact of deaf children's early short-term exposure to nonnative sign input on their spoken language and their phonological memory abilities. Deaf children who had been exposed to nonnative sign input before and after cochlear implantation were compared to deaf children who never had any exposure to sign input as well as to children with typical hearing. The children were between 5;1 and 7;1 years of age at the time of testing and were matched on age, sex, and socioeconomic status. The results suggest that even short-term exposure to nonnative sign input has positive effects on general language and phonological memory abilities as well as on nonverbal working memory-with total length of exposure to sign input being the best predictor of deaf children's performance on these measures. The present data suggest that even access to early short-term nonnative visual language input is beneficial for the language and phonological memory abilities of deaf children with cochlear implants, suggesting also that parents should not be discouraged from learning and exposing their child to sign language. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: This is the first study to examine the effects of early short-term exposure to nonnative sign input on French-speaking children with cochlear implants' spoken language and memory abilities. Early short-term nonnative exposure to sign input can have positive consequences for the language and phonological memory abilities of deaf children with CIs. Extended exposure to sign input has some additional and important benefits, allowing children to perform on par with children with typical hearing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Delcenserie
- Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada
- School of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, Universite de Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| | | | - F Champoux
- School of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, Universite de Montreal, Quebec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Lieberman AM, Mitchiner J, Pontecorvo E. Hearing parents learning American Sign Language with their deaf children: a mixed-methods survey. APPLIED LINGUISTICS REVIEW 2024; 15:309-333. [PMID: 38221976 PMCID: PMC10785677 DOI: 10.1515/applirev-2021-0120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2021] [Accepted: 04/27/2022] [Indexed: 01/16/2024]
Abstract
Hearing parents with deaf children face difficult decisions about what language(s) to use with their child. Sign languages such as American Sign Language (ASL) are fully accessible to deaf children, yet most hearing parents are not proficient in ASL prior to having a deaf child. Parents are often discouraged from learning ASL based in part on an assumption that it will be too difficult, yet there is little evidence supporting this claim. In this mixed-methods study, we surveyed hearing parents of deaf children (n = 100) who had learned ASL to learn more about their experiences. In their survey responses, parents identified a range of resources that supported their ASL learning as well as frequent barriers. Parents identified strongly with belief statements indicating the importance of ASL and affirmed that learning ASL is attainable for hearing parents. We discuss the implications of this study for parents who are considering ASL as a language choice and for the professionals who guide them.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amy M. Lieberman
- Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Julie Mitchiner
- Department of Education, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Elana Pontecorvo
- Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Bower C, Reilly BK, Richerson J, Hecht JL. Hearing Assessment in Infants, Children, and Adolescents: Recommendations Beyond Neonatal Screening. Pediatrics 2023; 152:e2023063288. [PMID: 37635686 DOI: 10.1542/peds.2023-063288] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/26/2023] [Indexed: 08/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Children who are deaf or hard of hearing (D/HH) are at high risk for permanent deficits in language acquisition and downstream effects such as poor academic performance, personal-social maladjustments, and emotional difficulties. Identification of children born D/HH through newborn hearing screening and subsequent timely early intervention can prevent or reduce many of these adverse consequences. Ongoing surveillance for changes in hearing thresholds after infancy is also important and should be accomplished by subjective assessment for signs of atypical hearing and with objective screening tests. Scheduled hearing screening may take place in the primary care setting, or via referral to an audiologist according to the Bright Futures/American Academy of Pediatrics "Recommendations for Preventive Pediatric Health Care" (also known as the periodicity schedule). This report covers hearing assessment beyond the newborn period, reviews risk factors for hearing level change, and provides guidance for providers of pediatric primary care on the assessment and care of children who are D/HH.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charles Bower
- Department of Otolaryngology, Arkansas Children's Hospital, Little Rock, Arkansas
| | - Brian Kip Reilly
- Division of Otolaryngology, Children's National Medical Center, Washington, District of Columbia
| | | | - Julia L Hecht
- Department of Pediatrics, University of New Mexico School of Medicine, Albuquerque, New Mexico
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Hall WC, Dye TDV, Siddiqi S. Associations of childhood hearing loss and adverse childhood experiences in deaf adults. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0287024. [PMID: 37343003 PMCID: PMC10284385 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0287024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2022] [Accepted: 05/30/2023] [Indexed: 06/23/2023] Open
Abstract
Childhood trauma and adverse childhood experiences have a strong relationship with health disparities across the lifespan. Despite experiencing approximately doubled rates of trauma, Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) are poorly characterized in deaf populations. We sought to characterize deaf-specific demographic factors and their association with multiple experiences of ACEs before the age of 18 years old. An analytical cross-sectional approach was used to ascertain associations of deaf-specific demographic factors and experiences with ACEs. The complete dataset included 520 participants for a total response rate of 56%. After adjusting for confounding effects, less severe hearing loss of 16-55 dB (2+ OR: 5.2, 4+ OR: 4.7), having a cochlear implant (2+ OR: 2.1, 4+ OR: 2.6), and not attending at least one school with signing access (2+ OR: 2.4, 4+ OR: 3.7) were significantly and independently associated with reported experiences of multiple ACEs. We conclude that factors associated with childhood hearing loss and language experiences increase risk of experiencing ACEs. Given the strong relationship between ACEs and poor social outcomes, early intervention clinical practice and health policies should consider interventions to support healthy home environments for deaf children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wyatte C. Hall
- Public Health Sciences, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Pediatrics, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Neurology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Center for Community Health and Prevention, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| | - Timothy D. V. Dye
- Public Health Sciences, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Pediatrics, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| | - Shazia Siddiqi
- Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Development of visual sustained selective attention and response inhibition in deaf children. Mem Cognit 2023; 51:509-525. [PMID: 35794408 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-022-01330-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Studies of deaf and hard-of-hearing (henceforth, deaf) children tend to make comparisons with typically hearing children for the purpose of either identifying deficits to be remediated or understanding the impact of auditory deprivation on visual or domain general processing. Here, we eschew these clinical and theoretical aims, seeking instead to understand factors that explain variability in cognitive function within deaf children. A total of 108 bilingual deaf children ages 7-13 years who use both English and American Sign Language (ASL) participated in a longitudinal study of executive function (EF) development. We report longitudinal data from a visual continuous performance task that measured sustained selective attention and response inhibition. Results show that the impact of deafness on these processes is negligible, but that language skills have a positive relationship with both: better English abilities were associated with better selective sustained attention, and better ASL abilities with better response inhibition. The relationship between sustained selective attention and English abilities may reflect the cognitive demands of spoken language acquisition for deaf children, whereas better ASL abilities may promote an "inner voice," associated with improved response inhibition. The current study cannot conclusively demonstrate causality or directionality of effects. However, these data highlight the importance of studies that focus on atypical individuals, for whom the relationships between language and cognition may be different from those observed in typically developing populations.
Collapse
|
15
|
Humphries T, Mathur G, Napoli DJ, Padden C, Rathmann C. Deaf Children Need Rich Language Input from the Start: Support in Advising Parents. CHILDREN (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2022; 9:1609. [PMID: 36360337 PMCID: PMC9688581 DOI: 10.3390/children9111609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2022] [Revised: 10/13/2022] [Accepted: 10/19/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Bilingual bimodalism is a great benefit to deaf children at home and in schooling. Deaf signing children perform better overall than non-signing deaf children, regardless of whether they use a cochlear implant. Raising a deaf child in a speech-only environment can carry cognitive and psycho-social risks that may have lifelong adverse effects. For children born deaf, or who become deaf in early childhood, we recommend comprehensible multimodal language exposure and engagement in joint activity with parents and friends to assure age-appropriate first-language acquisition. Accessible visual language input should begin as close to birth as possible. Hearing parents will need timely and extensive support; thus, we propose that, upon the birth of a deaf child and through the preschool years, among other things, the family needs an adult deaf presence in the home for several hours every day to be a linguistic model, to guide the family in taking sign language lessons, to show the family how to make spoken language accessible to their deaf child, and to be an encouraging liaison to deaf communities. While such a support program will be complicated and challenging to implement, it is far less costly than the harm of linguistic deprivation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tom Humphries
- Department of Communication, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | - Gaurav Mathur
- Department of Linguistics, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC 20002, USA
| | - Donna Jo Napoli
- Department of Linguistics, Swarthmore College, Swarthmore, PA 19081, USA
| | - Carol Padden
- Division of Social Sciences, Department of Communication and Dean, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | - Christian Rathmann
- Department of Deaf Studies and Sign Language Interpreting, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10019 Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Villwock A, Grin K. Somatosensory processing in deaf and deafblind individuals: How does the brain adapt as a function of sensory and linguistic experience? A critical review. Front Psychol 2022; 13:938842. [PMID: 36324786 PMCID: PMC9618853 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.938842] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2022] [Accepted: 09/22/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
How do deaf and deafblind individuals process touch? This question offers a unique model to understand the prospects and constraints of neural plasticity. Our brain constantly receives and processes signals from the environment and combines them into the most reliable information content. The nervous system adapts its functional and structural organization according to the input, and perceptual processing develops as a function of individual experience. However, there are still many unresolved questions regarding the deciding factors for these changes in deaf and deafblind individuals, and so far, findings are not consistent. To date, most studies have not taken the sensory and linguistic experiences of the included participants into account. As a result, the impact of sensory deprivation vs. language experience on somatosensory processing remains inconclusive. Even less is known about the impact of deafblindness on brain development. The resulting neural adaptations could be even more substantial, but no clear patterns have yet been identified. How do deafblind individuals process sensory input? Studies on deafblindness have mostly focused on single cases or groups of late-blind individuals. Importantly, the language backgrounds of deafblind communities are highly variable and include the usage of tactile languages. So far, this kind of linguistic experience and its consequences have not been considered in studies on basic perceptual functions. Here, we will provide a critical review of the literature, aiming at identifying determinants for neuroplasticity and gaps in our current knowledge of somatosensory processing in deaf and deafblind individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agnes Villwock
- Sign Languages, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Bosworth RG, Hwang SO, Corina DP. Visual attention for linguistic and non-linguistic body actions in non-signing and native signing children. Front Psychol 2022; 13:951057. [PMID: 36160576 PMCID: PMC9505519 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.951057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2022] [Accepted: 07/25/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Evidence from adult studies of deaf signers supports the dissociation between neural systems involved in processing visual linguistic and non-linguistic body actions. The question of how and when this specialization arises is poorly understood. Visual attention to these forms is likely to change with age and be affected by prior language experience. The present study used eye-tracking methodology with infants and children as they freely viewed alternating video sequences of lexical American sign language (ASL) signs and non-linguistic body actions (self-directed grooming action and object-directed pantomime). In Experiment 1, we quantified fixation patterns using an area of interest (AOI) approach and calculated face preference index (FPI) values to assess the developmental differences between 6 and 11-month-old hearing infants. Both groups were from monolingual English-speaking homes with no prior exposure to sign language. Six-month-olds attended the signer's face for grooming; but for mimes and signs, they were drawn to attend to the "articulatory space" where the hands and arms primarily fall. Eleven-month-olds, on the other hand, showed a similar attention to the face for all body action types. We interpret this to reflect an early visual language sensitivity that diminishes with age, just before the child's first birthday. In Experiment 2, we contrasted 18 hearing monolingual English-speaking children (mean age of 4.8 years) vs. 13 hearing children of deaf adults (CODAs; mean age of 5.7 years) whose primary language at home was ASL. Native signing children had a significantly greater face attentional bias than non-signing children for ASL signs, but not for grooming and mimes. The differences in the visual attention patterns that are contingent on age (in infants) and language experience (in children) may be related to both linguistic specialization over time and the emerging awareness of communicative gestural acts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rain G. Bosworth
- NTID PLAY Lab, National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY, United States
| | - So One Hwang
- Center for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States
| | - David P. Corina
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Gutierrez-Sigut E, Lamarche VM, Rowley K, Lago EF, Pardo-Guijarro MJ, Saenz I, Frigola B, Frigola S, Aliaga D, Goldberg L. How do face masks impact communication amongst deaf/HoH people? Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:81. [PMID: 36063244 PMCID: PMC9443624 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00431-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2021] [Accepted: 08/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Face coverings have been key in reducing the spread of COVID-19. At the same time, they have hindered interpersonal communication, particularly for those who rely on speechreading to aid communication. The available research indicated that deaf/hard of hearing (HoH) people experienced great difficulty communicating with people wearing masks and negative effects on wellbeing. Here we extended these findings by exploring which factors predict deaf/HoH people’s communication difficulties, loss of information, and wellbeing. We also explored the factors predicting perceived usefulness of transparent face coverings and alternative ways of communicating. We report the findings from an accessible survey study, released in two written and three signed languages. Responses from 395 deaf/HoH UK and Spanish residents were collected online at a time when masks were mandatory. We investigated whether onset and level of deafness, knowledge of sign language, speechreading fluency, and country of residence predicted communication difficulties, wellbeing, and degree to which transparent face coverings were considered useful. Overall, deaf/HoH people and their relatives used masks most of the time despite greater communication difficulties. Late-onset deaf people were the group that experienced more difficulties in communication, and also reported lower wellbeing. However, both early- and late-onset deaf people reported missing more information and feeling more disconnected from society than HoH people. Finally, signers valued transparent face shields more positively than non-signers. The latter suggests that, while seeing the lips is positive to everyone, signers appreciate seeing the whole facial expression. Importantly, our data also revealed the importance of visual communication other than speechreading to facilitate face-to-face interactions. Late-onset deaf people experienced more difficulties in communication and low wellbeing. Severely/profoundly deaf people missed more information and felt disconnected from society. Signers preferred completely transparent face coverings. More frequent use of masks doesn’t necessarily imply more difficulty communicating. Visual communication, pro-social behaviour, and societal structure might help easing communication.
Collapse
|
19
|
Clark MD, Greene-Woods A, Alofi A, Sides M, Buchanan B, Hauschildt S, Alford A, Courson F, Venable T. The Spoken Language Checklist: A User-Friendly Normed Language Acquisition Checklist. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2021; 26:251-262. [PMID: 33555011 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enaa043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2020] [Revised: 12/03/2020] [Accepted: 12/13/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
There are many variables having an impact on the spoken language acquisition of deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) children; therefore, it is critical for parents and professionals to have appropriate tools to monitor language acquisition. The Spoken Language Checklist (SLC) was developed to monitor and identify developmental milestones in a user-friendly checklist format that includes norms. The availability of the SLC will help parents and professionals to monitor the spoken language development of DHH children and provide interventions that should any delays be observed. Recognizing these delays early could prevent any insurmountable effects for cognitive development and further language development.
Collapse
|
20
|
Bosworth RG, Stone A. Rapid development of perceptual gaze control in hearing native signing Infants and children. Dev Sci 2021; 24:e13086. [PMID: 33484575 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13086] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2020] [Revised: 11/23/2020] [Accepted: 01/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Children's gaze behavior reflects emergent linguistic knowledge and real-time language processing of speech, but little is known about naturalistic gaze behaviors while watching signed narratives. Measuring gaze patterns in signing children could uncover how they master perceptual gaze control during a time of active language learning. Gaze patterns were recorded using a Tobii X120 eye tracker, in 31 non-signing and 30 signing hearing infants (5-14 months) and children (2-8 years) as they watched signed narratives on video. Intelligibility of the signed narratives was manipulated by presenting them naturally and in video-reversed ("low intelligibility") conditions. This video manipulation was used because it distorts semantic content, while preserving most surface phonological features. We examined where participants looked, using linear mixed models with Language Group (non-signing vs. signing) and Video Condition (Forward vs. Reversed), controlling for trial order. Non-signing infants and children showed a preference to look at the face as well as areas below the face, possibly because their gaze was drawn to the moving articulators in signing space. Native signing infants and children demonstrated resilient, face-focused gaze behavior. Moreover, their gaze behavior was unchanged for video-reversed signed narratives, similar to what was seen for adult native signers, possibly because they already have efficient highly focused gaze behavior. The present study demonstrates that human perceptual gaze control is sensitive to visual language experience over the first year of life and emerges early, by 6 months of age. Results have implications for the critical importance of early visual language exposure for deaf infants. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2ahWUluFAAg.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rain G Bosworth
- National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY, USA
| | - Adam Stone
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Manhardt F, Brouwer S, Özyürek A. A Tale of Two Modalities: Sign and Speech Influence Each Other in Bimodal Bilinguals. Psychol Sci 2021; 32:424-436. [PMID: 33621474 DOI: 10.1177/0956797620968789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Bimodal bilinguals are hearing individuals fluent in a sign and a spoken language. Can the two languages influence each other in such individuals despite differences in the visual (sign) and vocal (speech) modalities of expression? We investigated cross-linguistic influences on bimodal bilinguals' expression of spatial relations. Unlike spoken languages, sign uses iconic linguistic forms that resemble physical features of objects in a spatial relation and thus expresses specific semantic information. Hearing bimodal bilinguals (n = 21) fluent in Dutch and Sign Language of the Netherlands and their hearing nonsigning and deaf signing peers (n = 20 each) described left/right relations between two objects. Bimodal bilinguals expressed more specific information about physical features of objects in speech than nonsigners, showing influence from sign language. They also used fewer iconic signs with specific semantic information than deaf signers, demonstrating influence from speech. Bimodal bilinguals' speech and signs are shaped by two languages from different modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Aslı Özyürek
- Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University.,Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,Donders Center for Cognition, Radboud University
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Emmorey K, Lee B. The neurocognitive basis of skilled reading in prelingually and profoundly deaf adults. LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTICS COMPASS 2021; 15:e12407. [PMID: 34306178 PMCID: PMC8302003 DOI: 10.1111/lnc3.12407] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2020] [Accepted: 02/03/2021] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
Deaf individuals have unique sensory and linguistic experiences that influence how they read and become skilled readers. This review presents our current understanding of the neurocognitive underpinnings of reading skill in deaf adults. Key behavioural and neuroimaging studies are integrated to build a profile of skilled adult deaf readers and to examine how changes in visual attention and reduced access to auditory input and phonology shape how they read both words and sentences. Crucially, the behaviours, processes, and neural circuity of deaf readers are compared to those of hearing readers with similar reading ability to help identify alternative pathways to reading success. Overall, sensitivity to orthographic and semantic information is comparable for skilled deaf and hearing readers, but deaf readers rely less on phonology and show greater engagement of the right hemisphere in visual word processing. During sentence reading, deaf readers process visual word forms more efficiently and may have a greater reliance on and altered connectivity to semantic information compared to their hearing peers. These findings highlight the plasticity of the reading system and point to alternative pathways to reading success.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen Emmorey
- School of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA
- Joint Doctoral Program in Language and Communicative Disorders, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
| | - Brittany Lee
- School of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA
- Joint Doctoral Program in Language and Communicative Disorders, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Kushalnagar P, Ryan C, Paludneviciene R, Spellun A, Gulati S. Adverse Childhood Communication Experiences Associated With an Increased Risk of Chronic Diseases in Adults Who Are Deaf. Am J Prev Med 2020; 59:548-554. [PMID: 32636047 PMCID: PMC7508773 DOI: 10.1016/j.amepre.2020.04.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2019] [Revised: 02/15/2020] [Accepted: 04/12/2020] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION This study explores adverse childhood communication experiences and its RRs for acquiring specific chronic diseases and mental health disorders in adults who are deaf and hard of hearing. METHODS A cross-sectional design with snowball sampling was used to recruit adults who were deaf and hard of hearing and were born or became deaf in both ears before age 13 years. Patient-reported outcomes surveys in American Sign Language and English were disseminated to collect data about early life communication experiences with caregivers. Modified Poisson regression with robust SEs was used to calculate RR estimates and 95% CIs for all medical conditions with early life communication experiences as main predictors. RESULTS Data collection occurred from May 2016 to July 2016, October 2016 to April 2018, and October 2018 to May 2019. The U.S. sample consisted of 1,524 adults who were born or became deaf early. After adjusting for parental hearing status and known correlates of medical conditions, poorer direct child-caregiver communication was significantly associated with an increased risk of being diagnosed with diabetes (RRR=1.12, 95% CI=1.01, 1.24), hypertension (RRR=1.10, 95% CI=1.03, 1.17), and heart disease (RRR=1.61, 95% CI=1.39, 1.87). Poor indirect family communication/inclusion increased risks for lung diseases (RRR=1.19, 95% CI=1.07, 1.33) and depression/anxiety disorders (RRR=1.34, 95% CI=1.24, 1.44). The absolute risk increase and number needed to harm are also reported. CONCLUSIONS Outcomes data reported by patients who were deaf and hard of hearing demonstrated that poorer direct child-caregiver communication and ongoing exclusion from incidental family communication were associated with increased risks for multiple chronic health outcomes. Practices should consider developing and utilizing an adverse childhood communication screening measure to prevent or remediate language deprivation and communication neglect in pediatric patients who were deaf and hard of hearing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Poorna Kushalnagar
- Department of Psychology, Gallaudet University, Washington, District of Columbia.
| | - Claire Ryan
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| | | | - Arielle Spellun
- Department of Pediatrics, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut
| | - Sanjay Gulati
- UMass/Boston Children's Hospital, Waltham, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Clark MD, Cue KR, Delgado NJ, Greene-Woods AN, Wolsey JLA. Early Intervention Protocols: Proposing a Default Bimodal Bilingual Approach for Deaf Children. Matern Child Health J 2020; 24:1339-1344. [PMID: 32897446 PMCID: PMC7477485 DOI: 10.1007/s10995-020-03005-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/29/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Despite advances in hearing technology, a growing body of research, as well as early intervention protocols, deaf children largely fail to meet age-based language milestones. This gap in language acquisition points to the inconsistencies that exist between research and practice. Current research suggests that bimodal bilingual early interventions at deaf identification provide children language foundations that can lead to more effective outcomes. Recommendations that support implementing bimodal bilingualism at deaf identification include early intervention protocols, language foundations, and the development of appropriate bimodal bilingual environments. All recommendations serve as multifaceted tools in a deaf child’s repertoire as language and modality preferences develop and solidify. This versatile approach allows for children to determine their own language and communication preferences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Diane Clark
- Department of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, Lamar University, 4400 MLK Blvd., P. O. Box 10113, Beaumont, TX, 77710, USA
| | - Katrina R Cue
- Department of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, Lamar University, 4400 MLK Blvd., P. O. Box 10113, Beaumont, TX, 77710, USA.
| | - Natalie J Delgado
- Department of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, Lamar University, 4400 MLK Blvd., P. O. Box 10113, Beaumont, TX, 77710, USA
| | - Ashley N Greene-Woods
- Department of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, Lamar University, 4400 MLK Blvd., P. O. Box 10113, Beaumont, TX, 77710, USA
| | - Ju-Lee A Wolsey
- Deaf Studies, Interdisciplinary Programs, Towson University, 8000 York Road, Towson, MD, 21252, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Hecht JL. Responsibility in the Current Epidemic of Language Deprivation (1990-Present). Matern Child Health J 2020; 24:1319-1322. [PMID: 32761503 DOI: 10.1007/s10995-020-02989-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Language deprivation syndrome (LDS) is a permanent and preventable disability affecting language, cognition, and behavior that is epidemic in the deaf population. Since 1990, systemic and technological changes in the health care of deaf children have created a new paradigm that perpetuates this crisis. Physicians and other professionals have been largely unaware of their roles in this recent epidemic. An evidenced based system of care that addresses the causes and potential solutions can reverse this trend and prevent early language deprivation in deaf children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia L Hecht
- Department of Pediatrics, University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center, 2211 Lomas Blvd, NE, Albuquerque, NM, 87106, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Caselli NK, Hall WC, Henner J. American Sign Language Interpreters in Public Schools: An Illusion of Inclusion that Perpetuates Language Deprivation. Matern Child Health J 2020; 24:1323-1329. [DOI: 10.1007/s10995-020-02975-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
27
|
How Bilingualism Contributes to Healthy Development in Deaf Children: A Public Health Perspective. Matern Child Health J 2020; 24:1330-1338. [PMID: 32632844 DOI: 10.1007/s10995-020-02976-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
The aim of this article is to increase awareness of language practices in the deaf community that affect communication needs and health outcomes, focusing particularly on the prevalence of bilingualism among deaf adults. Language deprivation and poor health outcomes in the deaf population are risks that cannot be addressed solely by hearing intervention. We propose that bilingualism acts as a protective measure to minimize the health risks faced by deaf individuals. Provision of culturally and linguistically appropriate services to deaf stakeholders, and particularly hearing families of deaf children, requires familiarity with the developmental and social ramifications of bilingualism.
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
Here, a moral case is presented as to why sign languages such as Auslan should be made compulsory in general school curricula. Firstly, there are significant benefits that accrue to individuals from learning sign language. Secondly, sign language education is a matter of justice; the normalisation of sign language education and use would particularly benefit marginalised groups, such as those living with a communication disability. Finally, the integration of sign languages into the curricula would enable the flourishing of Deaf culture and go some way to resolving the tensions that have arisen from the promotion of oralist education facilitated by technologies such as cochlear implants. There are important reasons to further pursue policy proposals regarding the prioritisation of sign language in school curricula.
Collapse
|
29
|
Zarowski A, Molisz A, De Coninck L, Vermeiren A, Theunen T, Theuwis L, Przewoźny T, Siebert J, Offeciers FE. Influence of the pre- or postlingual status of cochlear implant recipients on behavioural T/C-levels. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2020; 131:109867. [PMID: 31999994 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2020.109867] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2019] [Revised: 01/05/2020] [Accepted: 01/05/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Previous auditory experience modifies the sensitivity of the auditory cortex to the afferent activity of the auditory pathways and may influence the threshold (T) and comfort (C) levels in patients receiving a cochlear implant (CI). Literature data on this particular topic is very scarce. OBJECTIVE This study aimed to evaluate the differences in T/C-levels between pre- and postlingually implanted cochlear implant patients. METHODS Retrospective case review in a quaternary otologic referral centre was performed. Data on the T/C-levels have been collected in 90 consecutive CI patients divided into 2 groups. Group 1 comprised 16 prelingually deaf children implanted between 8 months and 10 years of age. Group 2 comprised 74 postlingually deaf adults (average age of 62 years). All patients were users of the Nucleus 24RECA (Freedom, Contour Advance-of-Stylet electrode) cochlear implant. All measurements were performed at the fifth implant programming session at 4-6 months after surgery, when stable T/C thresholds have already been obtained. RESULTS The behavioural C-levels present important and statistically significant differences between the pre- and postlingually implanted patients for all electrode contacts that could reach 30 CL. For the T-levels the observed differences were smaller and statistically insignificant for most electrode contacts. CONCLUSIONS The previous auditory experience (pre- or postlingual deafness) seems to be an independent parameter influencing the T/C-levels in patients receiving a CI. Together with the electrode contact impedance and the contact position in the electrode array it can explain up to 37% of the variability in the definition of the C-levels. The fact that the stabilised C-levels measured 4-6 months postoperatively can be up to 30 CL higher in the prelingually deaf patients than in the postlingual ones results also in a much higher dynamic range observed in prelingual subjects. Therefore implant programming of the prelingual patients should be very cautious in order to avoid the risk of overstimulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Zarowski
- European Institute for Otorhinolaryngology, Sint-Augustinus Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium.
| | - A Molisz
- Department of Family Medicine, Medical University of Gdansk, Poland
| | - L De Coninck
- European Institute for Otorhinolaryngology, Sint-Augustinus Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - A Vermeiren
- European Institute for Otorhinolaryngology, Sint-Augustinus Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - T Theunen
- European Institute for Otorhinolaryngology, Sint-Augustinus Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - L Theuwis
- European Institute for Otorhinolaryngology, Sint-Augustinus Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - T Przewoźny
- Department of Otolaryngology, Medical University of Gdansk, Poland
| | - J Siebert
- Department of Family Medicine, Medical University of Gdansk, Poland
| | - F E Offeciers
- European Institute for Otorhinolaryngology, Sint-Augustinus Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Neild R, Diane Clark M. Assessment in deaf education: Perspectives and experiences linking it all together. PSYCHOLOGY IN THE SCHOOLS 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/pits.22337] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Raschelle Neild
- Department of Special EducationBall State University Muncie Indiana
| | - M. Diane Clark
- Department of Deaf Studies and Deaf EducationLamar University Beaumont Texas
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Brooks R, Singleton JL, Meltzoff AN. Enhanced gaze-following behavior in Deaf infants of Deaf parents. Dev Sci 2019; 23:e12900. [PMID: 31486168 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12900] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2019] [Revised: 08/04/2019] [Accepted: 08/30/2019] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
Gaze following plays a role in parent-infant communication and is a key mechanism by which infants acquire information about the world from social input. Gaze following in Deaf infants has been understudied. Twelve Deaf infants of Deaf parents (DoD) who had native exposure to American Sign Language (ASL) were gender-matched and age-matched (±7 days) to 60 spoken-language hearing control infants. Results showed that the DoD infants had significantly higher gaze-following scores than the hearing infants. We hypothesize that in the absence of auditory input, and with support from ASL-fluent Deaf parents, infants become attuned to visual-communicative signals from other people, which engenders increased gaze following. These findings underscore the need to revise the 'deficit model' of deafness. Deaf infants immersed in natural sign language from birth are better at understanding the signals and identifying the referential meaning of adults' gaze behavior compared to hearing infants not exposed to sign language. Broader implications for theories of social-cognitive development are discussed. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/QXCDK_CUmAI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rechele Brooks
- Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
| | - Jenny L Singleton
- Department of Linguistics, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas
| | - Andrew N Meltzoff
- Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Lau THM, Lee KYS, Lam EYC, Lam JHS, Yiu CKM, Tang GWL. Oral Language Performance of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Students in Mainstream Schools. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2019; 24:448-458. [PMID: 31220286 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enz012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2018] [Revised: 03/19/2019] [Accepted: 03/22/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
In Hong Kong, students are expected to speak fluent Cantonese, Putonghua, and English. However, the curriculum does not include Cantonese studies, as children are expected to have already acquired Cantonese by the age of school entry. This study examined the language outcomes of Cantonese-speaking deaf or hard-of-hearing children who attend primary schools within the Hong Kong educational system and considered whether the system currently meets the needs of these children. The Hong Kong Cantonese Oral Language Assessment Scale, which comprises six subtests, was used to assess 98 children with mild to profound hearing loss. A regression analysis was used to examine the influences of various variables on oral language performance in these children. Notably, 41% of the participants had achieved age-appropriate oral language skills, while 18% and 41% exhibited mild-to-moderate or severe oral language impairment, respectively. The degree of hearing loss and the use of speech therapy were identified as significant negative predictors of oral language performance. The issues of a relatively late diagnosis and device fitting, as well as the very poor oral language outcomes, strongly emphasize the need for policy makers to reconsider the existing educational approaches and support for deaf or hard-of-hearing children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tammy H M Lau
- The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
| | - Kathy Y S Lee
- The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
| | - Emily Y C Lam
- Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
| | - Joffee H S Lam
- The Hong Kong Institute of Education, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
| | - Chris K M Yiu
- Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
| | - Gladys W L Tang
- Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Toe D, Paatsch L, Szarkowski A. Assessing Pragmatic Skills Using Checklists with Children who are Deaf and Hard of Hearing: A Systematic Review. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2019; 24:189-200. [PMID: 30929005 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enz004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2018] [Revised: 01/28/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
This paper investigates the use of checklists to assess pragmatics in children and adolescents who are deaf and hard of hearing. A systematic literature review was undertaken to identify all of the published research articles between 1979 and 2018 on the topic of the assessment of pragmatics for this population of children and adolescents. The 67 papers identified in this review were analyzed and all papers that utilized a checklist to assess pragmatic skills were identified. Across the 18 different published papers on the use of pragmatic skills among children who are deaf and hard of hearing, nine checklists were identified. These nine checklists were then compared and contrasted on six key features including identification of a theoretical framework or model; the type of pragmatic skills measured; the age range of the child assessed; the information/outputs generated; the primary informant for the assessment; and reliability, validity, and normative data. The resulting analysis provides a comprehensive guide to aid clinicians, educators, and researchers in selecting an appropriate checklist to assess pragmatic skills for children and adolescents who are deaf and hard of hearing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Amy Szarkowski
- Children's Center for Communication/Beverly School for the Deaf, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Support for parents of deaf children: Common questions and informed, evidence-based answers. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2019; 118:134-142. [PMID: 30623850 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2018.12.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2018] [Revised: 11/15/2018] [Accepted: 12/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
To assist medical and hearing-science professionals in supporting parents of deaf children, we have identified common questions that parents may have and provide evidence-based answers. In doing so, a compassionate and positive narrative about deafness and deaf children is offered, one that relies on recent research evidence regarding the critical nature of early exposure to a fully accessible visual language, which in the United States is American Sign Language (ASL). This evidence includes the role of sign language in language acquisition, cognitive development, and literacy. In order for parents to provide a nurturing and anxiety-free environment for early childhood development, signing at home is important even if their child also has the additional nurturing and care of a signing community. It is not just the early years of a child's life that matter for language acquisition; it's the early months, the early weeks, even the early days. Deaf children cannot wait for accessible language input. The whole family must learn simultaneously as the deaf child learns. Even moderate fluency on the part of the family benefits the child enormously. And learning the sign language together can be one of the strongest bonding experiences that the family and deaf child have.
Collapse
|
35
|
Henner J, Novogrodsky R, Reis J, Hoffmeister R. Recent Issues in the Use of Signed Language Assessments for Diagnosis of Language Disorders in Signing Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2018; 23:307-316. [PMID: 29767737 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/eny014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2018] [Accepted: 04/11/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
In recent years, normed signed language assessments have become a useful tool for researchers, practitioners, and advocates. Nevertheless, there are limitations in their application, particularly for the diagnosis of language disorders, and learning disabilities. Here, we discuss some of the available normed, signed language assessments and some of their limitations. We have also provided information related to practices that should lead to improvement in the quality of signed language assessments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jon Henner
- University of North Carolina at Greensboro
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Toe D, Paatsch L. Communicative Competence of Oral Deaf Children While Explaining Game Rules. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2018; 23:369-381. [PMID: 29889224 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/eny017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2017] [Accepted: 05/29/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Classrooms are characterized by interactions in a range of genres. The concise language required by expository interactions can be challenging for children who have atypical language, including children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH). This study compared the way three groups of upper primary school students (aged 8-13 years) taught a peer to play a new unfamiliar board game: (a) DHH "experts" teaching a "novice" hearing peer; (b) hearing experts teaching a DHH novice; and (c) a hearing expert teaching a hearing novice. All DHH students were enrolled in mainstream schools and used spoken language as the main mode of communication. All three groups were able to convey game rules and purpose, and navigate clarifications. Differences emerged in the accuracy of the use of referents when instructing their peers how to play the game. The specific content vocabulary and the need to emphasize new concise information also challenged the DHH children. This study highlights the importance of including expository tasks in language support and intervention for children who are DHH.
Collapse
|
37
|
Hall WC, Smith SR, Sutter EJ, DeWindt LA, Dye TDV. Considering parental hearing status as a social determinant of deaf population health: Insights from experiences of the "dinner table syndrome". PLoS One 2018; 13:e0202169. [PMID: 30183711 PMCID: PMC6124705 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2017] [Accepted: 07/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The influence of early language and communication experiences on lifelong health outcomes is receiving increased public health attention. Most deaf children have non-signing hearing parents, and are at risk for not experiencing fully accessible language environments, a possible factor underlying known deaf population health disparities. Childhood indirect family communication–such as spontaneous conversations and listening in the routine family environment (e.g. family meals, recreation, car rides)–is an important source of health-related contextual learning opportunities. The goal of this study was to assess the influence of parental hearing status on deaf people’s recalled access to childhood indirect family communication. We analyzed data from the Rochester Deaf Health Survey–2013 (n = 211 deaf adults) for associations between sociodemographic factors including parental hearing status, and recalled access to childhood indirect family communication. Parental hearing status predicted deaf adults’ recalled access to childhood indirect family communication (χ2 = 31.939, p < .001). The likelihood of deaf adults reporting “sometimes to never” for recalled comprehension of childhood family indirect communication increased by 17.6 times for those with hearing parents. No other sociodemographic or deaf-specific factors in this study predicted deaf adults’ access to childhood indirect family communication. This study finds that deaf people who have hearing parents were more likely to report limited access to contextual learning opportunities during childhood. Parental hearing status and early childhood language experiences, therefore, require further investigation as possible social determinants of health to develop interventions that improve lifelong health and social outcomes of the underserved deaf population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wyatte C. Hall
- Obstetrics & Gynecology and Clinical & Translational Science Institute, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Scott R. Smith
- Office of the Associate Dean of Research, National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| | - Erika J. Sutter
- National Center for Deaf Health Research, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| | - Lori A. DeWindt
- National Center for Deaf Health Research, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Deaf Wellness Center, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| | - Timothy D. V. Dye
- Obstetrics & Gynecology and Clinical & Translational Science Institute, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
- Pediatrics and Public Health Sciences, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Bilingual Cancer Genetic Education Modules for the Deaf Community: Development and Evaluation of the Online Video Material. J Genet Couns 2017; 27:457-469. [PMID: 29260487 DOI: 10.1007/s10897-017-0188-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2017] [Accepted: 11/27/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Health information about inherited forms of cancer and the role of family history in cancer risk for the American Sign Language (ASL) Deaf community, a linguistic and cultural community, needs improvement. Cancer genetic education materials available in English print format are not accessible for many sign language users because English is not their native or primary language. Per Center for Disease Control and Prevention recommendations, the level of literacy for printed health education materials should not be higher than 6th grade level (~ 11 to 12 years old), and even with this recommendation, printed materials are still not accessible to sign language users or other nonnative English speakers. Genetic counseling is becoming an integral part of healthcare, but often ASL users are not considered when health education materials are developed. As a result, there are few genetic counseling materials available in ASL. Online tools such as video and closed captioning offer opportunities for educators and genetic counselors to provide digital access to genetic information in ASL to the Deaf community. The Deaf Genetics Project team used a bilingual approach to develop a 37-min interactive Cancer Genetics Education Module (CGEM) video in ASL with closed captions and quizzes, and demonstrated that this approach resulted in greater cancer genetic knowledge and increased intentions to obtain counseling or testing, compared to standard English text information (Palmer et al., Disability and Health Journal, 10(1):23-32, 2017). Though visually enhanced educational materials have been developed for sign language users with multimodal/lingual approach, little is known about design features that can accommodate a diverse audience of sign language users so the material is engaging to a wide audience. The main objectives of this paper are to describe the development of the CGEM and to determine if viewer demographic characteristics are associated with two measurable aspects of CGEM viewing behavior: (1) length of time spent viewing and (2) number of pause, play, and seek events. These objectives are important to address, especially for Deaf individuals because the amount of simultaneous content (video, print) requires cross-modal cognitive processing of visual and textual materials. The use of technology and presentational strategies is needed that enhance and not interfere with health learning in this population.
Collapse
|
39
|
Stone A, Petitto LA, Bosworth R. Visual Sonority Modulates Infants' Attraction to Sign Language. LANGUAGE LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT 2017; 14:130-148. [PMID: 32952461 PMCID: PMC7500480 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2017.1404468] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
The infant brain may be predisposed to identify perceptually salient cues that are common to both signed and spoken languages. Recent theory based on spoken languages has advanced sonority as one of these potential language acquisition cues. Using a preferential looking paradigm with an infrared eye tracker, we explored visual attention of hearing 6- and 12-month-olds with no sign language experience as they watched fingerspelling stimuli that either conformed to high sonority (well-formed) or low sonority (ill-formed) values, which are relevant to syllabic structure in signed language. Younger babies showed highly significant looking preferences for well-formed, high sonority fingerspelling, while older babies showed no preference for either fingerspelling variant, despite showing a strong preference in a control condition. The present findings suggest babies possess a sensitivity to specific sonority-based contrastive cues at the core of human language structure that is subject to perceptual narrowing, irrespective of language modality (visual or auditory), shedding new light on universals of early language learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam Stone
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA
| | - Laura-Ann Petitto
- PhD in Educational Neuroscience Program, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC
- NSF Science of Learning Center, Visual Language and Visual Learning (VL2), Gallaudet University, Washington, DC
- Department of Psychology, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC
| | - Rain Bosworth
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Hall WC. What You Don't Know Can Hurt You: The Risk of Language Deprivation by Impairing Sign Language Development in Deaf Children. Matern Child Health J 2017; 21:961-965. [PMID: 28185206 DOI: 10.1007/s10995-017-2287-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
A long-standing belief is that sign language interferes with spoken language development in deaf children, despite a chronic lack of evidence supporting this belief. This deserves discussion as poor life outcomes continue to be seen in the deaf population. This commentary synthesizes research outcomes with signing and non-signing children and highlights fully accessible language as a protective factor for healthy development. Brain changes associated with language deprivation may be misrepresented as sign language interfering with spoken language outcomes of cochlear implants. This may lead to professionals and organizations advocating for preventing sign language exposure before implantation and spreading misinformation. The existence of one-time-sensitive-language acquisition window means a strong possibility of permanent brain changes when spoken language is not fully accessible to the deaf child and sign language exposure is delayed, as is often standard practice. There is no empirical evidence for the harm of sign language exposure but there is some evidence for its benefits, and there is growing evidence that lack of language access has negative implications. This includes cognitive delays, mental health difficulties, lower quality of life, higher trauma, and limited health literacy. Claims of cochlear implant- and spoken language-only approaches being more effective than sign language-inclusive approaches are not empirically supported. Cochlear implants are an unreliable standalone first-language intervention for deaf children. Priorities of deaf child development should focus on healthy growth of all developmental domains through a fully-accessible first language foundation such as sign language, rather than auditory deprivation and speech skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wyatte C Hall
- Clinical & Translational Science Institute, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Marschark M, Zettler I, Dammeyer J. Social Dominance Orientation, Language Orientation, and Deaf Identity. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2017; 22:269-277. [PMID: 28575425 PMCID: PMC5881267 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enx018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2016] [Revised: 04/11/2017] [Accepted: 04/16/2017] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
The notion of the Deaf community as a linguistic-cultural minority has been increasingly recognized and studied over the last two decades. However, significant differences of opinion and perspective within that population typically have been neglected in the literature. Social dominance orientation (SDO), a theoretical construct, typically focusing on intergroup perceptions and relations, is one aspect that has been left unexplored and might prove particularly enlightening. The present study investigated SDO among 119 deaf and 49 hearing young adults through a standardized SDO questionnaire. SDO was examined with regard to cultural identities (deaf, hearing, bicultural, and marginal), cochlear implant use, and language orientation (sign language or spoken language). The deaf participants were found to be more egalitarian than hearing individuals overall. Deaf individuals who held the strongest deaf identities, those who were sign language oriented, and not cochlear implant users, were the most egalitarian.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc Marschark
- National Technical Institute for the Deaf – Rochester Institute of Technology
- University of Aberdeen
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Language deprivation syndrome: a possible neurodevelopmental disorder with sociocultural origins. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2017; 52:761-776. [PMID: 28204923 PMCID: PMC5469702 DOI: 10.1007/s00127-017-1351-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 77] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/07/2016] [Accepted: 01/22/2017] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE There is a need to better understand the epidemiological relationship between language development and psychiatric symptomatology. Language development can be particularly impacted by social factors-as seen in the developmental choices made for deaf children, which can create language deprivation. A possible mental health syndrome may be present in deaf patients with severe language deprivation. METHODS Electronic databases were searched to identify publications focusing on language development and mental health in the deaf population. Screening of relevant publications narrowed the search results to 35 publications. RESULTS Although there is very limited empirical evidence, there appears to be suggestions of a mental health syndrome by clinicians working with deaf patients. Possible features include language dysfluency, fund of knowledge deficits, and disruptions in thinking, mood, and/or behavior. CONCLUSION The clinical specialty of deaf mental health appears to be struggling with a clinically observed phenomenon that has yet to be empirically investigated and defined within the DSM. Descriptions of patients within the clinical setting suggest a language deprivation syndrome. Language development experiences have an epidemiological relationship with psychiatric outcomes in deaf people. This requires more empirical attention and has implications for other populations with behavioral health disparities as well.
Collapse
|
43
|
Bilingual approach to online cancer genetics education for Deaf American Sign Language users produces greater knowledge and confidence than English text only: A randomized study. Disabil Health J 2016; 10:23-32. [PMID: 27594054 DOI: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2016.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2016] [Revised: 07/12/2016] [Accepted: 07/20/2016] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Deaf American Sign Language-users (ASL) have limited access to cancer genetics information they can readily understand, increasing risk for health disparities. We compared effectiveness of online cancer genetics information presented using a bilingual approach (ASL with English closed captioning) and a monolingual approach (English text). HYPOTHESIS Bilingual modality would increase cancer genetics knowledge and confidence to create a family tree; education would interact with modality. METHODS We used a parallel 2:1 randomized pre-post study design stratified on education. 150 Deaf ASL-users ≥18 years old with computer and internet access participated online; 100 (70 high, 30 low education) and 50 (35 high, 15 low education) were randomized to the bilingual and monolingual modalities. Modalities provide virtually identical content on creating a family tree, using the family tree to identify inherited cancer risk factors, understanding how cancer predisposition can be inherited, and the role of genetic counseling and testing for prevention or treatment. 25 true/false items assessed knowledge; a Likert scale item assessed confidence. Data were collected within 2 weeks before and after viewing the information. RESULTS Significant interaction of language modality, education, and change in knowledge scores was observed (p = .01). High education group increased knowledge regardless of modality (Bilingual: p < .001; d = .56; Monolingual: p < .001; d = 1.08). Low education group increased knowledge with bilingual (p < .001; d = .85), but not monolingual (p = .79; d = .08) modality. Bilingual modality yielded greater confidence creating a family tree (p = .03). CONCLUSIONS Bilingual approach provides a better opportunity for lower educated Deaf ASL-users to access cancer genetics information than a monolingual approach.
Collapse
|
44
|
Brice PJ, Strauss G. Deaf adolescents in a hearing world: a review of factors affecting psychosocial adaptation. Adolesc Health Med Ther 2016; 7:67-76. [PMID: 27186150 PMCID: PMC4847601 DOI: 10.2147/ahmt.s60261] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Adolescence has long been viewed as a time of rapid change in many domains including physical, cognitive, and social. Adolescents must adapt based on developing skills and needs and acclimate to growing environmental pressures. Deaf adolescents are often faced with the additional challenge of managing these adaptations in a hearing world, where communication and access to information, especially about their social world, are incomplete at best and nonexistent at worst. This article discusses the research on several factors that influence a deaf adolescent's adaptation, including quality of life, self-concept, and identity development. Gaps in our knowledge are pointed out with suggestions for future research programs that can facilitate optimal development in adolescents who are deaf.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick J Brice
- Department of Psychology, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Gillie Strauss
- Department of Psychology, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Lysdahl KB, Oortwijn W, van der Wilt GJ, Refolo P, Sacchini D, Mozygemba K, Gerhardus A, Brereton L, Hofmann B. Ethical analysis in HTA of complex health interventions. BMC Med Ethics 2016; 17:16. [PMID: 27004792 PMCID: PMC4804607 DOI: 10.1186/s12910-016-0099-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2015] [Accepted: 03/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND In the field of health technology assessment (HTA), there are several approaches that can be used for ethical analysis. However, there is a scarcity of literature that critically evaluates and compares the strength and weaknesses of these approaches when they are applied in practice. In this paper, we analyse the applicability of some selected approaches for addressing ethical issues in HTA in the field of complex health interventions. Complex health interventions have been the focus of methodological attention in HTA. However, the potential methodological challenges for ethical analysis are as yet unknown. METHODS Six of the most frequently described and applied ethical approaches in HTA were critically assessed against a set of five characteristics of complex health interventions: multiple and changing perspectives, indeterminate phenomena, uncertain causality, unpredictable outcomes, and ethical complexity. The assessments are based on literature and the authors' experiences of developing, applying and assessing the approaches. RESULTS The Interactive, participatory HTA approach is by its nature and flexibility, applicable across most complexity characteristics. Wide Reflective Equilibrium is also flexible and its openness to different perspectives makes it better suited for complex health interventions than more rigid conventional approaches, such as Principlism and Casuistry. Approaches developed for HTA purposes are fairly applicable for complex health interventions, which one could expect because they include various ethical perspectives, such as the HTA Core Model® and the Socratic approach. CONCLUSION This study shows how the applicability for addressing ethical issues in HTA of complex health interventions differs between the selected ethical approaches. Knowledge about these differences may be helpful when choosing and applying an approach for ethical analyses in HTA. We believe that the study contributes to increasing awareness and interest of the ethical aspects of complex health interventions in general.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Gert Jan van der Wilt
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,Athena Institute for Innovation in the Health and Life Sciences, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Pietro Refolo
- Institute of Bioethics, "A. Gemelli" School of Medicine, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Rome, Italy
| | - Dario Sacchini
- Institute of Bioethics, "A. Gemelli" School of Medicine, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Rome, Italy
| | - Kati Mozygemba
- Department of Health Services Research, Institute of Public Health and Nursing Research, and Health Sciences, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany.,Health Sciences Bremen, Bremen, Germany
| | - Ansgar Gerhardus
- Department of Health Services Research, Institute of Public Health and Nursing Research, and Health Sciences, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany.,Health Sciences Bremen, Bremen, Germany
| | | | - Bjørn Hofmann
- Centre for Medical Ethics, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.,The Norwegian University for Science and Technology, Gjøvik, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Davidson K, Lillo-Martin D, Chen Pichler D. Spoken english language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2014; 19:238-50. [PMID: 24150489 PMCID: PMC3952677 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/ent045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 78] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/14/2023]
Abstract
Bilingualism is common throughout the world, and bilingual children regularly develop into fluently bilingual adults. In contrast, children with cochlear implants (CIs) are frequently encouraged to focus on a spoken language to the exclusion of sign language. Here, we investigate the spoken English language skills of 5 children with CIs who also have deaf signing parents, and so receive exposure to a full natural sign language (American Sign Language, ASL) from birth, in addition to spoken English after implantation. We compare their language skills with hearing ASL/English bilingual children of deaf parents. Our results show comparable English scores for the CI and hearing groups on a variety of standardized language measures, exceeding previously reported scores for children with CIs with the same age of implantation and years of CI use. We conclude that natural sign language input does no harm and may mitigate negative effects of early auditory deprivation for spoken language development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn Davidson
- Department of Linguistics, Yale University, Dow Hall, 370 Temple Street, New Haven, CT 06520-8366.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Infant hearing loss: from diagnosis to therapy Official Report of XXI Conference of Italian Society of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. ACTA OTORHINOLARYNGOLOGICA ITALICA : ORGANO UFFICIALE DELLA SOCIETA ITALIANA DI OTORINOLARINGOLOGIA E CHIRURGIA CERVICO-FACCIALE 2012; 32:347-70. [PMID: 23349554 PMCID: PMC3552543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2012] [Accepted: 09/01/2012] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Hearing loss is one of the most common disabilities and has lifelong consequences for affected children and their families. Both conductive and sensorineural hearing loss (SNHL) may be caused by a wide variety of congenital and acquired factors. Its early detection, together with appropriate intervention, is critical to speech, language and cognitive development in hearing-impaired children. In the last two decades, the application of universal neonatal hearing screening has improved identification of hearing loss early in life and facilitates early intervention. Developments in molecular medicine, genetics and neuroscience have improved the aetiological classification of hearing loss. Once deafness is established, a systematic approach to determining the cause is best undertaken within a dedicated multidisciplinary setting. This review addresses the innovative evidences on aetiology and management of deafness in children, including universal neonatal screening, advances in genetic diagnosis and the contribution of neuroimaging. Finally, therapy remains a major challenge in management of paediatric SNHL. Current approaches are represented by hearing aids and cochlear implants. However, recent advances in basic medicine which are identifying the mechanisms of cochlear damage and defective genes causing deafness, may represent the basis for novel therapeutic targets including implantable devices, auditory brainstem implants and cell therapy.
Collapse
|
48
|
First language acquisition differs from second language acquisition in prelingually deaf signers: evidence from sensitivity to grammaticality judgement in British Sign Language. Cognition 2012; 124:50-65. [PMID: 22578601 PMCID: PMC3657148 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2010] [Revised: 04/13/2012] [Accepted: 04/13/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Age of acquisition (AoA) effects have been used to support the notion of a critical period for first language acquisition. In this study, we examine AoA effects in deaf British Sign Language (BSL) users via a grammaticality judgment task. When English reading performance and nonverbal IQ are factored out, results show that accuracy of grammaticality judgement decreases as AoA increases, until around age 8, thus showing the unique effect of AoA on grammatical judgement in early learners. No such effects were found in those who acquired BSL after age 8. These late learners appear to have first language proficiency in English instead, which may have been used to scaffold learning of BSL as a second language later in life.
Collapse
|